In memory of David Thoreson Lykken (1928–2006), valued mentor, brilliant thinker, courageous scholar, and pioneer in the study of personality disorders.
                                                                                                                                                         
Copyright © 2007 by Sage Publications, Inc.
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced or utilized in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or by any information storage
and retrieval system, without permission in writing
For information:
Sage Publications, Inc.
2455 Teller Road
Thousand Oaks, California 91320 E-mail: order@sagepub.com
Sage Publications Ltd. 1 Oliver’s Yard
55 City Road London, EC1Y 1SP United Kingdom
Printed in the United States of America
from the publisher.
Sage Publications India Pvt. Ltd. B 1/I 1 Mohan Cooperative
Industrial Area
Mathura Road, New Delhi 110 044 India
Sage Publications Asia-Pacific Pte. Ltd. 33 Pekin Street #02-01
Far East Square
Singapore 048763
 
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Personality disorders: Toward the DSM-V/[edited by] William O’Donohue, Katherine A. Fowler, Scott O. Lilienfeld.
p. cm.
Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 978-1-4129-0422-3 (pbk. : alk. paper)
1. Personality disorders. 2. Psychotherapy. I. O’Donohue, William T. II. Fowler, Katherine A. III. Lilienfeld, Scott O., 1960-
[DNLM: 1. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders.
2. Personality Disorders. 3. Personality Disorders—classification.
WM 190 P4676 2007]
RC554.P4777 2007 616.85’81—dc22
This book is printed on acid-free paper.
07 08 09 10 11 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
2007000009

Acquisitions Editor: Editorial Assistant: Production Editor: Copy Editor: Typesetter: Proofreader: Indexer:
Cover Designer: Marketing Manager:
Kassie Graves Veronica Novak
Libby Larson
Rachel Keith
C&M Digitals (P) Ltd. Caryne Brown Michael Ferreira
Janet Foulger Thomas Mankowski
Contents______________
Foreword vii
C. Robert Cloninger
Acknowledgments xvii
Chapter 1: Introduction: Personality Disorders in Perspective 1
Katherine A. Fowler, William O’Donohue, Scott O. Lilienfeld
Chapter 2: Alternatives to DSM-IV: Axis II 21
Thomas A. Widiger
Chapter 3: Paranoid Personality Disorder 41
David P. Bernstein, J. David Useda
Chapter 4: Schizoid Personality Disorder 63
Vijay A. Mittal, Oren Kalus,
David P. Bernstein, Larry J. Siever
Chapter 5: Schizotypal Personality Disorder 81
Annie M. Bollini, Elaine F. Walker
Chapter 6: Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 109
Christopher J. Patrick
Chapter 7: Borderline Personality Disorder 167
Rebekah Bradley, Carolyn Zittel Conklin, Drew Westen
Chapter 8: Histrionic Personality Disorder 203
Pavel S. Blagov, Katherine A. Fowler, Scott O. Lilienfeld
Chapter 9: Narcissistic Personality Disorder 233
Kenneth N. Levy, Joseph S. Reynoso, Rachel H. Wasserman, John F. Clarkin
Chapter 10: Avoidant Personality Disorder 279
James D. Herbert
Chapter 11: Dependent Personality Disorder 307
Robert F. Bornstein
Chapter 12: Obsessive-Compulsive Personality Disorder 325
Jennifer Bartz, Alicia Kaplan, Eric Hollander
Chapter 13: Passive-Aggressive, Depressive, and
Sadistic Personality Disorders 353
Leslie C. Morey, Christopher J. Hopwood, Daniel N. Klein
Index 375 About the Editors 391 About the Contributors 393
Foreword _____________
C. Robert Cloninger Washington University
The study of personality disorders is at a critical point in its develop- ment. Human beings are naturally curious about their inner nature and what motivates their feelings, thoughts, and behavior. The study of human personality has an ancient and rich history that has led to many insightful theories and approaches to both well-being and personality disor- der. In the mid 20th century, personality theory and research flourished with the genius of many diverse thinkers, such as Freud, Allport, and Eysenck (Hall & Lindzey, 1970). Subsequently, work on personality was mostly stag- nant, with little empirical research on personality and a clinical shift to cat- egorical, behavioral, and biological approaches to mental disorders until the publication of the third edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III; American Psychiatric Association, 1980). DSM- III was both atheoretical and poorly grounded empirically, and most DSM categories had little construct validation. These limitations may have led psy- chiatry unwittingly toward the field’s current state of diagnostic dysfunction (Cloninger, Svrakic, & Przybeck, 2006).
Nevertheless, DSM-III placed personality disorders on a separate axis, which called attention to the importance of diagnosing personality disorders. There was a rapid increase in research and publications about personality and its disorders. Reliable methods for the assessment of personality and its disor- ders were developed, including both categorical and dimensional approaches (Cloninger, 1999b). Work on the etiology, development, and treatment of per- sonality disorders began with renewed enthusiasm. New organizations for the study of personality disorders and the Journal of Personality Disorders were
 
Author’s note: Correspondence concerning this paper should be addressed to C. Robert Cloninger, M.D., Wallace Renard Professor of Psychiatry, Genetics, & Psychology, Washington University, St. Louis, Missouri 63131. E-mail: clon@tci.wustl.edu
vii
viii
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

founded to bring together clinicians and researchers from psychology, psychi- atry, and related areas. However, during the past decade the field has once again become stagnant. Publications and research continue, but the field is not really moving forward.
Progress in understanding personality disorders is currently paralyzed by a per- vasive reluctance or resistance to face fundamental questions about human nature. Most work either is atheoretical, such as that based on DSM categories or descriptive personality tests derived by factor analysis, or is designed to confirm preexisting theories, such as behaviorism or psychoanalysis. There is no progres- sive program of hypothesis testing and refinement to answer the basic questions about human nature that have always inspired passionate interest in personality. Researchers with different assumptions about human nature collect themselves into narrow social networks that are self-affirming but blind or aggressive toward the assumptions and work of others. In compliance with tradition and funding pressures, research is mostly focused on evidence for the validity of discrete cate- gories. The most important work on personality is now being done outside the field of personality disorders, mostly by neuroscientists using functional brain imaging and developmental biologists studying gene-environment interactions (Kaasinen, Aalto, Nagren, & Rinne, 2004; Keltikangas-Jaervinen, Raeikkoenen, Ekelund, & Peltonen, 2004; Pezawas et al., 2005). There is so much conflict about the content of the dimensions of personality among clinicians investigating personality disorder that some clinicians seriously advocate describing personal- ity using checklists of the symptoms of the current committee-defined categories (Skodol et al., 2005) rather than resolving the number and content of personality dimensions scientifically. There is so much overlap among putative categories of personality disorder that the most common diagnosis is personality disorder not otherwise specified (Verheul & Widiger, 2004). Yet little work is done to apply and further evaluate evidence that the presence of any personality disorder can be reliably made by evaluating core features common to all personality disorders, such as self-directedness and cooperativeness (Cloninger, 2000).
Research on dimensional approaches is also not progressing because advocates of particular models are strongly resistant to communication and fundamental change. When alternative three- and five-factor models were found not to explain some aspects of human personality (Zuckerman & Cloninger, 1996), advocates of alternative models continued to debate the advantages of one incomplete model over another incomplete model (Widiger, Simonsen, Sirovatka, & Regier, 2006). For example, models of personality based on lexical traditions in English and other languages ini- tially suggested that there were only five factors of personality. More thor- ough studies found evidence for seven dimensions of personality, showing that the evidence for only five was based on incomplete lists of descriptors that neglected words for positive and negative valence (Waller, 1999; Waller & Zavala, 1993). Even when their own data show that positive and negative valence adds to the prediction of personality pathology beyond markers of the Big Five, proponents of the Big Five have continued to argue that five factors are comprehensive (Simms, 2007). When five-factor
Foreword ix

models were unable to explain most of the variance in a specific sixth dimension, such as spirituality or self-transcendence as measured by the Temperament and Character Inventory (Cloninger, 2006a; Zuckerman & Cloninger, 1996), they tried to ignore this by saying that spirituality was not something they wanted to measure, despite its importance for psy- chopathology and well-being (Cloninger, 2004). Like the absentminded White Queen in Alice in Wonderland, something is not real unless you want it to be. But facts do not change just because we want to ignore them.
Psychosocial and neurobiological research has also revealed that human per- sonality is an expression of nonlinear dynamic systems involving many genetic and environmental variables that influence development (Cloninger, Svrakic, & Svrakic, 1997). Yet personality researchers continue to depend on categorical systems or factor-analytic methods that ignore nonlinear dynamics. The result is that descriptions of personality disorders and of personality factors provide a view of differences between people that does not correspond well to the inter- nal dynamics or motivation relevant to personality development or treatment (Cervone, 2004). Social-cognitive psychologists generally ignore the work of personality researchers because DSM and factor-analytic models do not provide a model of the within-person structure of personality. It is possible to model the within-person structure of personality, as I do in my seven-factor psychobio- logical model, but the result is not what is given by the linearity assumptions made by factor analysis (Cloninger, Svrakic, & Przybeck, 1993). For example, neuroticism is a factor in most factor-analytic models of personality, but a per- son can be high in neuroticism if he or she has an anxiety disorder without per- sonality disorder or if he or she has a personality disorder without prominent anxiety. In fact, neuroticism is a composite of two traits regulated by different genetic determinants and different brain networks, which I have called harm avoidance and self-directedness (Cloninger, 2006a). Harm avoidance (but not self-directedness) is strongly related to individual differences in anxiety regula- tion mediated by limbic structures such as the connectivity of the amygdala and the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (Pezawas et al., 2005). In contrast, self- directedness (but not harm avoidance) is related to individual differences in executive cognitive processes mediated by the prefrontal cortex (Cloninger, 2004). Yet people trained in personality psychology and factor analysis often ignore the extensive and important work being done regarding the psychosocial and neurobiological bases of personality development (Kaasinen et al., 2004; Kaasinen, Maguire, Kurki, Bruck, & Rinne, 2005; Keltikangas et al., 2004), thereby treating each human being as a black box devoid of inner experience and emitting only self-reports or externally observable behaviors. Don’t we need to focus on inner experience if the distress and behaviors associated with personality disorders are the expression of maladaptive regulation of inner experiences?
Much current psychotherapy research about personality disorders is con- sumed by an effort to demonstrate efficacy and effectiveness so that they can be considered evidence-based therapies, like the drugs that meet FDA standards (Westen & Morrison, 2001). Advocates of different forms of
x
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

treatment, such as dialectical behavior therapy or transference-based psy- chotherapy, compare one treatment to another but seem reluctant to report that outcomes are largely explained by aspects of treatment common to all effective therapies, such as a helping therapeutic alliance (Gerstley et al., 1989; Lambert, 2004). There is little evidence of the importance of specific procedures for specific diagnoses, and yet the number of different evidence- based psychotherapies is growing like the number of newly patented medi- cines. Unfortunately, there is no corresponding increase in the average levels of happiness and mental health in the general population as a result of either the new medications or the new psychotherapies (Cloninger, 2004). Where has the field gone wrong?
Can a field of research be described as having a malaise or stagnation akin to a personality disorder? Such a statement would mean that the field is making untested and maladaptive assumptions about inner experience and behavior that are pervasive and inflexible and that impair scientific progress. The field of research on personality disorder seems to be infected with such a malaise, which I hope is curable. Committee-defined criteria and factor-analytically derived tests are no substitute for a coherent and testable theory of personality and its disorders.
In their insightful introduction, Fowler, O’Donohue, and Lilienfeld describe 13 serious problems that plague the field. These are the unan- swered fundamental issues that I have suggested are the cause of a serious malaise and stagnation of the field. They include the basic questions about whether personality disorders really exist as categories and about the number and content of the dimensions of personality. Until these questions are answered, it is difficult or impossible to make progress on derivative issues of construct validity, such as the etiology, course, and treatment of personality disorders. How do you describe the causes and course of some- thing that doesn’t exist as a taxon or discrete category (Meehl, 1995)? If you could, why would you want to when you could obtain more complete information by characterizing continuous dimensions of clinical variability and their configural interactions? Perhaps unique configurations of traits do have distinct developmental trajectories and require specific treatment procedures (Cloninger & Svrakic, 1997). Even if that is the case, as I think it may be, then we need to know precisely how to specify those configura- tions in a highly reliable fashion before progress can be made (Kendell, 1982). In my opinion, an integrative approach is also needed that considers the path to well-being (Cloninger, 2006b) and not just particular states of disability (Skodol et al., 2005).
The contributors to this important book have done an excellent job on their assignments to deal with individual putative categories of personality disorder. There is impressive individual and group talent at work in the field of personality disorders. The diversity of their perspectives and rec- ommendations illustrates, nevertheless, the serious problems that beset the field as a whole. The differences in perspective across chapters reveal much
  
Foreword xi

about the controversies within the field. Most chapters do focus on one putative category of personality disorder, and the majority of these relate one category of personality disorder on Axis II of DSM to one category of mental disorder on Axis I of DSM. For example, there are several pairs of Axis I–Axis II relationships discussed as parts of a continuous spectrum or frequently comorbid conditions: paranoid personality disorder with delu- sional disorder, schizotypal personality disorder with schizophrenia, avoidant personality disorder with social anxiety disorder, obsessive- compulsive personality disorder with obsessive-compulsive disorder, and depressive personality disorder with major depressive disorder. Of course, there are serious questions about the categorical nature of both Axis I and Axis II conditions (Kendell, 1982), so such pairings really tell us little about the fundamental question regarding categories and dimensions. The cate- gories of Axis I provide little validity for personality categories because the Axis I conditions also have serious problems with uncertain boundaries, exten- sive comorbidity, and nonspecific response to treatment. Counterbalancing the chapters emphasizing spectra bridging Axis I and Axis I, Widiger notes the incomplete coverage of personality disorder by existing categories, and Morey and Hopwood emphasize the high prevalence of personality disor- der not otherwise specified. But does it make sense to try to correct this problem by fewer dimensions than are known to exist or by putative cate- gories with little construct validity?
The current criteria for most putative categories are criticized, except for obsessive-compulsive personality disorder, which is regarded as part of a spec- trum with obsessive-compulsive disorder, body dysmorphic disorder, and pathological gambling. Such spectra are also controversial, however, with body dysmorphic disorder more closely resembling social anxiety disorder and pathological gambling overlapping with impulse control disorders like antiso- cial personality. So should we make body dysmorphic disorder part of the avoidant–social anxiety spectrum, or avoidant personality disorder part of the obsessive spectrum?
More generally, how do the various spectra overlap? Do they overlap as extensively as the clusters of personality disorders do? Disease spectra and the personality clusters to which they are related can be specified by their deviation on specific temperaments: Cluster A and related Axis I disorders with low reward dependence (i.e., aloof and cold), Cluster B and related Axis I disorders with high novelty seeking (i.e., impulsive and quick- tempered), and Cluster C and related Axis I disorders with high harm avoidance (i.e., anxious and pessimistic; Battaglia, Przybeck, Bellodi, & Cloninger, 1996; Cloninger, 2000; Goldman, Skodol, McGrath, & Oldham, 1994; Mulder & Joyce, 1997). As a result, clusters and the spec- tra related to them exist in all possible combinations rather than being mutually exclusive. The various clusters and syndromes within clusters show complex patterns of overlap and differentiation that can be better represented as a multidimensional configuration than by a unidimensional
  
xii
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

spectrum or set of independent spectra (Svrakic, Whitehead, Przybeck, & Cloninger, 1993).
For example, Patrick questions the adequacy of the behavioral criteria for antisocial personality disorder, preferring the concept of psychopathy as described by Cleckley and later Hare. The criteria for antisocial personality disorder are specified as reliably rated behaviors based on the classical work of Lee Robins (1966), so it is comparable to Axis I syndromes such as social anxiety rather than disorders specified in terms of inner experience and motivation, as personality traits are usually defined. When antisocial syn- dromes are assessed in terms of personality and inner experience, at least two configurations of personality traits are identified: both groups are immature in character traits such as self-directedness and cooperativeness, high in novelty seeking, and low in reward dependence, but primary psy- chopaths (“antisocial personality disorder”) are low in harm avoidance, and secondary psychopaths (“borderline personality disorder”) are high in harm avoidance (Cloninger, 2005). Some suggest that primary psychopathy is a taxon with poor response to treatment, but it can also be well described as an extreme configuration of traits normally distributed in the general population (Cloninger, 2005).
Likewise, some evidence for the categorical nature of schizophrenia (Cloninger, Martin, Guze, & Clayton, 1985; Lenzenweger, McLachlan, & Rubin, 2007) and schizotypy (Lenzenweger & Korfine, 1992) has also been presented, but the separation of even these putative categories from neigh- boring conditions is at best incomplete and weak. Early concepts of etiology that motivated categorical models, such as single genes or single traumatic events, have been found to oversimplify the complex adaptive systems under- lying the development of psychopathology. Current research indicates that configurations of multiple sets of genetic and environmental variables influ- ence the development of psychopathology, thereby motivating the search for the best ways to deconstruct the complex determinants of mental disorders in terms of either symptoms or the causes of symptoms (Cloninger, 2004). In fact, all the personality disorders, including schizotypal personality, can be defined as extreme configurations of normal personality traits (Cloninger, 1999a). Categorical thinking is now more of a hindrance to understanding personality disorders and psychopathology than it is a benefit.
None of these controversies would matter much except that people with personality disorders frequently have short and miserable lives, as well as a serious and costly impact on society through their maladaptive behaviors. Therefore, the stagnation of the field in not being able to face and answer the fundamental questions about the nature of personality and its disorders is unsatisfactory and harmful. It is remarkable that so many obviously bright people cannot find a way to communicate with one another in an open-minded way so that they can make progress beyond what was known two decades ago when the same questions were being asked and tentatively
  
Foreword xiii

answered (Cloninger, 1987; Cloninger et al., 1993; Svrakic et al., 1993). Personality and its disorders are too important to be treated like a private cottage industry in which no one is permitted to question the local author- ity’s basic assumptions.
Personality disorders are too important for us to rely on poorly grounded dimensional representations of unvalidated categories without resolving the number and content of the underlying dimensions, even if they are not currently familiar to clinicians. The field needs to move forward, not wallow in stagnant pools of ignorance and illusion.
At least for the personality disorders, DSM is really only a system of clas- sification for cataloguing and billing purposes and does not even begin to be an adequately validated scientific basis for research and treatment. As long as research classification and case descriptions are based on such an antiquated categorical system, little or no progress in understanding disorders of person- ality is likely, as evidenced by the current impasses stalemating the field.
Personality Disorders is an important book because it clearly exposes the major controversies facing the field. Each chapter is an erudite contribution that stands on its own quality. In addition, the insightful reader will be able to compare the divergent, and sometimes contradictory, perspectives and conclusions of different chapters. Consideration of the contradictions and unanswered questions throughout the book reveals the harsh consequences of blind reliance on false assumptions about how nature can be carved at its joints, when there are actually no such joints.
_________________________________________ References
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Battaglia, M., Przybeck, T. R., Bellodi, L., & Cloninger, C. R. (1996). Temperament dimensions explain the comorbidity of psychiatric disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 37, 292–298.
Cervone, D. (2004). The architecture of personality. Psychological Review, 111(1), 183–204.
Cloninger, C. R. (1987). A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants: A proposal. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 573–587. Cloninger, C. R. (1999a). Measurement of psychopathology as variants of person- ality. In C. R. Cloninger (Ed.), Personality and psychopathology (pp. 33–66).
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Cloninger, C. R. (Ed.). (1999b). Personality and psychopathology. Washington,
DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Cloninger, C. R. (2000). A practical way to diagnose personality disorder: A pro-
posal. Journal of Personality Disorders, 14(2), 99–108.
Cloninger, C. R. (2004). Feeling good: The science of well-being. New York:
Oxford University Press.
 
xiv
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Cloninger, C. R. (2005). Antisocial personality disorder: A Review. In World Psychiatric Association (Series Ed.) & M. Maj (Vol. Ed.), Evidence and expe- rience in psychiatry: Vol. 8. Personality disorders (pp. 125–129). London: Wiley.
Cloninger, C. R. (2006a). Personality as a dynamic psychobiological system. In T. A. Widiger, E. Simonsen, P. Sirovatka, & D. A. Regier (Eds.), Dimensional models of personality disorders: Refining the research agenda for DSM-V. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Cloninger, C. R. (2006b). The science of well-being: An integrated approach to mental health and its disorders. World Psychiatry, 5, 71–76.
Cloninger, C. R., Martin, R. L., Guze, S. B., & Clayton, P. J. (1985). Diagnosis and prognosis in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 12–25.
Cloninger, C. R., & Svrakic, D. M. (1997). Integrative psychobiological approach to psychiatric assessment and treatment. Psychiatry, 60, 120–141.
Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Przybeck, T. R. (1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975–990.
Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Przybeck, T. R. (2006). Can personality assess- ment predict future depression? A twelve-month follow-up of 631 subjects. Journal of Affective Disorders, 92(1), 35–44.
Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, N. M., & Svrakic, D. M. (1997). Role of personality self- organization in development of mental order and disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 881–906.
Gerstley, L., McLellan, A. T., Alterman, A. I., Woody, G. E., Luborsky, L., & Prout, M. (1989). Ability to form an alliance with the therapist: A possible marker of prognosis for patients with antisocial personality. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146(4), 508–512.
Goldman, R. G., Skodol, A. E., McGrath, P. J., & Oldham, J. M. (1994). Relationship between the Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire and DSM- III-R personality traits. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 274–276.
Hall, C. S., & Lindzey, G. (1970). Theories of personality. New York: Wiley. Kaasinen, V., Aalto, S., Nagren, K., & Rinne, J. O. (2004). Insular dopamine D2 receptors and novelty seeking personality in Parkinson’s disease. Movement
Disorders, 19(11), 1348–1351.
Kaasinen, V., Maguire, E. A., Kurki, T., Bruck, A., & Rinne, J. O. (2005). Mapping
brain structure and personality in late adulthood. NeuroImage, 24, 315–322. Keltikangas-Jaervinen, L., Raeikkoenen, K., Ekelund, J., & Peltonen, L. (2004).
Nature and nurture in novelty seeking. Molecular Psychiatry, 9(3), 308–311. Kendell, R. E. (1982). The choice of diagnostic criteria for biological research.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 39, 1334–1339.
Lambert, M. J. (Ed.). (2004). Bergin and Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy.
New York: Wiley.
Lenzenweger, M. F., & Korfine, L. (1992). Confirming the latent structure and base
rate of schizotypy: A taxometric analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
101(3), 567–571.
Lenzenweger, M. F., McLachlan, G., & Rubin, D. B. (2007). Resolving the latent struc-
ture of schizophrenia endophenotypes using expectation-maximization–based finite mixture modeling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(1), 16–29.
Foreword xv

Meehl, P. E. (1995). Bootstraps taxometrics: Solving the classification problem in psychopathology. American Psychologist, 50(4), 266–275.
Mulder, R. T., & Joyce, P. R. (1997). Temperament and the structure of personal- ity disorder symptoms. Psychological Medicine, 27, 99–106.
Pezawas, L., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Drabant, E. M., Verchinski, B. A., Munoz, K. E., Kolachana, B. S., et al. (2005). 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: A genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression. Nature Neuroscience, 8(6), 838–834.
Robins, L. N. (1966). Deviant children grown up: A sociological and psychiatric study of sociopathic personality. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
Simms, L. J. (2007). The Big Seven model of personality and its relevance to per- sonality pathology. Journal of Personality, 75(1), 65–94.
Skodol, A. E., Oldham, J. M., Bender, D. S., Dyck, I. R., Stout, R. L., Morey, L. C., et al. (2005). Dimensional representations of DSM-IV personality disorders: Relationships to functional impairment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(10), 1919–1925.
Svrakic, D. M., Whitehead, C., Przybeck, T. R., & Cloninger, C. R. (1993). Differential diagnosis of personality disorders by the seven-factor model of temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 991–999.
Verheul, R., & Widiger, T. A. (2004). A meta-analysis of the prevalence and usage of the personality disorder not otherwise specified (PDNOS) diagnosis. Journal of Personality Disorders, 18, 309–319.
Waller, N. G. (1999). Evaluating the structure of personality. In C. R. Cloninger (Ed.), Personality and psychopathology (pp. 155–197). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Waller, N. G., & Zavala, J. (1993). Evaluating the Big Five. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 131–134.
Westen, D., & Morrison, K. (2001). A multidimensional meta-analysis of treat- ments for depression, panic, and generalized anxiety disorder: An empirical examination of the status of empirically supported therapies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69(6), 875–899.
Widiger, T. A., Simonsen, E., Sirovatka, P. J., & Regier, D. A. (Eds.). (2006).
Dimensional models of personality disorders: Refining the research agenda for
DSM-V. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Zuckerman, M., & Cloninger, C. R. (1996). Relationships between Cloninger’s, Zuckerman’s, and Eysenck’s dimensions of personality. Personality and Individual
Differences, 21, 283–285.
Acknowledgments ______
First of all, we would like to acknowledge our chapter contributors. We know that it can be a challenge for busy and productive individuals to take time out of their professional and personal lives to write a book chap- ter. We very much appreciate their time, dedication, and excellent work. Second, we’d like to thank our editor at Sage, Veronica Novak, whose patience, support, and astute judgment were invaluable in completing this project. We extend our thanks to Kassie Graves and the art department per- sonnel at Sage, who were also very helpful. Without their support and pro- fessionalism this book would not have been possible. Third, we’d like to thank Rachel Keith for her perspicacious and astute editing and proofing. She made this often arduous process a pleasure. Fourth, we’d like to thank the wonderful individuals who helped us finish this book, especially Crissa Draper, Amy Harper, and Lauren Tolle. Finally, we’d like to thank our fam- ilies, Jane, Katie, Anna, and Sahil, for being there.
xvii

1 Introduction Personality Disorders in Perspective
Katherine A. Fowler Emory University
William O’Donohue University of Nevada, Reno
Scott O. Lilienfeld Emory University

The personality disorders are a complex, controversial, and fascinating class of diagnoses. Decades of clinical observations of developmentally fixated “character types” (Freud, 1916/1991), “neurotic styles” (Shapiro, 1965), and “character disorders” (e.g., Horney, 1939) preceded the formal classification “personality disorders,” which first emerged in 1980 in the third edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III; American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1980). Most of the newly termed personality disorders were not new; indeed, some (e.g., schizoid per- sonality, paranoid personality) had been included in prior editions of the DSM. In preparing the third edition, however, the DSM committee arrived at the consensus that personality disorders are a different “kind” of diagnostic category from the vast majority of disorders listed in the DSM. Thus, DSM- III was the first edition to adopt a multiaxial approach, assigning personality disorders to a separate axis (Axis II) from “clinical conditions” (Axis I). The most recent edition, DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000), briefly states the rationale for this decision:
1
2
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

The listing of Personality Disorders and Mental Retardation on a sepa- rate axis ensures that consideration will be given to the possible presence of Personality Disorders and Mental Retardation that might otherwise be overlooked when attention is directed to the usually more florid Axis I disorders. (p. 28)
The above seems to imply that a diagnosis of personality disorder may provide the context for, or be overshadowed by, more “florid” Axis I diag- noses (such as depression or schizophrenia). It is interesting to speculate why personality disorders share this axis with mental retardation. Both are fairly pervasive across many situations, and both certainly bear implications for the diagnosis and treatment of Axis I disorders (e.g., cognitive therapy for depression might be contraindicated or implemented quite differently in either context); perhaps these similarities justify their placement on Axis II and explain in part why DSM-IV relegates personality disorders to a differ- ent diagnostic class from the other mental disorders. Moreover, it has been suggested that most personality disorders tend to be ego-syntonic (i.e., con- sistent with self-concept), whereas most Axis I disorders are ego-dystonic (i.e., inconsistent with self-concept; Grove & Tellegen, 1991).
DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000) provides a broad definition of personality disorder:
A Personality Disorder is an enduring pattern of inner experience and behavior that deviates markedly from the expectations of the individ- ual’s culture, is pervasive and inflexible, has an onset in adolescence or early adulthood, is stable over time, and leads to distress or impair- ment. (p. 629)
This seemingly simple summary actually alludes to a complex set of prop- erties, requiring exegesis of its key phrases. Without some clarification, we cannot make reliable and valid diagnoses and come to a shared understand- ing of personality disorders. For example, the following terms and phrases raise important questions:
1. Enduring: How long must an individual exhibit features of a person- ality disorder before a diagnosis is applicable?
2. Pattern: How consistently over time must an individual exhibit the characteristics in question? If there are periods of time when the indi- vidual does not exhibit these characteristics, how long can these time periods be?
3. Inner experience: Clearly, this term potentially comprises an extremely broad range of mental processes, including emotions, thoughts, impulses, fantasies, schemas, and memories. Which of these are considered most central to the conceptualization of personality disorders?
  
Introduction 3

4. Deviates markedly: Whether a deviation is “marked” is clearly sub- jective. DSM-IV-TR encourages one to consider the individual’s con- text (e.g., family, culture) in making this determination. This is a step in the right direction, as it emphasizes that norms are not universal. However, it leads to the next point of inquiry:
5. Individual’s culture: Culture has many facets, including ethnicity, sex- ual orientation, socioeconomic status, geographic region, and gender. To which of these do we look to determine cultural norms, and what are the limits? How do we avoid confounding “cultural norms” with stereotypical perceptions?
6. Pervasive: How cross-situationally consistent must personality disor- der features be for an individual to meet criteria? For example, can a person meet criteria for a personality disorder if he or she exhibits marked features in personal life but less marked features at work?
7. Inflexible: How much flexibility is allowed, and in which domains (e.g., work, school, relationships), and how pervasive must this inflex- ibility be?
8. Onset in adolescence or early adulthood: How does one discern “onset”? Must the person fully meet criteria by a certain age, or do some prodromal symptoms count? Particularly in the case of late ado- lescence and early adulthood, determining developmental norms is a difficult task. How do we find a balance between overpathologizing normal-range teenage behavior and overlooking true pathology? Furthermore, the age limits constituting adolescence and early adult- hood are not specified.
9. Leads to distress and impairment: How does one establish the causal link between the symptoms and distress/impairment? What kinds of and how much distress/impairment are required?
Three levels of general questions surround personality disorders. First, there are semantic questions, such as those raised above. Second, there are operational questions: Once the semantics are clarified, what is the best way to gather the information we need? For example, say “enduring” is taken to mean “for at least 3 years.” Should we ask the client to self-report start dates and end dates of symptoms? Should we review records that cover this period, if available, to see if there are any corroborating or contradictory indicators? Should we use information provided by other informants who know the client well? The third problem is the measurement question: How does one begin to construct measures with adequate content- and criterion- related validity? This is no easy task, especially as many of these measures aim to assess complex constructs such as “impairment,” which requires historical or normative information often accessible only to the affected
  
4 PERSONALITY DISORDERS

individual (“inner experience”). Part of the controversy regarding personal- ity disorders can be tied to this complexity and to what some understand- ably perceive to be a lack of progress on these questions.
The DSM-IV Personality Disorders_____________________
DSM-IV-TR officially recognizes 10 personality disorders (PDs) and includes three others for further study (depressive personality disorder, passive- aggressive personality disorder, and sadistic personality disorder). A brief description of each of the 10 recognized PDs follows:
Paranoid Personality Disorder is a pattern of distrust and suspicious- ness such that others’ motives are interpreted as malevolent;
Schizoid Personality Disorder is a pattern of detachment from social relationships and a restricted range of emotional expression;
Schizotypal Personality Disorder is a pattern of acute discomfort in close relationships, cognitive or perceptual distortions, and eccentrici- ties of behavior;
Antisocial Personality Disorder is a pattern of disregard for, and vio- lation of, the rights of others;
Borderline Personality Disorder is a pattern of instability in interper- sonal relationships, self-image, and affects, and marked impulsivity;
Histrionic Personality Disorder is a pattern of excessive emotionality and attention seeking;
Narcissistic Personality Disorder is a pattern of grandiosity, need for admiration, and lack of empathy;
Avoidant Personality Disorder is a pattern of social inhibition, feelings of inadequacy, and hypersensitivity to negative evaluation;
Dependent Personality Disorder is a pattern of submissive and cling- ing behavior related to an excessive need to be taken care of;
Obsessive-Compulsive Personality Disorder is a pattern of preoccupa- tion with orderliness, perfectionism, and control. (APA, 2000, p. 685)
The same three levels of questions noted in our discussion of PD conceptual- ization in general can be asked regarding individual diagnoses. Take paranoid PD,
   
Introduction 5

for example: there are semantic questions (e.g., what is meant by “suspicious- ness”?), operational questions (how do we go about capturing this meaning?), and measurement questions (how do we develop a test that provides a valid mea- sure of “suspiciousness”?). When these questions are multiplied across all the cri- teria for all of the diagnoses, we can again see both the potential for controversy and the large amount of work that lies ahead.
The individual diagnoses are organized into three broad clusters repre- senting presumed superordinate features that characterize each group:
Cluster A (odd/eccentric disorders):
Paranoid personality disorder Schizoid personality disorder Schizotypal personality disorder
Cluster B (dramatic, emotional, or erratic disorders):
Antisocial personality disorder Borderline personality disorder Histrionic personality disorder Narcissistic personality disorder
Cluster C (anxious/fearful disorders):
Avoidant personality disorder
Dependent personality disorder Obsessive-compulsive personality disorder
These clusters presumably “carve nature at its joints” better than the individ- ual diagnoses. There are consistent findings of high co-occurrence of diagnoses within each cluster but less co-occurrence of diagnoses in different clusters (Morey, 1988; see below).
Major Questions and Controversies ______________________ in Personality Disorder Research
One consistent finding is that the prevalence of personality disorders in the general population is fairly high. For example, Mattia and Zimmerman (2001) averaged data across a number of epidemiological studies and found that in community samples, 13% of individuals had at least one personality disorder.
  
6
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Moreover, the prevalence of many of the individual diagnoses was relatively high; for example, obsessive-compulsive personality disorder was present in 4% of the population, while histrionic, schizotypal, and dependent personal- ity disorders were each present in 2% of the population. This high prevalence is one reason that personality disorders remain an area of lively debate.
It is beyond the scope of this chapter to settle these contentions, as many of the issues remain unresolved in the literature. Furthermore, we do not mean to imply that these controversies are unique to personality disorders. Nevertheless, to provide a context for the remaining chapters, we give you their general flavor:
1. The predictive utility and perhaps even the existence of personality itself have been called into question. For several decades, psycholo- gists actively debated whether personality is a valid construct rather than merely a convenient summary label that we invoke to describe behaviors that happen to covary (Mischel, 1968). Personality, partic- ularly for radical behaviorists, was a controversial construct. Skinner (1957) argued that “personality is nothing but the locus of behavior” (p. 182). He thought of personality as an “explanatory fiction” (p. 182) that often seems to involve circular reasoning. For example, imagine that someone observes Jane acting in a consistently outgoing and friendly fashion and asks, “Why does Jane behave this way?” If the answer is that she has an “extraverted personality” and if one con- cludes that she has an extraverted personality by observing that she is generally outgoing and friendly, this is a tautological, pseudo- explanation. Radical behaviorists would be more comfortable using personality traits as descriptive rather than explanatory concepts.
Personality researchers responded to these criticisms. In particular, they demonstrated that many personality trait measures predict laboratory and biological indices that cannot merely be derived from the behaviors subsumed by the trait labels themselves (Kenrick & Funder, 1988). Nevertheless, some critics justifiably continue to argue that some per- sonality disorders (e.g., dependent personality disorder) are little more than summary labels for behaviors that merit clinical attention.
Mischel re-ignited this controversy in the late 1960s in his book Personality and Assessment (1968), in which he pointed to the cross- situational inconsistency of personality traits and thereby called into question the traditional assumption of traits as pervasive behavioral dispositions. Once again, researchers responded to this criticism in a variety of ways. Some suggested that the sample of studies Mischel used to bolster his claim was unrepresentative and demonstrated how a review of different studies did not support his claim (e.g., Block, 1977), while others suggested that examination of moderator vari- ables, such as tendency to “self-monitor” (Snyder, 1974) and predic- tion of aggregated behaviors (i.e., trends) rather than single behaviors,
  
Introduction 7

yield considerably higher estimates of cross-situational consistency (Bem & Allen, 1974; Epstein, 1979).
Although many consider these controversies resolved when it comes to personality in general, they bear repeating with respect to person- ality disorders. Personality disorder researchers should meet these challenges as personality researchers did by demonstrating that these conditions predict important external criteria (e.g., natural history, laboratory findings).
2. It is not clear whether personality disorders are underpinned by cate- gories or by dimensions. The DSM-IV-TR adopts a categorical system for the diagnosis of personality disorders, as it does with all other dis- orders. Some have argued (e.g., Trull, 2005) that a dimensional system is better suited to the diagnosis of personality disorders. Categorical discriminations are made when constructs have or are thought to have clear boundaries in nature (tall or short, for example). Dimensional rat- ings, in contrast, simply provide a value on a continuum (such as 5′ 10″). In general, categorical approaches presume that natural discontinu- ities exist, whereas dimensional approaches presume the existence of natural continua. Although dimensional systems seem to have the advantage of precision, they raise questions about which dimensions of personality and/or psychopathology ought to be used in conceptu- alizing personality disorders. Is the use of cutoffs on dimensions for a particular disorder ever justified for pragmatic or social purposes? And, finally, can we measure a dimension accurately? For example, we can measure height, but can we measure extraversion with the same reliability and validity?
3. If personality disorders are better conceptualized dimensionally, it is not clear which system (e.g., Big Five, Big Three) is the most useful for doing so. Several dimensional models have been investigated as alter- natives to the current DSM categorical conceptualization of personal- ity disorders. Of these, the model necessitating the least departure from the current system adopts the approach of rating match to a pro- totypical description of each disorder. Although some have found that this approach may increase reliability (e.g., Heumann & Morey, 1990), others have noted that it does not necessarily increase validity for two reasons: (1) the validity of the prototypes is still dependent on the valid- ity of the conceptualizations found in the DSM, and (2) this approach does not address the problem of symptom heterogeneity found within most personality disorders (Clark, 1999).
A different dimensional approach examines the possibility that per- sonality disorders are best expressed as constellations of normal- range personality traits. Of these alternative conceptualizations, the five-factor model of personality (FFM) has received the most research attention. The FFM was derived from lexical studies of the English
  
8
PERSONALITY DISORDERS
   
4.
language, in which an overinclusive list of colloquial trait adjectives was factor-analyzed, yielding five separable dimensions: (1) extraver- sion (social potency, positive affectivity); (2) agreeableness (desire to get along with others); (3) conscientiousness (constraint, self- discipline); (4) neuroticism (anxiety, irritability); and (5) openness (intellect, unconventionality). Widiger, Trull, Clarkin, Sanderson, and Costa (1994) give an example of the characterization of a personality disorder within this framework:
From the perspective of the FFM avoidant personality disorder involves: a) introversion, particularly the facets of low gregarious- ness (no close friends, avoids significant interpersonal contact, and unwilling to get involved with others; APA, 1987); low excite- ment-seeking (exaggerates potential dangers, difficulties, or risks in doing anything outside of normal routine); and low activity (avoidance of social and occupational activities, and canceling of social plans) . . . and b) neuroticism, particularly the facets of vul- nerability, self-consciousness, and anxiety (e.g., easily hurt by crit- icism and disapproval, reticent in social situations because of fear of saying something foolish, fears being embarrassed, and afraid of being disliked). (p. 49)
They further raise the still unresolved question of whether using this model to describe personality disorders is superior to the current DSM system. Part of the argument for doing so is that the FFM may pro- vide an overarching and parsimonious model that accommodates all of the personality disorders on Axis II. Moreover, such a model could underscore trait commonalities that both underlie these disorders and account for their extensive covariation (see #4 below). Currently, sev- eral research groups are examining these questions (e.g., Bagby, Costa, Widiger, Ryder, & Marshall, 2005; Miller, Bagby, Pilkonis, Reynolds, & Lynam, 2005). Alternative dimensional models to the FFM, such as three-factor models (e.g., Clark, 1999; Cloninger, 1987), have been proposed and continue to be an active source of investigation.
The rampant comorbidity among many personality disorders calls into question their validity and/or existence as independent syndromes. Fiester, Ellison, Docherty, and Shea (1990) reported two kinds of comorbidity among PDs. First, they found that individuals diagnosed with one personality disorder are likely to be diagnosed with at least one other. In four samples of individuals diagnosed with a personality disorder, Widiger and Rogers (1989) found that the average propor- tion of patients who met criteria for at least one additional personality disorder was 85%, with a range of 76% for dependent personality dis- order to 100% for paranoid personality disorder. Second, they found that many persons diagnosed with a personality disorder are also diagnosed with a mood disorder, anxiety disorder, or schizophrenia.
Introduction 9

They concluded that “the fundamental relationships of these disorders to one another remains a puzzle” (p. 111). Moreover, they proposed some potential explanations for this second type of comorbidity, such as: (1) one personality disorder causes another disorder; (2) the other disorder causes the personality disorder; (3) the personality disorder and the other disorder influence and affect each other; and (4) the per- sonality disorder and the other disorder are etiologically separable and unrelated coeffects of some other disturbance. More research is needed to investigate these possibilities.
5. There are serious problems with the reliability of personality disor- ders. The DSM personality disorders suffer from problems of inter- nal consistency, test-retest reliability, and interrater reliability. Morey (1988) found that the internal consistency among diagnostic features for the DSM-III personality disorders as revealed by the median cor- relation among the criteria within each disorder ranged from r = .10 to r = .34. Loranger et al. (1988) studied the test-retest reliability of personality disorder diagnoses over a 1-week to 6-month interval. The median kappa for the presence or absence of the disorder on both occasions ranged between .52 and .57, except in the case of compulsive personality disorder (now called obsessive-compulsive personality disorder), in which it was only .26. Overall, there was a trend toward fewer observed diagnoses and traits over time. With regard to interrater reliability, in the DSM-III field trials the kappa coefficient for the presence or absence of any personality disorder was .61 based on a joint-interview design. The interrater reliability evidence for later versions of the DSM is scarcely more promising (see Zimmerman, 1994, for a review).
6. There is a paucity of evidence for the construct validity of most per- sonality disorders, including their natural history and laboratory cor- relates. The chapters that follow will reveal that much information about the course, family history, and laboratory and physiological correlates of personality disorders is lacking. Of course, this criticism must be tempered by the fact that many other groups of disorders in the DSM-IV also lack this sort of information (e.g., sexual dysfunc- tions and paraphilias). This paucity of evidence leaves the DSM vul- nerable to the radical behaviorists’ criticism that these disorders are little more than tautological summary labels for covarying thoughts, emotions, and behaviors.
7. The best means of assessing personality disorders is unclear. Currently, there are a number of omnibus instruments for the assessment of person- ality disorders, such as the Personality Disorders Examination (Loranger, 1988), the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R Personality Disorders (SCID-II; Spitzer, Williams, & Gibbon, 1987), and the Personality Disorders Questionnaire–4+ (PDQ-4+; Hyler & Rieder,
  
10
PERSONALITY DISORDERS
 
8.
1994). In a thorough review of the extant measures, Perry (1992) found that although several instruments showed moderate to high levels of interrater and test-retest reliability for some diagnoses, many showed poor levels. Perry also found little agreement among the mea- sures (median kappa = .25), calling into question the convergent valid- ity of commonly used personality disorder measures.
Moreover, Zimmerman (1994) has suggested that most of the fol- lowing fundamental questions regarding personality disorder assess- ment remain unresolved: (1) Who should provide information (e.g., client, relatives, both)? (2) What instruments should be used? and (3) When should the assessment be conducted (e.g., when the patient is free of Axis I symptoms, or when he or she is experiencing them)?
ThedistinctionbetweenAxisIandAxisIIpsychopathologyisfrequently unclear and seemingly arbitrary. The rationale for these two axes has never been entirely clear or grounded in solid evidence (Harkness & Lilienfeld, 1997). For example, some Axis I problems, such as schizo- phrenia and cyclothymic disorder, can last at least as long as Axis II dis- orders, particularly if they are left untreated. In addition, the DSM’s placement of Axis II disorders has often been inconsistent. For example, cyclothymic disorder appears to be a subsyndromal form of bipolar dis- order and is located on Axis I, whereas schizotypal personality disorder appears to be a subsyndromal form of schizophrenia and is located on Axis II. Additionally, as mentioned previously, the way in which Axis I and Axis II pathology interact remains unclear. Thus one can raise potentially interesting questions, such as, does dysphoria set the stage for the perhaps more florid histrionic personality disorder, or is it the other way around? That is, although the DSM seems to view personal- ity disorders as a distinct class of disorders, it has not clearly elucidated the distinctive properties of this class.
The diagnosis of certain personality disorders may be compromised by gender bias. According to DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000), some person- ality disorders are diagnosed more frequently in men (e.g., antisocial personality disorder, narcissistic personality disorder, obsessive- compulsive personality disorder, and the Cluster A disorders), whereas others are diagnosed more frequently in women (e.g., borderline personality disorder, histrionic personality disorder, and dependent personality disorder). However, there have been inconsistent findings: for example, Golomb, Fava, Abraham, and Rosenbaum (1995) found no significant gender differences for borderline, histrionic, and depen- dent personality disorders.
Some have argued that the DSM unfairly pathologizes individuals who are extreme examples of stereotypical sex roles (both male and female), and that the conformity of many healthy individuals to these roles accounts for the observed pattern of sex differences in personality

9.

Introduction 11

disorder diagnosis (e.g., Caplan, 1987; Landrine, 1989). Moreover, a widely cited study by Warner (1978) reported that clinicians tended to diagnose males with antisocial personality disorder and females with histrionic personality disorder when they were given identical patient profiles that were supposed to describe a patient with histrionic person- ality disorder. Taken together, these points raise questions of sex bias in both the diagnostic decision-making process and in the criteria them- selves.
In a 1991 review, Widiger and Spitzer examined several potential sources of sex bias in PD diagnosis: (1) social-cultural sex bias (in which differences result from differences in child rearing, social opportunities, etc.), (2) sampling sex bias (in which sex differences are caused by data collection at sites where one sex with a disorder happens to be more represented than the other), and (3) diagnostic sex bias (in which either the assessment instruments or the diagnos- tic criteria cause false positives or false negatives that misrepresent true prevalence for one sex). They concluded that for questions of gender bias to be properly examined, the DSM must first clarify the threshold at which behaviors become maladaptive (to protect against false positives). They further emphasized that differential prevalence rates do not necessarily indicate bias and may instead reflect appropriate attention to the base rates of disorders in the real world.
10. There is a marked paucity of well-conducted research examining effective treatments for most personality disorders. As the chapters that follow will underscore, beyond a few treatments for borderline personality disorder that have promising empirical support, there are few documented effective interventions for personality disorders. Furthermore, clinical lore regarding the intractability of some per- sonality disorders (e.g., antisocial personality disorder; see Fowler & Lilienfeld, 2006) has led to marked pessimism surrounding therapeu- tic intervention, possibly further contributing to this inertia. These observations highlight the importance of further well-conducted treat- ment outcome studies for personality disorders.
11. There is disagreement regarding specific diagnostic criteria for most personality disorders. As will be discussed in greater detail within several chapters, there have been numerous changes in personality disorder diagnostic criteria across the DSM revisions. These changes reflect a lack of consensus in the field surrounding these criteria. For example, there has been particular controversy surrounding antis- ocial personality disorder. Some argue that this diagnosis should be modified to include dispositional criteria (e.g., lack of empathy, superficial charm), which were largely eliminated in DSM-III (APA, 1980) in an attempt to increase reliability, but which may have decreased validity (Lykken, 1995).
  
12
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

12.
There is marked symptom heterogeneity within diagnoses. Since its third edition (DSM-III; APA, 1980), the DSM has employed a largely polythetic classification system—that is, one that lists a number of characteristics, of which the individual is required to possess only a subset to meet the diagnosis. As a result, it is possible for there to be little to no overlap in symptoms among patients with the same diag- nosis. This problem is not limited to personality disorders but is a consequence of the DSM’s classification method, which some criti- cize as a “Chinese menu” approach.
Many personality disorders appear to be heterogeneous in etiology.
Recent findings suggest that some personality disorders (e.g., antiso- cial personality disorder, borderline personality disorder) subsume dis- tinct variants that differ in etiology, course, and treatment response (Bradley, Conklin, & Westen, 2005; Skeem, Poythress, Edens, Lilienfeld, & Cale, 2003). These results raise concerns regarding the construct valid- ity of some current personality disorder conceptualizations, because such diagnoses may comprise multiple syndromes.

13.
Despite myriad surrounding controversies, personality disorder diagnoses are frequently associated with important social and clinical outcomes. For example, individuals with paranoid personality disorder are disproportionately likely to become involved in litigation (see Chapter 3), and individuals with bor- derline personality disorder have a high rate of suicide attempts (see Chapter 7).
Findings such as these bring us full circle in our delineation of current con- troversies surrounding personality disorders as we return to questions of pre- dictive validity. Because many personality disorders show associations with important real-world outcomes, the controversies outlined in this chapter are more than discouraging theoretical musings or armchair debates. Instead, they should be considered potentially fruitful and interesting lines of inquiry that may improve our understanding of important clinical phenomena.
Purpose and Outline of This Volume ___________________
We intend this handbook to be a widely used resource that will appeal to a broad audience, including researchers, beginning and advanced graduate students, and practitioners in a variety of mental health settings. We asked that our contributors minimize the use of technical terms, supplying a user-friendly summary and integration of major trends, findings, and future directions.
The book is organized by two broad sections: (1) an opening section (including this chapter) that reviews basic theoretical and methodological controversies in personality disorder research and practice and highlights pro- posed alternatives to DSM-IV Axis II conceptualizations, and (2) a section in which the DSM-IV personality disorders, organized by cluster, as well as the
 
Introduction 13

three personality disorders designated for further research, are discussed and critically evaluated. Each of these chapters includes a summary and thought- ful integration of the best available scientific evidence bearing on the etiology, assessment, diagnosis, and (where relevant) treatment of the disorder. Further, chapters include discussion of any relevant psychobiological models and cor- relates. Wherever possible, case examples are offered as illustrations of each disorder’s clinical presentation. Finally, each chapter includes explicit recom- mendations for further research, with a focus on unresolved conceptual and methodological issues. This includes a thoughtful review and evaluation of alternative conceptualizations (e.g., dimensional, interpersonal, behavior ana- lytic) of each disorder, with an eye toward the publication of DSM-V. You will notice that the controversies outlined in the previous section provide the scaffolding for the chapters that follow.
By now, you have read most of Chapter 1, which is framed on the current state of personality disorder research and practice issues. In Chapter 2, Widiger further discusses problems with current Axis II conceptualizations, such as pejorative connotations of its nomenclature, seemingly arbitrary diag- nostic boundaries, and inadequate coverage of clinical phenomena. He then provides a thorough review of proposed alternative models, including dimen- sional models, prototype matching approaches, and possible reconfigura- tions of the Axis I/Axis II distinction.
In Chapter 3, Bernstein and Useda present and discuss paranoid person- ality disorder (PPD). They highlight important differential diagnostic chal- lenges, such as distinguishing between the extreme suspiciousness that characterizes PPD and the paranoid delusions that characterize paranoid schizophrenia. They present the history of paranoia in the psychiatric litera- ture and critique the current DSM conceptualization, which they argue may overrepresent cognitive features of the condition. Further, they note poten- tially important social consequences for individuals affected by PPD, such as relationship difficulties and involvement in unnecessary litigation, as they underscore the need for effective treatments for PPD. As of now, they add, the treatment literature is unfortunately composed solely of case studies. Although some of these have shown promising results, they point to the need for larger-scale treatment studies as well as further construct validation efforts as an important future direction for PPD research.
In Chapter 4, Mittal, Kalus, Bernstein, and Siever present and discuss schizoid personality disorder (SCD). They outline the historical conceptual- ization of “schizoid character” as first described by Bleuler, its subsequent revisions, and the present DSM conceptualization, in which it mirrors a sub- syndromal variant of the “negative” symptoms associated with schizophre- nia. They discuss the extremely low prevalence of SCD in clinical settings, describe psychometric problems regarding low internal consistency of the diagnosis, and critically evaluate the sensitivity and specificity of the indi- vidual criteria. Additionally, they discuss differential diagnostic challenges in distinguishing SCD from avoidant personality disorder and Asperger’s
  
14
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

syndrome. Last, they discuss the difficulties inherent in drawing individuals with SCD into treatment, due to their characteristic social isolation, and the resulting lack of treatment outcome studies. They conclude with recommen- dations regarding potential treatment strategies as well as recommended changes for DSM-V.
In Chapter 5, Bollini and Walker present and discuss schizotypal person- ality disorder (SPD). From the outset, they emphasize SPD as an Axis II con- dition for which there is a good deal of empirical support. They address the history of SPD as a condition conceptualized as a reflection of biological lia- bility for schizophrenia rooted in Meehl’s (1962) concept of “schizotaxia.” They discuss the extension of this conceptualization into the diathesis-stress model of schizophrenia spectrum disorders, which for decades has been the dominant etiologic model of schizophrenia. They present evidence establish- ing genetic links between SPD (the criteria for which describe positive and negative subsyndromal features of schizophrenia) and schizophrenia. Further, they discuss the evidence for environmental factors (e.g., obstetrical complications) that may be important in the etiology of SPD. Additionally, they discuss brain and psychophysiological abnormalities common to both SPD and schizophrenia. They conclude by noting the lack of controlled treat- ment studies of SPD, a target for future research.
In Chapter 6, Patrick presents and discusses antisocial personality disorder (APD) and a related syndrome, psychopathy. He first describes the history of psychopathy as a construct that evolved into the more behavior-based APD diagnosis now found in the DSM. He then presents recent empirical findings regarding APD, with an emphasis on APD’s place in a broader context of impulse control disorders. This is followed by a discussion of contemporary conceptualizations of psychopathy, a construct that many agree provides a richer picture of personality features often associated with chronic criminal behaviors, and that possesses a somewhat paradoxical mix of behavioral and personality pathology and adaptive personality features. Finally, Patrick dis- cusses the lack of well-controlled APD treatment studies and offers suggestions for future treatment efforts based on work with other externalizing conditions.
In Chapter 7, Bradley, Conklin, and Westen present and discuss border- line personality disorder (BPD). They first examine the history of the bor- derline construct, from its roots in Kernberg’s assertion that these patients’ personality organization lies on the borderline between neurosis and psy- chosis to more contemporary views of BPD as a disorder of emotion dys- regulation. They then introduce contemporary biological findings, including behavior genetics support for subsyndromal markers (i.e., endophenotypes) of BPD and neuroimaging studies. Further, they discuss literature regarding life events potentially etiologically relevant to BPD, such as separation from caretakers and childhood abuse. Additionally, they review findings related to the course and outcome of BPD, including high suicide rates and symptom reduction in middle age. They present findings regarding the most widely discussed contemporary therapy for BPD, which has shown promising
  
Introduction 15

empirical support: dialectical behavior therapy (DBT; Linehan, 1993). They discuss other interventions for which there are encouraging preliminary find- ings. They conclude with a discussion of future directions for BPD research, including improvement of diagnostic criteria and procedures and further examination of recent findings of subtypes of BPD.
In Chapter 8, Blagov, Fowler, and Lilienfeld present and discuss histrionic personality disorder (HPD). First they trace its historical roots, from ancient concepts of “wandering womb,” through its early psychiatric examination by Charcot, and finally to the present, where it has been separated from somatization symptoms and now describes a personality pattern of excessive attention seeking. They then discuss the considerable overlap between HPD and other Cluster B disorders, as well as findings related to associations between HPD and Axis I conditions. This is followed by a discussion of the impact of gender and culture on HPD diagnosis and prognosis and a review of theories that posit antisocial personality disorder and HPD as gender- typed manifestations of the same underlying construct. They then present the extant treatment literature for HPD, which consists primarily of treatments under development, awaiting empirical support. They conclude with a dis- cussion of future directions for HPD research, including elucidating its real- world impact, identifying endophenotypic markers that might differentiate it from other Cluster B disorders, and conducting controlled treatment studies.
In Chapter 9, Levy, Reynoso, Wasserman, and Clarkin present and discuss narcissistic personality disorder (NPD). First, they present the history of narcis- sism, from its roots in Greek mythology and Freudian theory to current DSM conceptualizations. They follow with a discussion of the extensive literature on subtypes of NPD and the disorder’s co-occurrence with a number of Axis I and Axis II conditions, including its frequent comorbidity with substance abuse. Although there are no controlled treatment studies of NPD, they briefly discuss findings from case studies. They then present the extant data regarding its course and outcome, including reports of symptom remittance over time and some evidence of increased suicide risk in NPD patients. Additionally, they discuss the relation between features of NPD and contemporary Western cul- ture. They conclude with recommendations for future research, including more research on course and outcome and well-done treatment studies.
In Chapter 10, Herbert presents and discusses avoidant personality disor- der (APD). He begins by reviewing the extensive literature examining the extent to which APD can be considered a distinct syndrome above and beyond the sum of its clinical features (shyness, social anxiety, and interper- sonal avoidance). In his discussion of overlap between APD and Axis I and II pathology, he particularly highlights findings pertaining to the overlap between APD and the condition that presents the greatest differential diag- nostic challenge: social anxiety disorder (SAD). Due to their high degree of overlap and the greater body of research on SAD, Herbert then reviews literature on the etiology, cognitive and social skills deficits, and assessment of APD and SAD together. He adds that there is little literature regarding
  
16
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

interventions specifically for APD, whereas there is a large literature sup- porting interventions for SAD, particularly social skills training and phar- macotherapy. He concludes with recommendations for future research, such as clarifying whether APD should be considered a subtype of SAD rather than a personality disorder and carrying out further treatment research.
In Chapter 11, Bornstein presents and discusses dependent personality disorder (DPD). He traces its history from early diagnosticians’ mentions of exaggerated dependency needs to current DSM conceptualizations of DPD. He then reviews findings from behavior genetics studies and studies of par- enting styles as they relate to DPD. Adding to his discussion of possible eti- ologic factors, Bornstein reviews cultural factors that may influence the perception of dependency as pathological. He then discusses epidemiologi- cal findings, which indicate that DPD is one of the most prevalent person- ality disorders in clinical settings and has a higher prevalence in women. Further, he discusses controversies over potential gender bias of DPD crite- ria and diagnosis. Bornstein then reviews the sparse treatment literature regarding DPD symptom reduction (for which there are conflicting results) and the impact of DPD on treatment outcome. He concludes with recom- mendations for future research, including criteria revision, careful evalua- tion of the temporal stability of DPD, and testing of different conceptual frameworks, such as those that incorporate symptom intensity and adap- tiveness (as dependence on others can at times be an adaptive strategy).
In Chapter 12, Bartz, Kaplan, and Hollander present and discuss obsessive-compulsive personality disorder (OCPD). They first review the history of OCPD as one of the few personality disorders that have appeared in some form in every edition of the DSM, with few changes to its criteria over time. They then present a major diagnostic controversy surrounding OCPD: its relation to obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). They discuss psychoanalytic, cognitive, and interpersonal conceptualizations of OCPD. Following this, they review recent proposals of an obsessive-compulsive spectrum, which some have argued may be mediated by biological mecha- nisms (e.g., serotonergic dysfunction) that result in varied but related clini- cal manifestations (e.g., OCD, OCPD, body dysmorphic disorder). Further, they present etiologic theories of OCPD as an adaptation to Axis I pathol- ogy such as anxiety disorders, with which it demonstrates a high degree of overlap. The literature regarding treatment of OCPD lacks well-controlled studies, but Bartz and her colleagues present and discuss different treatment models (psychodynamic, cognitive, pharmacotherapeutic) and present case study results when available. Last, they highlight more rigorous treatment studies and more research on the possible conceptualization of OCPD as an obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorder as key areas for future research.
Finally, in Chapter 13, Morey, Hopwood, and Klein present and discuss the three personality disorders designated for further research: depressive personal- ity disorder (DPD), passive-aggressive personality disorder (PAPD), and sadis- tic personality disorder (SPD). They cite the high prevalence of PDNOS
  
Introduction 17

(personality disorder not otherwise specified) diagnoses in clinical settings as a rationale for examining other potential personality disorders that have been identified in prior clinical and research literature but excluded from the cur- rent DSM. They first discuss PAPD (also called negativistic personality disor- der) and review theories regarding its etiology, such as dramatic shifts in parenting style in childhood and cognitive mechanisms such as overfocus on authority and power. Next, they discuss historical and current conceptual- izations of SPD, including its differentiation from sexual sadism. They under- score the potential importance of SPD if it accompanies a diagnosis of antisocial personality disorder (APD), as it adds information not found in the criteria for APD and would uniquely characterize those predisposed to cru- elty and violence as well as criminality. Last, they discuss DPD and its roots in German phenomenological literature on “depressive temperament.” They review long-standing controversies regarding the relationship of DPD with Axis I mood disorders (particularly cyclothymia) as well as behavior genetics studies that have indicated higher rates of mood disorders in relatives of probands with DPD. Morey, Hopwood, and Klein offer suggestions for future research on each of the three disorders.
We hope you will find that the 13 issues raised within this chapter come into sharper focus as you read through the volume. In many cases, however, there are a number of unanswered questions pertaining to these controver- sies, and we therefore further hope that these chapters will help stimulate your thought and interest and raise new questions toward the future of per- sonality disorder research.
_________________________________________ References
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of men- tal disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of men- tal disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.
Bagby, R. M., Costa, P. T., Jr., Widiger, T. A., Ryder, A. G., & Marshall, M. (2005). DSM-IV personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality: A multi- method examination of domain- and facet-level predictions. European Journal of Personality, 19, 307–324.
Bem, D. J., & Allen, A. (1974). On predicting some of the people some of the time: The search for cross-situational consistencies in behavior. Psychological Review, 81, 506–520.
Block, J. (1977). Advancing the science of psychology: Paradigmatic shift or improv- ing the quality of research? In D. Magnusson & N. S. Endler (Eds.), Personality at the crossroads: Current issues in interactional psychology (pp. 37–64). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bradley, R., Conklin, C. Z., & Westen, D. (2005). The borderline personality diagnosis in adolescents: Gender differences and subtypes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 1006–1019.
  
18
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Caplan, P. (1987). The psychiatric association’s failure to meet its own standards: The dangers of self-defeating personality disorder as a category. Journal of Personality Disorders, 1, 178–182.
Clark, L. A. (1999). Dimensional approaches to personality disorder assessment and diagnosis. In C. R. Cloninger (Ed.), Personality and psychopathology (pp. 219–244). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Cloninger, C. R. (1987). A systematic method for clinical description and classifica- tion of personality variants: A proposal. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 573–588.
Epstein, S. (1979). The stability of behavior: I. On predicting most of the people much of the time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1097–1126. Fiester, S. J., Ellison, J. M., Docherty, J. P., & Shea, T. (1990). Comorbidity of per- sonality disorders: Two for the price of three. New Directions for Mental Health
Services, 47, 103–114.
Fowler, K. A., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2006). Antisocial personality disorder. In
J. E. Fisher & W. O’Donohue (Eds.), Practice guidelines for evidence-based psy-
chotherapy. New York: Springer.
Freud, S. (1916/1991). Some character-types met with in psycho-analytic work. In
M. F. R. Kets de Vries & S. M. Perzow (Eds.), Handbook of character studies: Psychoanalytic explorations (pp. 27–50). Madison, CT: International Universities Press.
Golomb, M., Fava, M., Abraham, M., & Rosenbaum, J. F. (1995). Gender differ- ences in personality disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 579–582.
Grove, W. M., & Tellegen, A. (1991). Problems in the classification of personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 5(1), 31–41.
Harkness, A. R., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (1997). Individual differences science for treat- ment planning: Personality traits. Psychological Assessment, 9, 349–360.
Heumann, K. A., & Morey, L. C. (1990). Reliability of categorical and dimensional judgments of personality disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 498–500. Horney, K. (1939). What is a neurosis? American Journal of Sociology, 45, 426–432. Hyler, S. E., & Rieder, R. O. (1994). Personality diagnostic questionnaire–4+. New
York: Authors.
Kenrick, D. T., & Funder, D.C. (1988). Profiting from controversy: Lessons from the
person-situation debate. American Psychologist, 43, 23–34.
Landrine, H. (1989). The politics of personality disorder. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 13, 325–339.
Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline personality
disorder. New York: Guilford Press.
Loranger, A.W. (1988). Personality disorders examination (PDE) manual. Yonkers,
NY: DV Communications.
Lykken, D. T. (1995). The antisocial personalities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Mattia, J. I., & Zimmerman, M. (2001). Epidemiology. In W. J. Livesley (Ed.),
Handbook of personality disorders: Theory, research and treatment (pp. 107–123).
New York: Guilford Press.
Meehl, P. (1962). Schizotaxia, schizotypy, schizophrenia. American Psychologist, 17,
827–838.
Miller, J. D., Bagby, R. M., Pilkonis, P. A., Reynolds, S. K., & Lynam, D. R. (2005).
A simplified technique for scoring DSM-IV personality disorders with the five- factor model. Assessment, 12, 404–415.
Introduction 19

Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley.
Morey, L. C. (1988). The categorical representation of personality disorder: A clus- ter analysis of DSM-III-R personality features. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
97, 314–321.
Perry, J. C. (1992). Problems and considerations in the valid assessment of personal-
ity disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 1645–1653.
Shapiro, D. (1965). Neurotic styles. Oxford, UK: Basic Books.
Skeem, J. L., Poythress, N., Edens, J. F., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Cale, E. (2003).
Psychopathic personality or personalities? Exploring potential variants of psy- chopathy and their implications for risk assessment. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 8, 513–546.
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Snyder, M. (1974). Self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 30, 526–537.
Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., & Gibbon, M. (1987). Structured clinical interview
for DSM-III-R personality disorders (SCID-II). New York: New York State
Psychiatric Institute.
Trull, T. J. (2005). Dimensional models of personality disorder: Coverage and cut-
offs. Journal of Personality Disorders, 19, 262–282.
Warner, R. (1978). The diagnosis of antisocial and hysterical personality disorders:
An example of sex bias. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 166, 839–845. Widiger, T. A., & Rogers, J. H. (1989). Prevalence and comorbidity of personality
disorders. Psychiatric Annals, 19, 132–136.
Widiger, T. A., & Spitzer, R. L. (1991). Sex bias in the diagnosis of personality disor-
ders: Conceptual and methodological issues. Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 1–22. Widiger, T. A., Trull, T. J., Clarkin, J. F., Sanderson, C., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1994). A description of the DSM-III-R and DSM-IV personality disorders with the five-fac- tor model of personality. In P. T. Costa, Jr., & T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality (pp. 41–56). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Zimmerman, M. (1994). Diagnosing personality disorders. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 51, 225–249.

2 Alternatives to DSM-IV Axis II
Thomas A. Widiger University of Kentucky

Personality disorders could be among the more important diagnoses within the American Psychiatric Association’s (APA) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (APA, 2000). They have the unique distinction of being placed on a separate diagnostic axis, thereby drawing the attention of many clinicians who may otherwise not have con- sidered them. They were placed on a separate axis in DSM-III (APA, 1980) to encourage clinicians to recognize the presence of maladaptive personality traits even when their attention is understandably drawn to a disorder of more pressing concern (Frances, 1980; Spitzer, Williams, & Skodol, 1980). The reason that the authors of the multiaxial system of DSM-III wanted to draw attention to personality disorders was the “accumulating evidence that the quality and quantity of preexisting personality disturbance may . . . influ- ence the predisposition, manifestation, course, and response to treatment of various Axis I conditions” (Frances, 1980, p. 1050).
Personality disorders, however, are among the more controversial and problematic within the diagnostic manual. Maser, Kaelber, and Weise (1991) surveyed clinicians from 42 countries with respect to DSM-III-R (APA, 1987): “The personality disorders led the list of diagnostic categories
Author’s note: Correspondence concerning this chapter should be addressed to Thomas A. Widiger, Ph.D., 115 Kastle Hall, Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky 40506–0044. E-mail: widiger@uky.edu
21
22
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

with which respondents were dissatisfied” (p. 275). There are many reasons for this dissatisfaction, and there are a number of proposed alternatives. This chapter will begin with a brief description of some of the more fundamental and pressing concerns, followed by a discussion of proposals that have been made for the next edition of the diagnostic manual.
Problems With Axis II of DSM-IV ______________________
A number of significant problems and concerns have been raised with respect to the personality disorder diagnoses. Seven briefly outlined here are unreli- ability of diagnoses in general clinical practice, pejorative connotations, het- erogeneity within diagnoses, excessive diagnostic co-occurrence, inconsistent and unstable diagnostic boundaries, inadequate coverage, and inadequate scientific base.
Diagnostic Unreliability
One of the major innovations of DSM-III (APA, 1980) was the provision of behaviorally specific diagnostic criterion sets (Spitzer et al., 1980). “The order of inference is relatively low, and the characteristic features consist of easily identifiable behavioral signs or symptoms” (APA, 1980, p. 7). These behaviorally specific diagnostic criteria substantially improved the ability of clinicians to obtain reliable diagnoses (Nathan & Langenbucher, 1999). However, the development of behaviorally specific diagnostic criterion sets has been problematic for the personality disorders. As acknowledged in DSM-III-R, “for some disorders . . . , particularly the personality disorders, the criteria require much more inference on the part of the observer” (APA, 1987, p. xxiii). Narcissistic lack of empathy and borderline identity distur- bance, for example, are abstract clinical constructs that can be interpreted in a variety of ways by different clinicians and can be inferred on the basis of a variety of behaviors. As a result, personality disorders continue to be diag- nosed unreliably in general clinical practice (Mellsop, Varghese, Joshua, & Hicks, 1982; Nazikian, Rudd, Edwards, & Jackson, 1990; Spitzer, Forman, & Nee, 1979).
Studies have also indicated that, in the absence of a structured interview, clinicians routinely fail to identify all of the clinically significant maladaptive personality functioning that is present (Blashfield & Herkov, 1996; Wood, Garb, Lilienfeld, & Nezworski, 2002; Zimmerman & Mattia, 1999a, 1999b). Illustrating the general phenomenon of diagnostic overshadowing (i.e., once a diagnosis has been made, there is a tendency to attribute all other problems to that disorder, leaving coexisting conditions unrecognized), clinicians tend to diagnose personality disorders hierarchically, failing to conduct additional assessments once a particular construct has been identified (Blashfield & Flanagan, 1998; Herkov & Blashfield, 1995). The personality disorder that
  
Alternatives to DSM-IV 23

is provided preferential attention may even be idiosyncratic to the clinician (Gunderson, 1992; Mellsop et al., 1982).
Pejorative Connotations
The Assembly of the American Psychiatric Association has passed a number of resolutions to explore proposals to change the name of borderline person- ality disorder and to move the personality disorders to Axis I. The motivation for these resolutions has been in large part the frustration many clinicians reportedly experience in obtaining insurance coverage. There is an unfortunate perception that Axis II is for disorders for which there is currently no effective treatment (Frances et al., 1991). There is compelling empirical research indicating that at least some personality disorders are in fact treatable (Leichsenring & Leibing, 2003; Perry, Banon, & Ianni, 1999). Nevertheless, the perception that they are untreatable has fueled arguments for their refor- mulation as Axis I disorders. For example, Liebowitz et al. (1998) argued dur- ing the development of DSM-IV that most instances of avoidant personality disorder should be diagnosed on Axis I as a generalized social phobia because there are more effective treatments available: “We believe that the more exten- sive evidence for syndromal validity of social phobia, including pharmaco- logical and cognitive-behavioral treatment efficacy, make it the more useful designation in cases of overlap with avoidant personality” (p. 1060).
Heterogeneity Within Diagnoses
Most of the personality disorder diagnostic criterion sets in DSM-III (APA, 1980) were monothetic, in that all of the criteria were required in order to provide a diagnosis. However, it soon became apparent that persons with the same diagnosis rarely had all of the same personality features. Therefore, the authors of DSM-III-R switched to polythetic criterion sets in which only a subset of diagnostic criteria are required (Widiger, Frances, Spitzer, & Williams, 1988). For example, in DSM-IV, any four of eight optional crite- ria are required for the diagnosis of obsessive-compulsive personality disor- der (APA, 2000).
Polythetic criterions sets, however, do not actually resolve the problems associated with the heterogeneity among persons sharing the same diagno- sis. Polythetic criterion sets are simply an acknowledgment or acceptance of the problematic heterogeneity. For example, it is now possible for two patients to meet the DSM-IV criteria for obsessive-compulsive personality disorder yet not have any diagnostic features in common!
The differences among patients sharing the same diagnosis are not trivial. For example, only a subset of persons who meet the DSM-IV criteria for antisocial personality disorder will have the prototypical features of the cal- lous, ruthless, arrogant, charming, and scheming psychopath (Hare, 2003),
  
24
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

and there are even important differences among persons who would be diag- nosed as psychopathic (Brinkley, Newman, Widiger, & Lynam, 2004; Skeem, Poythress, Edens, Lilienfeld, & Cale, 2003).
Excessive Diagnostic Co-occurrence
DSM-IV provides diagnostic criterion sets to help guide the clinician toward the correct diagnosis and a section devoted to differential diagnosis that indicates “how to differentiate [the] disorder from other disorders that have similar presenting characteristics” (APA, 2000, p. 10). The intention of this information is to help the clinician determine which particular disorder is present, the selection of which will indicate the presence of a specific pathology that will explain the occurrence of the symptoms and suggest a specific treatment that will ameliorate the patient’s suffering (Frances, First, & Pincus, 1995).
It is evident, however, that DSM-IV routinely fails in the goal of guiding the clinician to the presence of one specific personality disorder. Studies have con- sistently indicated that many patients meet diagnostic criteria for an excessive number of personality disorder diagnoses (Bornstein, 1998; Lilienfeld, Waldman, & Israel, 1994; Livesley, 2003; Widiger & Trull, 1998). The mal- adaptive personality functioning of patients does not appear to be ade- quately described by a single diagnostic category.
Inconsistent, Unstable,
and Arbitrary Diagnostic Boundaries
An additional problem has been establishing a consistent, nonarbitrary boundary with normal personality functioning. The existing diagnostic thresholds lack a compelling rationale (Tyrer & Johnson, 1996). In fact, no explanation or justification has ever been provided for most of them (Samuel & Widiger, 2006). The thresholds for the DSM-III schizotypal and border- line diagnoses are the only two for which a published rationale has ever been provided. The DSM-III requirements that the patient have four of eight fea- tures for the schizotypal diagnosis and five of eight for borderline (APA, 1980) were determined on the basis of maximizing agreement with similar diagnoses provided by clinicians (Spitzer, Endicott, & Gibbon, 1979). However, the current diagnostic thresholds for these personality disorders bear little relationship with the original thresholds established for DSM-III. Blashfield, Blum, and Pfohl (1992) reported a kappa of only -.025 for the DSM-III and DSM-III-R schizotypal personality disorders, with a reduction in prevalence from 11% to 1%. Seemingly minor changes to diagnostic cri- terion sets have resulted in unexpected and substantial shifts in prevalence rates that profoundly complicate scientific theory and public health decisions (Blashfield et al., 1992; Narrow, Rae, Robins, & Regier, 2002).
  
Alternatives to DSM-IV 25

Inadequate Coverage
Clinicians provide a diagnosis of personality disorder not otherwise speci- fied (PDNOS) when they determine that a person has a personality disorder not adequately represented by any one of the 10 officially recognized diag- noses (APA, 2000). PDNOS is often the single most frequently used diagno- sis in clinical practice, one explanation being that the existing categories are not providing adequate coverage (Verheul & Widiger, 2004). Westen and Arkowitz-Westen (1998) surveyed 238 psychiatrists and psychologists with respect to their clinical practice and reported that “the majority of patients with personality pathology significant enough to warrant clinical psychother- apeutic attention (60.6%) are currently undiagnosable on Axis II” (p. 1769). The clinicians reported the treatment of commitment difficulties, intimacy problems, shyness, work inhibition, perfectionism, and devaluation of others, issues not well described by any of the existing diagnoses. One approach to this problem is to add more diagnostic categories, but there is considerable reluctance to do so, in part because this would further increase the difficulties of excessive diagnostic co-occurrence and differential diagnosis (Pincus, McQueen, & Elinson, 2003).
Inadequate Scientific Base
Blashfield and Intoccia (2000) conducted a computer search and con- cluded that there were “five disorders (dependent, narcissistic, obsessive- compulsive, paranoid, and passive-aggressive) that had very small literatures, averaging fewer than 10 articles per year” (p. 473). “The only personality dis- order whose literature is clearly alive and growing is that of borderline per- sonality disorder” (p. 473). They characterized the literature concerning the dependent, narcissistic, obsessive-compulsive, paranoid, passive-aggressive, schizoid, and histrionic personality disorders as “dead” or “dying” (p. 473).
The conclusions of Blashfield and Intoccia (2000) may have been overly negative. Their search appears to have missed (for example) the considerable research literature concerning psychopathy (Hare, 2003), the many studies on sociotropy, dependency, and attachment (Bornstein, 1992), and the many studies concerning narcissism and conflicted self-esteem published in the gen- eral personality literature (Ronningstam, 2005). Nevertheless, their warning is well taken: “Disorders with literature growth that is dead or dying are not suc- ceeding at accumulating new empirical knowledge, nor are they likely to be stimulating substantial clinical interest” (Blashfield & Intoccia, 2000, p. 473). DSM-III provided specific and explicit criterion sets, in part to facilitate the development of systematic empirical studies that would provide the Robins and Guze (1970) follow-up, family history, and laboratory construct valida- tion studies that would document their validity as distinct diagnostic entities (Spitzer et al., 1980). Yet it is challenging to even find a systematic study devoted to an understanding of the etiology, pathology, family history, or
  
26 PERSONALITY DISORDERS

treatment of the histrionic, schizoid, paranoid, obsessive-compulsive, and other personality disorders despite the provision over 20 years ago of research diagnostic criteria within DSM-III.
Proposals for DSM-V ________________________________
Quite a few proposals will be considered by the authors of DSM-V to address the above problems. These proposals will run the gamut from tin- kering with criterion sets to improve their specificity to deleting the more problematic or weakly validated diagnoses. Described below are proposals that involve fundamental alterations.
Abandonment of the Concept of Personality Disorder
One proposal for DSM-V is to abandon the concept of a personality dis- order. Proposals have been made to redefine personality disorders as early- onset, chronic variants of existing Axis I disorders (First et al., 2002; Siever & Davis, 1991). A strong precedent for such a shift in conceptualization and classification is schizotypal personality disorder. Schizotypal personality dis- order is genetically related to schizophrenia, most of its neurobiological risk factors and psychophysiological correlates are shared with schizophrenia (e.g., eye tracking, orienting, startle blink, and neurodevelopmental abnor- malities), and the treatments that are effective in ameliorating schizotypal symptoms overlap with treatments used for persons with schizophrenia (Raine, 2006). In fact, the World Health Organization’s (WHO) International Classification of Diseases (ICD; WHO, 1992), the parent classification to the APA diagnostic manual, does not recognize the existence of schizotypal personality disorder, instead including a diagnosis of schizotypal disorder within its manual’s section on disorders of schizophrenia. It is quite possible that a DSM-V personality disorders work group will follow the lead of the WHO and transfer schizotypal personality disorder out of the personality disorders section and into the section of the manual dealing with schizo- phrenia-related disorders (First et al., 2002).
A similar fate could befall other personality disorders. Just as schizotypal personality disorder could be readily subsumed within an existing section of Axis I, avoidant personality disorder could be reclassified as a generalized social phobia (Liebowitz et al., 1998); depressive personality disorder (cur- rently in an appendix to DSM-IV; APA, 2000) as an early-onset dysthymia; borderline personality disorder as an affective dysregulation and/or impulse dyscontrol disorder; schizoid as an early-onset and chronic variant of the neg- ative (anhedonic) symptoms of schizophrenic pathology (although empirical support for a genetic relationship with schizophrenia is equivocal [Miller, Useda, Trull, Burrs, & Minks-Brown, 2001]); paranoid personality disorder
   
Alternatives to DSM-IV 27

as an early-onset, chronic, and milder variant of a delusional disorder; obsessive- compulsive personality disorder as a generalized and chronic variant of obsessive-compulsive anxiety disorder (although there is in fact only weak evidence to support a close relationship between the obsessive-compulsive anxiety and personality disorders [Costa, Samuels, Bagby, Daffin, & Norton, 2005]); antisocial personality disorder as an adult variant of conduct disor- der; and passive-aggressive personality disorder (currently in an appendix to DSM-IV [APA, 2000]) as an adult variant of oppositional defiant disorder (with which it was explicitly affiliated in DSM-III [APA, 1980]). Three per- sonality disorders would be left behind in such a move as they have no appar- ent location within Axis I: histrionic, narcissistic, and dependent. For some, however, these diagnoses would not be greatly missed, as they have a strong psychodynamic heritage (Millon et al., 1996), and the trend in psychiatry appears to be toward disorders in which neurobiological etiology is better established (Charney et al., 2002; Hyman, 2002).
A concern with reformulating personality disorders as early-onset and chronic variants of Axis I disorders, beyond the fundamental consideration that the diagnostic manual would no longer recognize the existence of mal- adaptive personality functioning, is that it might create more problems than it solves (Widiger, 2003). It appears that persons have constellations of mal- adaptive personality traits not well described by just one or even multiple per- sonality disorder diagnoses (Bornstein, 1998; Livesley, 2003; Trull & Durrett, 2005; Widiger & Sanderson, 1995), but these constellations of maladaptive personality traits would be even less well described by multiple diagnoses across the broad classes of mood, anxiety, impulse dyscontrol, delusional, and disruptive behavior disorders. In addition, the existing problem of lack of cov- erage would be further worsened by the deletion of personality disorder diag- noses that could not be subsumed within an existing Axis I diagnosis.
Prototype Matching
A much less radical proposal for DSM-V is to provide a dimensional pro- file of the existing (or somewhat revised) diagnostic categories (Oldham & Skodol, 2000; Tyrer & Johnson, 1996; Widiger & Sanderson, 1995). A per- sonality disorder could be characterized as prototypical if all of the diagnos- tic criteria are met, moderately present if one or two criteria beyond the threshold for a categorical diagnosis are present, threshold if the patient just barely meets diagnostic threshold, subthreshold if symptoms are present but just below diagnostic threshold, traits if no more than one to three symp- toms are present, and absent if no diagnostic criteria are present (Oldham & Skodol, 2000). Oldham and Skodol propose further that, if a patient meets diagnostic criteria for three or more personality disorders, a diagnosis of “extensive personality disorder” be provided, along with an indication of the extent to which each personality disorder is present.
  
28
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

This proposed revision would be beneficial in shifting clinicians some- what toward a more dimensional model of classification. It would encour- age them to provide a more comprehensive and precise description of a patient’s profile of maladaptive personality functioning and would discour- age fruitless differential diagnoses of overlapping constructs. In addition, reliability of diagnoses do generally improve when a more quantitative, dimensional classification is used (Widiger & Sanderson, 1995).
Another version of the prototype matching proposal is provided by Westen and Shedler (2000). This proposal is similar to Oldham and Skodol’s (2000) in that it largely retains the existing diagnostic categories, each of which would be rated on a five-point scale. However, an important differ- ence is that Westen and Shedler’s five-point rating is not based on the number of diagnostic criteria. They propose instead that the diagnostic man- ual provide a narrative description of a prototypical case of each personality disorder (half to full page, containing 18 to 20 features), and that the clini- cian indicate on a five-point scale the extent to which a patient matches this description (i.e., 1 = little to no match; 2 = slight match, only minor features; 3 = significant match; 4 = good match, patient has the disorder; and 5 = very good match, prototypical case). “To make a diagnosis, diagnosticians rate the overall similarity or ‘match’ between a patient and the prototype using a 5-point rating scale, considering the prototype as a whole rather than count- ing individual symptoms” (Westen, Shedler, & Bradley, 2006, p. 847).
A limitation of the prototype matching proposal of Oldham and Skodol (2000) is that clinicians would continue to describe patients in terms of markedly heterogeneous and overlapping constructs. A profile description of a patient in terms of the antisocial, borderline, dependent, histrionic, and other DSM-IV constructs would essentially just reify the excessive diagnostic co-occurrence that is currently being obtained (Bornstein, 1998; Lilienfeld et al., 1994). The problem of excessive diagnostic co-occurrence would be “solved” by simply accepting it. This is comparable to the decision made by the authors of DSM-III-R (APA, 1987) to address the problematic hetero- geneity of the diagnostic categories by abandoning monothetic criterion sets that required homogeneity and converting to polythetic criterion sets that accepted the existence of the problematic heterogeneity (Widiger et al., 1988).
Westen et al. (2006) suggest that their version of the prototype matching procedure does actually address the problem of diagnostic co-occurrence. They empirically compared the extent of diagnostic co-occurrence obtained with their prototype matching with the extent of diagnostic co-occurrence obtained when the same clinicians systematically consider each DSM-IV diag- nostic criterion. They reported considerably less diagnostic co-occurrence with their prototype matching. However, their findings could in fact indicate that their prototype matching procedure is now “solving” the problem of diagnostic co-occurrence by simply neglecting to provide an adequate recog- nition of its existence. The fact that diagnostic co-occurrence increased when the clinicians were encouraged to consider specific features of other
  
Alternatives to DSM-IV 29

personality disorders suggests that the co-occurrence is actually present but is not being recognized when clinicians are allowed to base their diagnoses on whatever feature or features they wish to consider. Prior studies have shown that clinicians who do not systematically use diagnostic criterion sets grossly underestimate the extent of maladaptive personality function- ing actually present (Blashfield & Herkov, 1996; Wood et al., 2002; Zimmerman & Mattia, 1999a, 1999b).
A concern regarding the narrative prototype matching procedure is that it is essentially a return to the method used for DSM-II (APA, 1968), in which only a narrative description of each disorder was provided. Clinical diag- noses were grossly unreliable prior to DSM-III because no specific or explicit guidelines were provided regarding which features should be used to deter- mine whether or not a person was close enough to a prototypical case to warrant a diagnosis (Spitzer et al., 1980). One had no way of knowing which features, or even how many, were being used to make a diagnosis. Westen et al. (2006), however, suggest that reliable and valid diagnoses can be obtained in the absence of specific and explicit criterion sets. Rather than spending 2 hours systematically assessing each diagnostic criterion, “clini- cians could make a complete Axis II diagnosis in 1 or 2 minutes” (Westen et al., 2006, p. 855). They reported in their study good validity for their pro- totype matching approach. However, it should perhaps be noted that the validity data were in all cases provided by the same clinicians who conducted the prototype matching. Validation was confined simply to the internal con- sistency of each clinician’s description (Smith, 2005).
One of the more telling findings of Westen et al. (2006) was their com- parison of prototype matching based on their rich narrative descriptions (consisting of 18 to 20 features) with matching based on a grossly simplified description consisting of just one sentence (i.e., the single essential feature of each respective personality disorder described in DSM-IV [APA, 2000]). They compared the validity of the richer narrative prototype matching with the validity of the grossly simplified version to determine if the prototype match procedure outperformed the specific and explicit criterion sets simply because it provided more information. They discovered that “the data were strikingly similar” (p. 852) even when the clinicians based the matching on just one sentence. “The rate of comorbidity was [again] substantially lower than with the DSM-IV dimensional diagnoses . . . and the pattern of exter- nal correlates was equivalent” (p. 852). In other words, the rich depth and breadth of information provided within the narrative descriptions wasn’t actually necessary, perhaps because the clinicians had already reached their conclusions months before the study was conducted (average length of treat- ment prior to onset of study was over 1 year).
Specific and explicit criterion sets may not be useful or necessary for reach- ing a diagnosis once a clinician is well familiar with the patient (e.g., after 1 year of treatment). A diagnostic manual is generally used during an intake for treatment or research. At this time, the clinician can be very unfamiliar with
  
30
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

the patient’s symptomatology, and specific and explicit criterion sets can be very useful, if not necessary, for obtaining reliable and valid diagnosis. They compel the clinician to assess for the presence of a given mental disorder in a systematic, comprehensive, consistent, and replicable manner (Rogers, 2003; Spitzer et al., 1980; Zimmerman, 2003).
The prototype matching of Westen and Shedler (2000) can be supported by the Shedler-Westen Assessment Procedure–200 (SWAP-200), a clinician’s ranking form of 200 items drawn from the psychoanalytic and personality disorder literature (Shedler, 2002; Westen & Shedler, 1999a). The SWAP- 200 is “a method for studying personality and personality pathology that strives to capture the richness and complexity of psychoanalytic constructs and formulations” (Shedler, 2002, p. 429). Comparable to the descriptions of the narrative prototype matching procedure, SWAP-200 items are not ranked on the basis of a systematic administration of a series of questions; instead, the SWAP-200 relies on “the empathically attuned and dynamically sophisticated clinician given free rein to practice his or her craft” (Shedler, 2002, p. 433). Nevertheless, the requirement that clinicians consider each of the 200 SWAP-200 items does at least ensure that they do not simply ignore many of the sentences within the narrative descriptions.
Initial research with the SWAP-200 has reported good to excellent con- vergent and discriminant validity (e.g., Westen & Shedler, 1999b; Westen, Shedler, Durrett, Glass, & Martens, 2003). However, the positive results obtained with the SWAP-200 should be tempered by methodological limita- tions of the initial research (Widiger & Samuel, 2005b). For example, clini- cians who provided the personality disorder criterion rankings were again the same persons who provided the SWAP-200 rankings (e.g., Conklin & Westen, 2005; Shedler & Westen, 2004; Westen & Shedler, 1999a, 1999b; Westen et al., 2003). This is comparable to having conductors of semistruc- tured interviews provide their own criterion diagnoses in a study testing the validity of their semistructured interview assessments. Determining whether the results of the interview are in agreement with the impressions of the per- son conducting the interview is not a compelling test of the validity of the interview (Smith, 2005). An additional methodological concern is that the clinicians in each study have been provided with guidelines for the distribu- tion of their rankings. For example, in the typical SWAP-200 administration, clinicians are required to identify half of the items as absent and also required to identify an increasingly restrictive distribution of more highly ranked items. Only eight SWAP-200 items can be given the highest rankings (e.g., Westen & Shedler, 1999b), no matter the opinions of the raters or the symptoms present; potentially, this could force clinicians to overcorrect and generate more discriminant validity than they actually see. (For a different view, see Chapter 7.) If a clinician has provided a patient with the highest rating of 7 for 12 SWAP-200 items, he or she is presented with a list of these 12 items and told to shift four to a lower rating. It is perhaps not surprising that clinicians often shift out of this list those items that are least convergent
  
Alternatives to DSM-IV 31

with the other items. Convergent and discriminant validity of a semistruc- tured interview would also be improved dramatically if interviewers were instructed to code half of the diagnostic criteria as absent and identify only a few of the diagnostic criteria as present.
Dimensional Model of Classification
Categorical classifications of psychopathology have become so problem- atic that an APA Research Planning Work Group for DSM-V concluded that it will be “important that consideration be given to advantages and disad- vantages of basing part or all of DSM-V on dimensions rather than cate- gories” (Rounsaville et al., 2002, p. 12). They suggested in particular that the first section of the diagnostic manual to be converted to a dimensional classification might be the personality disorders section. “If a dimensional system of personality performs well and is acceptable to clinicians, it might then be appropriate to explore dimensional approaches in other domains” (Rounsaville et al., 2002, p. 13). The APA subsequently cosponsored a series of international conferences devoted to further enriching the empirical data- base in preparation for the eventual development of DSM-V. The first con- ference was devoted to reviewing the research and setting a research agenda that would be most useful and effective in leading the field toward a dimen- sional classification of personality disorder (Widiger & Simonsen, 2005b; Widiger, Simonsen, Krueger, Livesley, & Verheul, 2005).
By one count, there are at least 18 alternative proposals for a dimensional model of personality disorder (Widiger & Simonsen, 2005a). Even if one con- fines the list to dimensional models of general personality structure that have been applied to the DSM-IV personality disorders, the list remains extensive, including, for example, Eysenck’s (1987) three dimensions of neuroticism, extraversion, and psychoticism; Harkness and McNulty’s five factors of posi- tive emotionality/extraversion, aggressiveness, constraint, negative emotional- ity/neuroticism, and psychoticism (Harkness, McNulty, & Ben-Porath, 1995); Tellegen’s (1982) three dimensions of negative affectivity (emotionality), posi- tive affectivity (emotionality), and constraint; the interpersonal circumplex dimensions of agency and communion (Pincus & Gurtman, 2006); Millon’s six polarities of self, other, active, passive, pleasure, and pain (Millon et al., 1996); Zuckerman’s (2002) five dimensions of sociability, activity, aggression- hostility, impulsive sensation seeking, and neuroticism-anxiety; Cloninger’s (2006) seven factors of novelty seeking, harm avoidance, reward dependence, persistence, self-directedness, cooperativeness, and self-transcendence; and the five-factor model dimensions of neuroticism, extraversion, openness, consci- entiousness, and agreeableness (Widiger, Costa, & McCrae, 2002).
The existence of so many alternatives could suggest that the proposals are so disparate that no consensus is likely to emerge (Millon et al., 1996). However, it is apparent that most, perhaps all, of these alternative models
  
32
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

can be integrated within a common hierarchical structure (Bouchard & Loehlin, 2001; John & Srivastava, 1999; Larstone, Jang, Livesley, Vernon, & Wolf, 2002; Markon, Krueger, & Watson, 2005; Trull & Durrett, 2005; Widiger & Mullins-Sweatt, 2005; Zuckerman, 2002). This should not be surprising, as most of them are attempting to do largely the same thing (i.e., identify the fundamental dimensions of maladaptive personality functioning that underlie and cut across the existing diagnostic categories).
Widiger and Simonsen (2005a) provide a potential integration. At one level would be four to five broad domains of general personality functioning: emo- tional instability, antagonism versus compliance, extraversion versus introver- sion, constraint versus impulsivity, and (perhaps) unconventionality. These broad domains could be confined largely to a description of normal personal- ity structure. Based on the elevations obtained within each domain, one would proceed to assess for maladaptive variants (approximately two to four within each domain) that would consist of more specific facet scales. For example, persons who evidence a high level of constraint could be assessed for the mal- adaptive variant of compulsivity, whereas persons who evidence a low level of constraint could be assessed for the maladaptive variant of impulsivity.
A dimensional classification of personality disorder would address many of the limitations inherent in a categorical classification of personality disor- der. Heterogeneity among persons is addressed through the provision of multifactorial descriptions of an individual’s personality profile. Any rea- sonably comprehensive dimensional model would also be able to cover a greater range of maladaptive personality functioning than the existing diag- nostic categories in a number of ways: by avoiding the inclusion of redun- dant, overlapping constructs; by organizing the traits within a hierarchical structure; by representing a broader range of maladaptive personality func- tioning along a single dimension (e.g., antagonism versus compliance); by including aspects of normal and abnormal personality functioning not cur- rently represented within DSM-IV (e.g., alexithymia and closed-mindedness); and by allowing for the representation of relatively unique or atypical per- sonality profiles (Widiger & Samuel, 2005a). The need for the frequently used wastebasket NOS diagnosis would decrease substantially.
An additional advantage of this approach is the integration of the classifi- cation of abnormal personality traits with the existing research on general personality structure (Widiger, Simonsen, Krueger, et al., 2005). For example, there is considerable cross-cultural support for the five-factor model of gen- eral personality structure in terms of both emic (Ashton & Lee, 2001) and etic (Allik, 2005) research. Emic studies use constructs or measures that are indigenous to a particular culture. The extensive lexical research on the struc- ture of trait terms within different languages would be considered emic research (Church, 2001). Etic studies use constructs or measures imported from one culture into another. McCrae and his colleagues’ cross-cultural study of the five-factor personality structure using observer ratings by 11,985 persons sampled from 51 different cultures would be considered an etic study
  
Alternatives to DSM-IV 33

(McCrae, Terracciano, & 78 Members of the Personality Profiles of Cultures Project, 2005). Roberts and DelVecchio (2000) documented temporal stabil- ity for the five-factor domains across the life span in their integrative meta- analysis of 152 longitudinal studies. Mervielde, De Clercq, De Fruyt, and Van Leeuwen (2005) and Shiner and Caspi (2003) indicated how the fundamen- tal childhood temperaments are well integrated within the five-factor person- ality structure. The search for the specific genes of personality has produced quite mixed results, but it is encouraging that two meta-analytic reviews of the molecular genetics research has provided support for the five-factor domain of neuroticism (Schinka, Busch, & Robichaux-Keene, 2004; Sen, Burmeister, & Ghosh, 2004). Behavior genetics research has supported the heritability of most personality traits. Yamagata et al. (2006) went further with a multivariate behavioral genetics study (using twin samples from three different countries) to provide genetic support for the specific five-factor per- sonality structure. In sum, this is a scientific foundation for construct validity that is not even remotely evident for the personality disorder diagnostic cate- gories (Blashfield & Intoccia, 2000).
A further advantage of integrating the classification of personality disor- der with general personality structure is the ability to provide a more com- prehensive description of each individual’s personality, including traits that might facilitate treatment responsivity (Widiger & Simonsen, 2005a). For example, moderate elevations within the domain of conscientiousness (or constraint) are likely to predict willingness and ability to maintain the rigors of a demanding clinical regimen, moderate levels of agreeableness (compli- ance) can suggest an increased likelihood of establishing therapeutic rapport and engagement within interpersonal models of therapy, and moderate levels of openness (unconventionality) would likely suggest interest and motivation for questioning existing cognitive schemas or engaging in a dynamic explo- ration of unconscious conflicts (Sanderson & Clarkin, 2002).
An integration of the classification of personality disorder with general personality structure might even help with the stigmatization of a mental dis- order diagnosis, as no longer would a personality disorder be conceptualized as something qualitatively distinct from general personality. All persons vary in the extent to which they are agreeable versus antagonistic or constraining versus impulsive. Persons with personality disorders would no longer be said to have disorders that are qualitatively distinct from normal psychological functioning but would instead simply be persons who have maladaptive vari- ants of the personality traits that are evident within all persons.
A limitation of this proposal is the absence of established cutoff points for clinical decisions. Inherent in a dimensional model of classification is the absence of qualitative distinctions, yet clinicians do have to make a number of specific decisions (e.g., whether to treat a patient, whether to provide a partic- ular medication, whether to provide disability coverage, and whether to hos- pitalize a patient). However, the absence of illusory qualitative distinctions within a dimensional model does not imply that no useful clinical distinctions
  
34
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

could be made. In fact, a dimensional classification could be better suited to these myriad clinical decisions than the existing diagnostic categories, as it can provide different cutoff points for different clinical decisions (Trull, 2005; Widiger & Samuel, 2005a).
Widiger et al. (2002) recommended that the cutoff point for diagnosis be based on the Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF) scale provided on Axis V of DSM-IV (APA, 2000). A score of 71 or above on the GAF indicates a normal range of functioning (e.g., problems are transient and expectable reac- tions to stressors); a score of 60 or below represents a clinically significant level of impairment (moderate difficulty in social or occupational function- ing, such as having few friends or significant conflicts with co-workers; APA, 2000). Further explication of this scale is provided by the Global Assessment of Relational Functioning (GARF) and the Social and Occupational Functioning (SOF) scales (APA, 2000). There is no empirical support for using this cutoff point to provide a diagnosis of personality disorder (First, 2005), but it is at least an explicit rationale that is consistent with the defin- ition of mental disorder provided in DSM-IV and that can be applied uni- formly across different variants of maladaptive personality functioning, in contrast to the inconsistent and unexplained diagnostic thresholds that are currently provided (Samuel & Widiger, 2006).
An additional concern is clinical utility (First, 2005; Verheul, 2005). A significant limitation of the five-factor model of general personality structure as a classification of personality disorder is that current versions of this model focus primarily on the normal variants of personality functioning (e.g., altruism, openness to aesthetics, straightforwardness) that are unlikely to be the focus of clinical interventions. However, an integrative model (Widiger & Simonsen, 2005a) would have as its facets scales from such instruments as the Dimensional Assessment of Personality Pathology (DAPP; Livesley, 2003) and the Schedule for Nonadaptive and Adaptive Personality (SNAP; Clark, Simms, Wu, & Casillas, in press) that would describe pre- cisely the behaviors and traits that are of direct clinical interest and attention (e.g., mistrust, manipulativeness, insecure attachment, identity problems, affective lability, & self-harm; Clark et al., in press; Livesley, 2003).
Conclusions ________________________________________
The problems that bedevil the personality disorder diagnoses are not theirs alone. Excessive diagnostic co-occurrence, lack of treatment specificity, and inadequate boundaries with normal psychological functioning are funda- mental problems endemic to the diagnostic manual (Rounsaville et al., 2002; Watson, 2005; Widiger & Clark, 2000). Nevertheless, these problems do appear to be particularly problematic for the personality disorders (First et al., 2002), perhaps contributing to the lack of interest among researchers in empirically studying the etiology, pathology, treatment, or course of individual
  
Alternatives to DSM-IV 35

personality disorders. It might be premature for the next edition of the diag- nostic manual to abandon the personality disorder diagnostic categories, replacing them with early onset and chronic variants of Axis I disorders or with a dimensional model of general personality structure. However, any such revision could also represent a bold and innovative paradigmatic shift that could help advance and reinvigorate a seriously troubled field. In any case, researchers should continue to work toward the further development of these proposed alternatives.
_________________________________________ References
Allik, J. (2005). Personality dimensions across cultures. Journal of Personality Disorders, 19, 212–232.
American Psychiatric Association. (1968). Diagnostic and statistical manual of men- tal disorders (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of men- tal disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., revised). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.
Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2001). A theoretical basis for the major dimensions of per- sonality. European Journal of Personality, 15, 327–353.
Blashfield, R. K., Blum, N., & Pfohl, B. (1992). The effects of changing Axis II diag- nostic criteria. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 33, 245–252.
Blashfield, R. K., & Flanagan, E. (1998). A prototypic nonprototype of a personal- ity disorder. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 186, 244–246.
Blashfield, R. K., & Herkov, M. J. (1996). Investigating clinician adherence to diagno- sis by criteria: A replication of Morey and Ochoa (1989). Journal of Personality Disorders, 10, 219–228.
Blashfield, R. K, & Intoccia, V. (2000). Growth of the literature on the topic of per- sonality disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 472–473.
Bornstein, R. F. (1992). The dependent personality: Developmental, social, and clin- ical perspectives. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 3–23.
Bornstein, R. F. (1998). Reconceptualizing personality disorder diagnosis in the DSM-V: The discriminant validity challenge. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 5, 333–343.
Bouchard, T. J., & Loehlin, J. C. (2001). Genes, evolution, and personality. Behavior Genetics, 31, 243–273.
Brinkley, C. A., Newman, J. P., Widiger, T. A., & Lynam, D. R. (1994). Two approaches to parsing the heterogeneity of psychopathy. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 69–94.
Charney, D. S., Barlow, D. H., Botteron, K., Cohen, J. D., Goldman, D., Gur, R. E., et al. (2002). Neuroscience research agenda to guide development of a patho- physiologically based classification system. In D. J. Kupfer, M. B. First, & D. A. Regier (Eds.), A research agenda for DSM-V (pp. 31–83). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

36
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Church, A. T. (2001). Personality measurement in cross-cultural perspective. Journal of Personality, 69, 979–1006.
Clark, L. A., Simms, L. J., Wu, K. D., & Casillas, A. (in press). Manual for the sched- ule for nonadaptive and adaptive personality (SNAP-2). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Cloninger, C. R. (2006). Differentiating personality deviance, normality, and well-being by the seven-factor psychobiological model. In S. Strack (Ed.), Differentiating normal and abnormal personality (2nd ed., pp. 65–81). New York: Springer.
Conklin, C. Z., & Westen, D. (2005). Borderline personality disorder in clinical prac- tice. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 867–875.
Costa, P. T., Jr., Samuels, J., Bagby, M., Daffin, L., & Norton, H. (2005). Obsessive- compulsive personality disorder: A review. In M. Maj, H. S. Akiskal, J. E. Mezzich, & A. Okasha (Eds.), Personality disorders (pp. 405–439). New York: Wiley.
Eysenck, H. J. (1987). The definition of personality disorders and the criteria appro- priate for their description. Journal of Personality Disorders, 1, 211–219. First, M. B. (2005). Clinical utility: A prerequisite for the adoption of a dimensional
approach in DSM. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 560–564.
First, M. B., Bell, C. B., Cuthbert, B., Krystal, J. H., Malison, R., Offord, D. R., et al. (2002). Personality disorders and relational disorders: A research agenda for addressing crucial gaps in DSM. In D. J. Kupfer, M. B. First, & D. A. Regier (Eds.), A research agenda for DSM-V (pp. 123–199). Washington, DC: American
Psychiatric Association.
Frances, A. J. (1980). The DSM-III personality disorders section: A commentary.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 137, 1050–1054.
Frances, A. J., First, M. B., & Pincus, H. A. (1995). DSM-IV guidebook. Washington,
DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Frances, A. J., First, M. B., Widiger, T. A., Miele, G., Tilly, S. M., Davis, W. W., et al.
(1991). An A to Z guide to DSM-IV conundrums. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 100, 407–412.
Gunderson, J. G. (1992). Diagnostic controversies. In A. Tasman & M. B. Riba
(Eds.), Review of psychiatry (Vol. 11, pp. 9–24). Washington, DC: American
Psychiatric Press.
Hare, R. D. (2003). Hare Psychopathy Checklist—Revised (PCL-R) technical man-
ual. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.
Harkness, A. R., McNulty, J. L., & Ben-Porath, Y. S. (1995). The Personality
Psychopathology Five (PSY-5): Constructs and MMPI-2 scales. Psychological
Assessment, 7, 104–114.
Herkov, M. J., & Blashfield, R. K. (1995). Clinicians’ diagnoses of personality dis-
order: Evidence of a hierarchical structure. Journal of Personality Assessment,
65, 313–321.
Hyman, S. (2002). Neuroscience, genetics, and the future of psychiatric diagnosis.
Psychopathology, 35, 139–144.
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History, mea-
surement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 102–138). New York: Guilford Press.
Larstone, R. M., Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., Vernon, P. A., & Wolf, H. (2002). The relationship between Eysenck’s P-E-N model of personality, the five-factor
Alternatives to DSM-IV 37

model of personality, and traits delineating personality dysfunction. Personality
and Individual Differences, 33, 25–37.
Leichsenring, F., & Leibing, E. (2003). The effectiveness of psychodynamic therapy
and cognitive behavior therapy in the treatment of personality disorders: A
meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1223–1232.
Liebowitz, M. R., Barlow, D. H., Ballenger, J. C., Davidson, J., Foa, E. B., Fyer, A. J., et al. (1998). DSM-IV anxiety disorders: Final overview. In T. A. Widiger, A. J. Frances, H. A. Pincus, R. Ross, M. B. First, W. Davis, et al. (Eds.), DSM-IV sourcebook
(Vol. 4, pp. 1047–1076). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Lilienfeld, S. O., Waldman, I. D., & Israel, A. C. (1994). A critical examination of the use of the term “comorbidity” in psychopathology research. Clinical Psychology:
Science and Practice, 1, 71–83.
Livesley, W. J. (2003). Diagnostic dilemmas in classifying personality disorder. In
K. A. Phillips, M. B. First, & H. A. Pincus (Eds.), Advancing DSM: Dilemmas in psychiatric diagnosis (pp. 153–190). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Markon, K.E., Krueger, R. F., & Watson, D. (2005). Delineating the structure of normal and abnormal personality: An integrative hierarchical approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 139–157.
Maser, J. D., Kaelber, C., & Weise, R. F. (1991). International use and attitudes toward DSM-III and DSM-III-R: Growing consensus in psychiatric classifica- tion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 271–279.
McCrae, R. R., Terracciano, A., & 78 Members of the Personality Profiles of Cultures Project. (2005). Universal features of personality traits from the observer’s per- spective: Data from 50 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 547–561.
Mellsop, G., Varghese, F., Joshua, S., & Hicks, A. (1982). Reliability of Axis II of DSM-III. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 1360–1361.
Mervielde, I., De Clercq, B., De Fruyt, F., & Van Leeuwen, K. (2005). Temperament, personality, and developmental psychopathology as childhood antecedents of personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 19, 171–201.
Miller, M. B., Useda, J. D., Trull, T. J., Burr, R. M., & Minks-Brown, C. (2001). Paranoid, schizoid, and schizotypal personality disorders. In P. B. Sutker & H. E. Adams (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of psychopathology (3rd ed., pp. 535–558). New York: Plenum.
Millon, T., Davis, R. D., Millon, C. M., Wenger, A. W., Van Zuilen, M. H., Fuchs, M., et al. (1996). Disorders of personality: DSM-IV and beyond. New York: Wiley.
Narrow, W. E., Rae, D. S., Robins, L. N., & Regier, D. A. (2002). Revised prevalence estimates of mental disorders in the United States: Using a clinical significance crite- rion to reconcile 2 surveys’ estimates. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 115–123.
Nathan, P. E., & Langenbucher, J. W. (1999). Psychopathology: Description and classification. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 79–107.
Nazikian, H., Rudd, R. P., Edwards, J., & Jackson, H. J. (1990). Personality disor- der assessments for psychiatric inpatients. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 24, 37–46.
Oldham, J. M., & Skodol, A. E. (2000). Charting the future of Axis II. Journal of Personality Disorders, 14, 17–29.
Perry, J. C., Banon, E., & Ianni, F. (1999). Effectiveness of psychotherapy for per- sonality disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 1312–1321.
  
38
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Pincus, A. L., & Gurtman, M. B. (2006). Interpersonal theory and the interpersonal circumplex: Evolving perspectives on normal and abnormal personality. In S. Strack (Ed.), Differentiating normal and abnormal personality (2nd ed., pp. 83–111). New York: Springer.
Pincus, H. A., McQueen, L. E., & Elinson, L. (2003). Subthreshold mental disorders: Nosological and research recommendations. In K. A. Phillips, M. B. First, & H. A. Pincus (Eds.), Advancing DSM: Dilemmas in psychiatric diagnosis (pp. 129–144). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Raine, A. (2006). Schizotypal personality: Neurodevelopmental and psychosocial trajectories. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 2, 291–326.
Roberts, B. W., & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order consistency of person- ality traits from childhood to old age: A quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 3–25.
Robins, E., & Guze, S. B. (1970). Establishment of diagnostic validity in psychiatric illness: Its application to schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 107–111.
Rogers, R. (2003). Standardizing DSM-IV diagnoses: The clinical applications of structured interviews. Journal of Personality Assessment, 81, 220–225.
Ronningstam, E. (2005). Narcissistic personality disorder: A review. In M. Maj, H. S. Akiskal, J. E. Mezzich, & A. Okasha (Eds.), Personality disorders (pp. 277–327). New York: Wiley.
Rounsaville, B. J., Alarcon, R. D., Andrews, G., Jackson, J. S., Kendell, R. E., & Kendler, K. (2002). Basic nomenclature issues for DSM-V. In D. J. Kupfer, M. B. First, & D. E. Regier (Eds.), A research agenda for DSM-V (pp. 1–29). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Samuel, D. B., & Widiger, T. A. (2006). Differentiating normal and abnormal per- sonality from the perspective of the DSM. In S. Strack (Ed.), Differentiating nor- mal and abnormal personality (2nd ed., pp. 165–183). New York: Springer.
Sanderson, C., & Clarkin, J. F. (2002). Further use of the NEO-PI-R personality dimensions in differential treatment planning. In P. T. Costa, Jr., & T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality (2nd ed., pp. 351–375). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Schinka, J. A, Busch, R. M., & Robichaux-Keene, N. (2004). A meta-analysis of the association between the serotonin transporter gene polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and trait anxiety. Molecular Psychiatry, 9, 197–202.
Sen, S., Burmeister, M., & Ghosh, D. (2004). Meta-analysis of the association between a serotonin transporter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and anxiety-related personality traits. American Journal of Medical Genetics: Part B, 127B, 85–89.
Shedler, J. (2002). A new language for psychoanalytic diagnosis. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 50, 429–456.
Shedler, J., & Westen, D. (2004). Dimensions of personality pathology: An alterna- tive to the five-factor model. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 1743–1754. Shiner, R. L., & Caspi, A. (2003). Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: Measurement, development, and consequences. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 44, 2–32.
Siever, L. J., & Davis, K. L. (1991). A psychobiological perspective on the personal-
ity disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 1647–1658.
Skeem, J. L., Poythress, N., Edens, J. F., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Cale, E. M. (2003). Psychopathic personality or personalities? Exploring potential variants of
Alternatives to DSM-IV 39

psychopathy and their implications for risk assessment. Aggression and Violent
Behavior, 8, 513–546.
Smith, G. T. (2005). On the complexity of quantifying construct validity. Psychological
Assessment, 17, 413–414.
Spitzer, R., Endicott, J., & Gibbon, M. (1979). Crossing the border into borderline
personality and borderline schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 36, 17–24. Spitzer, R. L., Forman, J. B. W., & Nee, J. (1979). DSM-III field trials: I. Initial inter-
rater diagnostic reliability. American Journal of Psychiatry, 136, 815–817. Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., & Skodol, A. E. (1980). DSM-III: The major achievements and an overview. American Journal of Psychiatry, 137, 151–164. Tellegen, A. (1982). Brief manual for the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire.
Unpublished manuscript, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis.
Trull, T. J. (2005). Dimensional models of personality disorder: Coverage and cut-
offs. Journal of Personality Disorders, 19, 262–282.
Trull, T. J., & Durrett, C. A. (2005). Categorical and dimensional models of per-
sonality disorder. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 355–380.
Tyrer, P., & Johnson, T. (1996). Establishing the severity of personality disorder.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 1593–1597.
Verheul, R. (2005). Clinical utility for dimensional models of personality pathology.
Journal of Personality Disorders, 19, 283–302. .
Verheul, R., & Widiger, T. A. (2004). A meta-analysis of the prevalence and usage
of the personality disorder not otherwise specified (PDNOS) diagnosis. Journal
of Personality Disorders, 18, 309–319.
Watson, D. (2005). Rethinking the mood and anxiety disorders: A quantitative hier-
archical model for DSM-V. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 522–536. Westen, D., & Arkowitz-Westen, L. (1998). Limitations of Axis II in diagnosing per- sonality pathology in clinical practice. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155,
1767–1771.
Westen, D., & Shedler, J. (1999a). Revising and assessing Axis II, Part I: Developing
a clinically and empirically valid assessment method. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 156, 258–272.
Westen, D., & Shedler, J. (1999b). Revising and assessing Axis II, Part II: Toward an
empirically based and clinically useful classification of personality disorders.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 273–285.
Westen, D., & Shedler, J. (2000). A prototype matching approach to diagnosing per-
sonality disorders: Toward DSM-V. Journal of Personality Disorders, 14,
109–126.
Westen, D., Shedler, J., & Bradley, R. (2006). A prototype approach to personality
disorder diagnosis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 846–856.
Westen, D., Shedler, J., Durrett, C., Glass, S., & Martens, A. (2003). Personality diagnoses in adolescence: DSM-IV Axis II diagnoses and an empirically derived
alternative. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 952–966.
Widiger, T. A. (2003). Personality disorder and Axis I psychopathology: The problem- atic boundary of Axis I and Axis II. Journal of Personality Disorders, 17, 90–108. Widiger, T. A., & Clark, L. A. (2000). Toward DSM-V and the classification of
psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 946–963.
Widiger, T. A., Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (2002). A proposal for Axis II:
Diagnosing personality disorders using the five-factor model. In P. T. Costa, Jr., & T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality disorders and the five-factor model of
40 PERSONALITY DISORDERS

personality (2nd ed., pp. 431–456). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Widiger, T., Frances, A., Spitzer, R., & Williams, J. (1988). The DSM-III-R person-
ality disorders: An overview. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 786–795. Widiger, T. A., & Mullins-Sweatt, S. (2005). Categorical and dimensional models of personality disorder. In J. Oldham, A. Skodol, & D. Bender (Eds.), Textbook of personality disorders (pp. 35–53). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Widiger, T. A., & Samuel, D. B. (2005a). Diagnostic categories or dimensions: A
question for DSM-V. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 494–504. Widiger, T. A., & Samuel, D. B. (2005b). Evidence-based assessment of personality
disorders. Psychological Assessment, 17, 278–287.
Widiger, T. A., & Sanderson, C. J. (1995). Toward a dimensional model of person-
ality disorders in DSM-IV and DSM-V. In W. J. Livesley (Ed.), The DSM-IV per-
sonality disorders (pp. 433–458). New York: Guilford Press.
Widiger, T. A., & Simonsen, E. (2005a). Alternative dimensional models of personality disorder: Finding a common ground. Journal of Personality Disorders, 19, 110–130. Widiger, T. A., & Simonsen, E. (2005b). The American Psychiatric Association’s research agenda for the DSM-V. Journal of Personality Disorders, 19, 103–109. Widiger, T. A., Simonsen, E., Krueger, R., Livesley, J., & Verheul, R. (2005). Personality disorder research agenda for the DSM-V. Journal of Personality
Disorders, 19, 317–340.
Widiger, T. A., & Trull, T. J. (1998). Performance characteristics of the DSM-III-R
personality disorder criteria sets. In T. A. Widiger, A. J. Frances, H. A. Pincus, R. Ross, M. B. First, W. W. Davis, et al. (Eds.), DSM-IV sourcebook (Vol. 4., pp. 357–373). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Wood, J. M., Garb, H. N., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Nezworski, M. T. (2002). Clinical assessment. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 519–543.
World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD-10 classification of mental and behav- ioural disorders: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
Yamagata, S., Suzuki, A., Ando, J., One, Y., Kijimi, N., Yoshimura, K., et al. (2006). Is the genetic structure of human personality universal? A cross-cultural twin study from North America, Europe, and Asia. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 987–998.
Zimmerman, M. (2003). What should the standard of care for psychiatric diagnos- tic evaluations be? Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 191, 281–286. Zimmerman, M., & Mattia, J. I. (1999a). Differences between clinical and research
practices in diagnosing borderline personality disorder. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 156, 1570–1574.
Zimmerman, M., & Mattia, J. I. (1999b). Psychiatric diagnosis in clinical practice: Is
comorbidity being missed? Comprehensive Psychiatry, 40, 182–191. Zuckerman, M. (2002). Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire (ZKPQ): An alternative five-factorial model. In B. de Raad & M. Perugini (Eds.), Big Five
assessment (pp. 377–397). Kirkland, WA: Hogrefe & Huber.

Paranoid 3 Personality Disorder
David P. Bernstein Maastricht University
J. David Useda University of Rochester School of Medicine
Disorder

________________________________ Description of the
Paranoid personality disorder (PPD) is characterized by a pervasive mistrust of other people (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994; Bernstein, Useda, & Siever, 1995; Miller, Useda, Trull, Burr, & Minks-Brown, 2001). Other common features of the disorder include quarrelsomeness, hostility, emotional coldness, hypersensitivity to slights or criticism, stubbornness, and rigidly held maladaptive beliefs of others’ intents (APA, 1994; Bernstein et al., 1995; Miller et al., 2001). The prototypical picture is of someone who is preoccupied with real or imagined slights or threats, mistrusts the inten- tions or motives of others, and rarely trusts the seemingly benign appearance of things. The guiding underlying assumption is that others are malevolent— they can betray, hurt, take advantage, or humiliate. Thus, measures must be taken to protect oneself—by keeping one’s distance from other people, not appearing weak or vulnerable, searching for signs of threat even in seemingly innocuous situations, preemptively attacking others who are viewed as threatening, and vigorously counterattacking when threatened or provoked. People with paranoid personality disorder tend to hold grudges, have “ene- mies,” are often litigious, and can be pathologically jealous, preoccupied with their partner’s supposed sexual infidelities. Thus, in many respects, their antagonistic behavior exemplifies one extreme pole of the agreeableness- antagonism dimension of the five-factor model of personality (Widiger, Trull, Clarkin, Sanderson, & Costa, 2002).
41
42
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Not surprisingly, this pattern of antagonistic behavior often causes diffi- ulties in interpersonal relationships, including provocation of the very kinds f attacks these individuals fear. For example, people with paranoid person- lity disorder may correctly deduce that others are speaking ill of them or lotting against them behind their backs, but they do not recognize that this may be a consequence of their own antagonistic behavior. It is important to ote, however, that people with paranoid personality disorder are usually ot frankly psychotic, although they may experience transient psychotic-like ymptoms under conditions of extreme stress (Miller et al., 2001). Unlike the ymptoms of psychotic disorders such as paranoid schizophrenia and delu- ional disorder, in which a frank break with reality has occurred, the nfounded beliefs in paranoid personality disorder are rarely of psychotic roportions. Thus someone with paranoid personality disorder may believe without cause that coworkers are harassing him, for example, when he eceives a call with no one responding on the other end. However, he is not ikely to believe that the CIA is plotting against him. Thus his beliefs are
lausible but often unfounded.

c o a p
n n s s s u p

r l p

CASE EXAMPLE
A woman believed, without cause, that her neighbors were harassing her by allowing their young children to make loud noise outside her apartment door. Rather than asking the neighbors to be more considerate, she stopped speaking to them and began a campaign of unceasingly antagonistic behavior: giving them “dirty looks,” pushing past them aggressively in the hallway, slamming doors, and behaving rudely toward their visitors. After over a year had passed, when the neighbors finally confronted her about her obnoxious behavior, she accused them of purposely harassing her. “Everyone knows that these doors are paper thin,” she said, “and that I can hear everything that goes on in the hall- way. You are doing it deliberately.” Nothing that the neighbors said could con- vince her otherwise. Despite their attempts to be more considerate about the noise outside her apartment, she continued to behave in a rude and aggressive manner toward them.
Neighbors and visitors commented that the woman appeared tense and angry. Her face looked like a hard mask. She was rarely seen smiling. She walked around the neighborhood wearing dark sunglasses, even on cloudy days. She was often seen yelling at her children, behavior that had earned her the nickname “the screamer” among the parents at her children’s school. She had forced her children to change schools several times within the same dis- trict because she was dissatisfied with the education they were receiving. An unstated reason, perhaps, was that she had alienated so many other parents. She worked at home during the day at a job that required her to have little con- tact with other people. She had few social contacts, and in conversation was often perceived to be sarcastic and hypercritical.

Paranoid Personality Disorder 43

This case illustrates some of the central features of PPD according to the DSM-IV (APA, 1994), including the paranoid person’s unwavering belief that others are out to harm her (PPD criterion 1), tendency to read malevo- lent intentions into otherwise benign events (PPD criterion 4), and persis- tence in holding grudges (PPD criterion 5). Many of the associated features of the disorder are also evident, including the paranoid person’s tension and hostility, aloofness, hypercriticalness, and tendency to provoke reactions in other people that confirm her conviction that others can’t be trusted.
________________________________Differential Diagnosis
PPD must be distinguished from other disorders involving paranoia, particu- larly paranoid schizophrenia and delusional disorder (APA, 1994). Unlike PPD, paranoid schizophrenia and delusional disorder involve frank delusions—that is, false beliefs of psychotic proportions. However, the pres- ence of such beliefs is not always evident, as paranoid persons may take pains to hide such “crazy” beliefs from others. Moreover, the unwarranted beliefs of someone with PPD are not always so easy to distinguish from true delu- sions. In the above case, for example, the differential diagnosis would become easier if the woman began to accuse her neighbors of bugging her telephone, intercepting and opening her mail, surreptitiously breaking into her apart- ment, and so forth. Although such suspicions are not bizarre, they are of such an extreme nature that they suggest a delusional process may be present.
Finally, people with PPD sometimes develop transient delusions when under extreme stress (Miller et al., 2001). For example, a man with long- standing PPD developed lung cancer. Shortly after receiving the diagnosis, he developed the unfounded belief that his brothers, with whom he worked in a family business, were trying to swindle him, and cut off relations with them. His delusions did not appear to have a neurological basis: they occurred in the absence of delirium and other neurological complications due to the illness, and prior to his beginning medical treatment with radia- tion and chemotherapy. After his medical condition stabilized and he began taking antidepressant medication, this belief eventually disappeared and was never spoken about again.
PPD must also be distinguished from other personality disorders that share overlapping diagnostic features, particularly the other Cluster A per- sonality disorders: schizotypal personality disorder, which can also be char- acterized by suspiciousness or paranoid ideation (schizotypal criterion 5), and schizoid personality disorder, which can also be characterized by aloof- ness (schizoid criteria 2 and 5) and emotional coldness (schizoid criterion 7; APA, 1994; Bernstein et al., 1995; Miller et al., 2001). However, schizotypal individuals display odd or eccentric ideas, peculiar thinking or speech, unusual perceptual experiences, and other “schizophrenia-like” features that
   
44
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

are not seen in PPD. Schizoid individuals, on the other hand, are socially withdrawn because of a preference for being alone rather than a desire to protect themselves from imagined threats.
PPD also shows a high degree of content overlap with personality disor- ders outside of Cluster A, particularly borderline (e.g., criterion 8, “inappro- priate, intense anger,” and criterion 9, “transient, stress-related paranoid ideation”), narcissistic (e.g., criterion 9, “arrogant and haughty”), and avoidant (e.g., criterion 1, “avoids occupational activities involving signifi- cant interpersonal contact,” criterion 3, “shows restraint within interpersonal relationships because of fear of being shamed or ridiculed,” and criterion 4, “is preoccupied with being criticized or rejected”) personality disorders (APA, 1994; Bernstein et al., 1995; Miller et al., 2001).
Outstanding Issues __________________________________
There is a paucity of empirical literature on PPD, despite the fact that this disorder is easily recognized and has long been described in the clinical and theoretical literature (Millon & Davis, 1996). Ten years ago, when we first reviewed the literature on paranoid personality disorder, we noted a similar state of affairs (Bernstein, Useda, & Siever, 1993; Bernstein et al., 1995). Over the past 10 years, the most notable develop- ments have been a promising new questionnaire for PPD (Useda, 2002) and some interesting but inconclusive research findings on the question of whether PPD is a schizophrenia spectrum disorder (Asarnow et al., 2001; Nicolson et al., 2003). Otherwise, the literature on PPD during this period has consisted mostly of single-case studies and some theoretical articles written from a psychoanalytic object relations perspective. This is a pity, because PPD deserves greater attention.
The costs of PPD include disruptive behavior in the workplace, unneces- sary litigation, relationship problems (e.g., pathological jealousy), violence (e.g., stalking), and increased psychiatric comorbidities (Miller et al., 2001). PPD patients are seen in a variety of clinical populations, and they can pose special problems for treatment when their mistrust affects the therapeutic relationship (Miller et al., 2001). Nevertheless, in our own experience, some PPD patients can achieve good therapeutic outcomes when they are given treatments appropriate to their problems (see “What Kinds of Treatments Are Likely to Be Most Effective for People With PPD?” below). Thus, there is a need for more research on PPD, including both fundamental studies (e.g., phenomenological, epidemiological, longitudinal, and laboratorial) and studies of promising assessment and treatment approaches. We hope that this chapter will stimulate more researchers to study this important but still poorly understood personality disorder.
This chapter is organized around the following unanswered questions about PPD:
   
Paranoid Personality Disorder 45

Are the DSM-IV criteria for PPD valid descriptors of the disorder?
What theoretical models might explain the features of PPD?
Is PPD a true category or a dimension that cuts across categories?
Is PPD a schizophrenia-spectrum or delusional-spectrum disorder?
Which environmental factors might contribute to the development of PPD?
What is the most accurate means of assessing PPD?
What kinds of treatments are likely to be most effective for people with PPD?
_____________________ Descriptive and Theoretical Issues
Are the DSM-IV Criteria for
PPD Valid Descriptors of the Disorder?
Paranoid phenomena have long been described in the clinical and theoret- ical literature (Millon & Davis, 1996). By the end of the 19th century, clini- cians recognized that paranoid phenomena could manifest themselves in a variety of forms and psychiatric contexts, from the frank persecutory beliefs and delusions systems often seen in schizophrenia to more subtle and cir- cumscribed forms of paranoia (Millon & Davis, 1996). Kraepelin’s (1921) seminal nosology of psychiatric disorders described three types of paranoid conditions resembling today’s conceptualizations of paranoid schizophrenia, delusional disorder, and paranoid personality disorder, respectively. Most rel- evant to the present discussion, Kraepelin recognized that some individuals displayed milder paranoid phenomena characterized by fixed delusions but without the hallucinations and chronic deterioration of personality seen in dementia praecox (i.e., schizophrenia). A long-standing debate, which con- tinues to this day, centers on the question of whether less severe paranoid dis- orders, such as PPD and delusional disorder, lie on a genetic continuum with schizophrenia, or whether they form a paranoid spectrum distinct from that of schizophrenia (Asarnow et al., 2001; Coryell & Zimmerman, 1989; Kendler & Gruenberg, 1982; Kendler, Masterson, & Davis, 1985; Maier, Lichtermann, Minges, & Heun, 1994; Nicolson et al., 2003).
A diagnostic term for paranoid personality disorder has existed in the psy- chiatric nomenclature of the United States since 1952 (APA, 1952) and has been included in every edition of the DSM. DSM-III (APA, 1980) and sub- sequent editions of the DSM recognized three personality disorders charac- terized by oddness and eccentricity: paranoid, schizoid, and schizotypal personality disorders. The description of schizotypal personality was based largely on features—such as suspiciousness, peculiar ideation, and peculiar
   
46
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

interpersonal behavior—seen in the biological relatives of schizophrenics in the Danish Adoption Study of the 1960s (Kety, Rosenthal, Wender, & Schulsinger, 1968). Thus from its inception, schizotypal personality disorder was conceptualized as a schizophrenia spectrum disorder. Although individ- uals with PPD are sometimes seen among the relatives of schizophrenic probands, the genetic connection between PPD and schizophrenia remains an open question (see below).
The six main traits associated with PPD, as described in the extensive clin- ical and theoretical literature on PPD (Cameron, 1943, 1963; Kraepelin, 1921; Kretschmer, 1925; Millon, 1969, 1981; Shapiro, 1965; Sheldon, 1940; Sheldon & Stevens, 1942; Turkat, 1985) and by the DSM-IV’s Associated Features section (APA, 1994), are mistrust/suspiciousness, antagonism/ aggressiveness, introversion/excessive autonomy, hypersensitivity, hypervig- ilance, and rigidity. Mistrust/suspiciousness is a lack of trust in others. PPD individuals question the loyalty or truthfulness of other people and are prone to think that others are “out to get them” and to respond defensively. Hypersensitivity is one’s tendency to perceive others’ remarks or comments as attacks or criticisms directed against oneself, one’s beliefs, or one’s per- formance of a task. PPD individuals tend to believe that others are judging them negatively, and, in response, they tend to experience anger and anxi- ety. Antagonism/aggressiveness is a tendency to feel angry, to be combative toward others, and to view others and the world as hostile. Introversion/ excessive autonomy is a tendency to distance oneself from others, remain aloof, and feel tense around others. Hypervigilance is a tendency to contin- ually scan the environment in an attempt to confirm hypotheses about the malevolent intentions or motives of others. Rigidity is a personality trait in which an individual’s beliefs, behavior, and affective style are not readily open to questioning or change.
The DSM-IV criteria for PPD, however, appear to overrepresent the cognitive PPD trait mistrust/suspiciousness and to underrepresent the proto- typical behavioral, affective, and interpersonal expressions of paranoid per- sonality traits (Table 3.1). As is evident from Table 3.1, nearly all of the DSM-IV PPD criteria (six of seven) reflect the cognitive trait of mistrust/ suspiciousness. Three of the seven DSM-IV criteria reflect the affective/ interpersonal trait of hypersensitivity; two reflect the affective/interpersonal trait of antagonism; two reflect the cognitive trait of hypervigilance; one reflects the interpersonal trait of introversion; and one reflects the cognitive trait of rigidity. Thus the DSM-IV criteria mainly reflect the cognitive features of PPD, especially mistrust/suspiciousness, and poorly represent the other pri- mary traits that have long been thought to underlie the disorder. Ironically, a sample selected on the basis of DSM-IV criteria without additional assess- ment would not be representative of PPD as it is represented in the DSM-IV’s own description (i.e., in the Associated Features section for PPD) or in the clinical literature specific to PPD. For this reason, the diagnostic criteria for PPD appear to be greatly in need of revision.
  
Paranoid Personality Disorder
47
 
Table 3.1 Correspondence of DSM-IV PPD diagnostic criteria to the primary traits of PPD
 
Individuals Exhibit the DSM-IV Diagnostic Criteria for PPD When They . . .
1. Suspect that others exploit, harm, or deceive
2. Doubt others’ loyalty or trustworthiness
3. Are reluctant to confide in others for fear that information will be used against them
4. Read hidden or demeaning meanings into benign remarks or events
5. Bear grudges (i.e., are unforgiving of insults, injuries, or slights)
6. Perceive attacks on their character or reputation and are quick to react with anger or counterattack
7. Have recurrent suspicions of their partner’s sexual infidelity
Mistrust/ Suspiciousness
Yes
Yes Yes
Yes
No
Yes
Yes
Introversion/ Antagonism/ Excessive
Aggressiveness Autonomy
No No
No No No Yes
No No
Yes No
Yes No
No No
Hyper- Hyper-
sensitivity vigilance Rigidity
No
No No
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
For the DSM-V, we recommend that the PPD criteria
revised to reflect the primary traits underlying the disorder in
and proportionate manner. Such a revision should result in
tic validity for PPD and may reduce the now considerable diagnostic overlap between PPD and other Axis II disorders that share some paranoid features (e.g., borderline, schizotypal, narcissistic, and avoidant personality disorders). An improved PPD criteria set would have important ramifications for this underresearched and often clinically neglected personality disorder. For one thing, it would lead to more accurate selection of PPD cases for research
Does This Behavior Correspond to the Six Main Traits Associated With PPD?
   
be substantially a more balanced greater diagnos-
No No
No No No No
Yes No
No Yes
Yes No
No No
 
SOURCE: Reprinted with permission from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision, (Copyright 2000). American Psychiatric Association.
48
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

purposes. For example, diagnostic criteria that more accurately reflect the phenotype of PPD would be of considerable benefit for future family/genetics studies, including studies that seek to identify a schizophrenia-spectrum or delusional-spectrum genotype. From a clinical perspective, criteria that better reflect the underlying traits of PPD should lead to better assessment and, as everyone hopes, better treatment of PPD. A self-report questionnaire assessing these revised criteria, the Paranoid Personality Disorder Features Questionnaire (Useda, 2002), has recently been developed and has shown promising initial evidence of reliability and validity (see below).
What Theoretical Models
Might Explain the Features of PPD?
Traditional psychoanalytic models of PPD have focused on the defense mechanism of projection: that is, the disavowing of one’s own aggressive feel- ings and thoughts by projecting them onto another person (e.g., “I’m not feel- ing hostile toward you; you are feeling hostile toward me!” Vaillant, 1994). Despite controversy over the validity of the concept of projection (Holmes, 1978), there is considerable empirical support for the notion that patients with severe personality disorders employ projection and other maladaptive defenses. For example, studies using both self-rated and clinician-rated measures of defensive style have found that the use of projection as a defense is highly asso- ciated with severe personality disorders, including paranoid, borderline, and antisocial personality disorders (Drake & Vaillant, 1985; Koenigsberg et al., 2001; Lingiardi et al., 1999; Paris, Zweig-Frank, Bond, & Guzder, 1996). Moreover, improvements in maladaptive defenses such as projection predict the outcome of psychodynamic psychotherapy over and above other predictors, including the severity of pretreatment psychopathology (Bond & Perry, 2004).
However, paranoid phenomena may also be explained using more con- temporary cognitive processing models (Williams, Watts, MacLeod, & Mathews, 1997). For example, the misattributions of people with PPD (e.g., reading malevolent intentions into benign remarks or events) may be under- stood in terms of characteristic cognitive biases (Williams et al., 1997), including attentional biases (e.g., overfocusing on possible signs of threat), interpretative biases (e.g., misinterpreting innocuous comments or behavior as malevolent), and memory biases (e.g., dwelling on past slights or insults). Such cognitive processing models have the advantage of being amenable to empirical testing using standardized laboratory procedures. Moreover, the development of cognitive models has led to more effective forms of cognitive therapy for disorders, such as social phobia, that, like PPD, are characterized by misinterpretation of social cues (Bögels & Mansell, 2004). Because cog- nitive schemas, such as the conviction that others are not trustworthy, appear to be so central to PPD pathology (Beck, Freeman, & Associates, 1990), cognitive information processing models may prove to be highly fruitful in terms of our understanding and treating this difficult disorder.
  
Paranoid Personality Disorder 49

Beck and his colleagues (1990) have argued that the core cognitive schemas in PPD concern feelings of inadequacy. These feelings of inade- quacy, in combination with poor social skills and the external attribution of blame as a means of reducing anxiety, account for the features of PPD. In some respects, the cognitive biases in PPD described by Beck and his col- leagues (1990) resemble those in social phobia, which is also characterized by feelings of inadequacy and the overfocusing on and misinterpretation of social cues (Bögels & Mansell, 2004). Unlike social phobics, however, who are painfully aware of their feelings of adequacy and dwell on their past social blunders, people with PPD attribute the cause of their feelings of inad- equacy to others (“I am not inferior; you are trying to make me feel infe- rior!”). Thus, the central interpretative bias in PPD appears to be a causal misattribution or externalization of blame onto other people (Beck et al., 1990). Rather than dwelling on their past social mistakes, as in social pho- bia, people with PPD ruminate on the injuries and injustices others have caused them. Beck and his colleagues’ cognitive model of PPD appears to be a useful one that warrants empirical investigation.
Another potentially useful model is the integrative cognitive model of Jeffrey Young (Bernstein, 2005; Young, Klosko, & Weishaar, 2003). Young has posited that early maladaptive schemas (i.e., chronic, repetitive self-defeating themes or patterns originating in adverse childhood experi- ences and early temperament), schema modes (i.e., transient state–related patterns of schematic activation), and maladaptive coping mechanisms (i.e., maladaptive ways of coping with schematic activation) are the conceptual core of personality disorders. Young has identified 18 specific early mal- adaptive schemas (e.g., defectiveness, abandonment, emotional depriva- tion) as well as a variety of schema modes and three broad forms of maladaptive coping (i.e., schema surrender, schema avoidance, and schema overcompensation). Young and his colleagues have developed an integra- tive psychotherapy for personality disorders, schema therapy, which targets these maladaptive beliefs and coping mechanisms (Young et al., 2003). In a recent 3-year randomized clinical trial in the Netherlands, schema therapy produced substantial reductions in the features of borderline personality disorder, including improvements in the core personality characteristics (e.g., identity disturbance, unstable relationships) and behavioral features of the disorder (e.g., suicidal and parasuicidal behavior; Giesen-Bloo, Arntz, van Dyck, Spinhoven, & van Tilburg, 2004). A randomized clinical trial of the efficacy of schema therapy for PPD and other personality disorders (e.g., narcissistic, obsessive-compulsive, avoidant) is currently under way in the Netherlands (A. Arntz, personal communication, November 10, 2005).
Factor-analytic studies of the Young Schema Questionnaire, a self-report measure of early maladaptive schemas, have supported the validity of nearly all of the 18 early maladaptive schemas proposed by Young (Schmidt, Joiner, Young, & Telch, 1995; Waller, Meyer, & Ohanian, 2001). Retrospective studies have also found that early maladaptive schemas are associated with
  
50
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

etiologic factors hypothesized by Young, such as childhood trauma and insecure attachment, in both clinical and nonclinical samples (Cecero, Nelson, & Gillie, 2004; Leung, Thomas, & Waller, 2000; Waller, Meyer, Ohanian, Elliott, et al., 2001). Thus empirical research on the schema ther- apy model, though still in its early stages, supports the construct validity of early maladaptive schemas and related concepts.
Young has not applied his model specifically to PPD. However, the early maladaptive schemas that appear most relevant to PPD are defectiveness/ shame, abuse/mistrust, and vulnerability to harm. Thus the person with PPD has deep feelings of inferiority and inadequacy (i.e., defectiveness); antici- pates that others are out to harm, exploit, or humiliate him (i.e., abuse/ mistrust); and feels fundamentally unsafe in the world (i.e., vulnerability to harm). As a result, he adopts a belligerent, overcompensating form of cop- ing: he presents a hostile, aggressive face toward others, remains vigilant to possible attacks, and preemptively attacks or counterattacks in situations where he feels that he will be, or has been, harmed. Young’s model would also posit that these early maladaptive schemas, schema modes, and mal- adaptive coping responses have their origins in childhood experiences, such as early experiences of abuse or neglect. Like Beck and his colleagues’ (1990) model, Young’s model (Young et al., 2003) appears to hold considerable promise for aiding our understanding of the cognitive mechanisms in PPD and is worthy of empirical investigation.
Empirical Issues_____________________________________
Although PPD has a long history in the clinical and theoretical literature (Cameron, 1943, 1963; Kraepelin, 1921; Kretschmer, 1925; Millon, 1969, 1981; Shapiro, 1965; Sheldon, 1940; Sheldon & Stevens, 1942; Turkat, 1985), research to date has provided only limited information about the course, family history, and treatment of this severe personality disorder.
Prevalence. The prevalence of PPD appears to range from about 0.5% to 2.5% in the general population, from about 2% to 10% in psychiatric out- patient settings, and from about 10% to 30% in psychiatric inpatient settings (APA, 1994; Bernstein, Useda, et al., 1993; Bernstein et al., 1995; Miller et al., 2001). However, these epidemiological data were based on DSM-III or DSM-III-R criteria for PPD; no more recent data on the popula- tion prevalence of PPD based on DSM-IV criteria are available. PPD has been found to be more prevalent among males than among females in clini- cal samples (APA, 1994; Bernstein, Useda, et al., 1993; Bernstein et al., 1995; Miller et al., 2001).
Longitudinal Course. There is scant evidence regarding the longitudinal course of PPD (Bernstein, Cohen, et al., 1993). However, recent evidence sug- gests that personality disorders in general exhibit a more fluctuating course than was previously believed possible (Grilo et al., 2004; Warner et al.,
  
Paranoid Personality Disorder 51

2004). Thus even people with severe personality disorders, such as borderlin personality disorder, may live for months or years without presenting seriou manifestations of the disorder. Thus, it appears that the phenotypic manifes tations of personality disorders exhibit a variable course over the life span probably due to factors such as life stress and adaptation to changing life cir cumstances, while the underlying trait vulnerability for personality disorder appears to remain intact (Warner et al., 2004). On the other hand, it seem possible that some individuals “grow out of” their personality disorders either because they obtain professional help or, more typical, due to amelio rative life experiences. There is no obvious reason that these consideration would not apply equally to PPD as they would to other severe personalit disorders.
e s – , – s s , – s y
CASE EXAMPLE
A divorced man with long-standing PPD became noticeably warmer, less critical, more trusting, and more open to other people after he happily remarried and expe- rienced the birth of his second child, a beloved daughter. Although he remained emotionally aloof compared to the average person and at times could still be sarcastic, belittling, and combative, these traits had considerably diminished—a noticeable and welcome change for friends, coworkers, and family members.
r s s r s –
f
; d s y d a ,
y l
Is PPD a True Category or a
Dimension That Cuts Across Categories?
Diagnostic Comorbidity. Patients with PPD often exhibit features of othe personality disorders, especially the other Cluster A personality disorder (i.e., schizoid and schizotypal), but also the Cluster B personality disorder (i.e., borderline, narcissistic, and antisocial) and avoidant personality disorde (APA, 1994; Bernstein et al., 1995; Miller et al., 2001). In fact, one rarely find a case of PPD that is not accompanied by one or more other comorbid per sonality disorders (Widiger & Rogers, 1989; Zimmerman, 1994). Studies o inpatient samples indicate that three fourths of those diagnosed with PPD receive additional personality disorder diagnoses (Widiger & Trull, 1998 Zimmerman, 1994). In studying an outpatient sample, Morey (1988) reporte that those diagnosed with PPD most frequently received additional diagnose of borderline (48%), narcissistic (35.9%), and avoidant (48.4%) personalit disorders. In their review of the performance of the DSM-III-R criteria base on published and unpublished data sets, Widiger and Trull (1998) reported high degree of overlap (i.e., > 38%) between PPD and borderline, avoidant schizoid, schizotypal, and narcissistic personality disorders.
There are several potential explanations for the high degree of comorbidit between PPD and other personality disorders, including phenomenologica
52
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

imilarity between the personality disorders, artifactual overlap due to impre-
ision in the DSM diagnostic criteria, and the presence of shared or related underlying pathological processes. For example, both PPD and narcissistic personality disorder may share an underlying pathological process, namely, overcompensation for underlying feelings of inferiority and inadequacy. Thus, both paranoid and narcissistic individuals may be hypersensitive to slights or potential humiliations, but they cope with them in different ways: the para- noid person through aggression, and the narcissistic person through grandios-
ty. Clinical observation suggests that some individuals develop both paranoid nd narcissistic forms of adaptation and therefore exhibit a comorbidity between the personality disorders based on a shared underlying etiology. Such ndividuals may be observed to fluctuate between paranoid and narcissistic modes of adaptation (Young, 2003), depending on situational factors, such as
riggering life events.
s c
i a
i t
CASE EXAMPLE
A successful businessman usually acted as if he were “on top of the world”— dressing impeccably, making sure that he was seen in the company of famous people and in all the “right places,” and trumpeting his accomplishments to anyone who would listen. However, when his business began to crumble, he blamed members of his management team, whom he accused of conspiring against him, smeared their reputations, and forced some of them to resign from the company. Thus when his usual narcissistic, grandiose mode of compensa- tion failed and his inadequacies were publicly exposed, he resorted to an attack on his “enemies,” whom he perceived to be the source of his humiliation.
Categories Versus Dimensions. The high rates of comorbidity between PPD and other personality disorders, especially across clusters, raises the question of whether PPD is truly a discrete disorder or is more accurately described as a dimension that cuts across diagnostic categories. These issues
ouch on a major debate regarding personality disorder measurement: the dimensional versus categorical approach to personality disorder classifica- ion (Widiger & Frances, 2002). The dimensional approach focuses on the degree to which one exhibits a syndrome or construct. Although a variety of dimensional systems for personality disorders have been proposed, there ppear to be two major variants of the dimensional approach. In the first pproach, personality disorders are conceptualized as the extreme ends of dimensions that are shared with normal personality (Widiger & Frances, 2002). Most of the research based on this “normative” approach to person- lity disorders has utilized the five-factor model of personality as a theoret- cal framework (Widiger & Frances, 2002). In the alternative approach, personality disorders are conceptualized as spectrum variants of mental
t t
a a
a i
Paranoid Personality Disorder 53

illness, with Axis I disorders forming the extreme ends of the continuum (Siever & Davis, 1991).
When viewed from the perspective of the five-factor model of personality, there is consistent empirical evidence that PPD is negatively related to the personality dimension of agreeableness and positively related to the dimen- sion of neuroticism (Costa & McCrae, 1990; Saulsman & Page, 2004; Trull, 1992; Wiggins & Pincus, 1989). These associations are moderate in magnitude. In addition, there is less consistent evidence that PPD shows weak, negative relationships to the personality dimensions of extraversion and conscientious- ness (Saulsman & Page, 2004). Thus there is some empirical support for the notion that PPD lies on a continuum with normal personality. However, the hypothesized inverse relationship between PPD and extraversion—that is, the excessive need for autonomy often described in the clinical and theoret- ical literature on PPD—has received somewhat weaker empirical support. Evidence bearing on the question of whether PPD lies on a continuum with more extreme paranoid conditions—namely, schizophrenia and delusional disorder—is discussed below.
Is PPD a Schizophrenia-Spectrum or Delusional-Spectrum Disorder?
Family/Genetics Studies. It has long been noted that characteristics such as suspiciousness, referential thinking, and peculiar ideas are prevalent among relatives of schizophrenics (Bleuler, 1922; Kretschmer, 1925; Ray, 1863/1968). The famous Danish Adoption Study of the 1960s (Kety et al., 1968) was the first methodologically sophisticated attempt to study the prevalence of subsyndromal schizophrenialike traits in the adopted bio- logical offspring of schizophrenic probands. The findings of this study supported the existence of a “schizophrenia spectrum”—a continuum of schizophrenialike characteristics in the nonpsychotic relatives of schizo- phrenic individuals. The description of these nonpsychotic but peculiar rel- atives of schizophrenics became the basis for the DSM-III/DSM-IV criteria for schizotypal personality disorder and, to a lesser degree, the other Cluster A personality disorders, PPD and schizoid personality disorder (Spitzer, Endicott, & Gibbon, 1979). Considerable subsequent evidence has been found to support the notion that schizotypal personality disorder falls on the schizophrenia spectrum (Siever & Davis, 2004). However, the evidence for PPD and schizoid personality disorder is less conclusive (Asarnow et al., 2001, Nicolson et al., 2003).
Another possibility is that PPD is a delusional spectrum disorder, on a genetic continuum with delusional disorder. The differential diagnosis between PPD and delusional disorder is based on the presence of frank delusions in the latter. However, in practice, this distinction is sometimes difficult to make (see “Differential Diagnosis” above). In fact, individuals
  
54
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

with delusional disorder often have features that are quite similar to those of PPD (e.g., pathological jealousy). Moreover, when under stress, people with PPD can sometimes develop transient delusions (Miller et al., 2001). These considerations suggest the possibility that PPD and delusional disorder share a common genetic diathesis that is different from the genetic diathesis of schizophrenia.
Two recent well-designed studies have addressed the question of whether PPD and other putative schizophrenia spectrum disorders are overrepre- sented among the relatives of schizophrenic probands. Asarnow and his col- leagues (2001) found only slightly and nonsignificantly elevated morbid risks of PPD in the relatives of probands with childhood-onset schizophre- nia compared to ADHD and community control groups. In contrast, Nicolson and his colleagues (2003) found large and statistically significant morbid risks of PPD in both child- and adult-onset schizophrenic probands compared to community controls. Both studies supported the hypothesized genetic linkages between schizotypal PD and schizophrenia but found no evidence of a genetic relationship between schizoid PD and schizophrenia. Interestingly, Asarnow and his colleagues (2001) also found elevated rates of avoidant personality disorder in the relatives of probands with child- hood-onset schizophrenia compared to community controls. Thus the find- ings of the above studies offer contradictory support for the notion that PPD is a schizophrenia spectrum disorder.
The hypothesis that PPD is a delusional spectrum disorder has also received some empirical support (Baron et al., 1985; Erlenmeyer-Kimling et al., 1995; Kendler, Gruenberg, & Strauss, 1981; Kendler & Hays, 1981; Kendler, Masterson, et al., 1985; Winokur, 1985). However, many of these studies were hampered by small samples of probands with delusional dis- order, and no recent replication of these findings using larger samples has been attempted. Thus the idea that PPD shares genetic linkages with delu- sional disorder remains an intriguing possibility.
However, a major shortcoming of all of these studies has been the uncer- tain diagnostic validity of PPD. Because the DSM-IV criteria may not fully represent the PPD phenotype, the ability to determine whether genetic link- ages for PPD exist may have been hampered.
Which Environmental Factors
Might Contribute to the Development of PPD?
There is relatively little research on environmental factors in the etiology of PPD. However, evidence from one longitudinal study suggests that trau- matic childhood events, such as childhood abuse and neglect, may play a role in the development of PPD (Johnson, Cohen, Brown, Smailes, & Bernstein, 1999; Johnson, Smailes, Cohen, Brown, & Bernstein, 2000). Johnson and colleagues (Johnson et al., 1999, 2000) found that children
  
Paranoid Personality Disorder 55

with substantiated histories of child abuse or neglect were at significantly greater risk for Cluster A personality disorders, including PPD, in young adulthood, even when other risk factors were taken into account. There is a considerable body of literature suggesting that childhood physical abuse is associated with anger and aggression in children and adolescents (Kolko, 2002)—features that are similar to those seen in PPD. Childhood physical abuse may therefore prove to play a specific etiologic role in the develop- ment of PPD. However, this hypothesis remains to be tested empirically.
In general, research suggests that both genetic and environmental factors play significant roles in the development of the traits that make up personality disorders (Livesley, Jang, & Vernon, 1998). There is little reason to suspect that PPD would be an exception. For example, an analysis of twin study data revealed that the types of traits that appear to characterize PPD, such as sus- piciousness, hostility, oppositionality, and restricted expression of affect, have both strong heritable and environmental components (Livesley et al., 1998).
What Is the Most Accurate Means of Assessing PPD?
Accurate and efficient assessment of PPD is essential in treatment. Without an initial understanding of the client’s paranoid perception of the world, a clinician who approaches a PPD client in a manner that may threaten her excessive need for autonomy and privacy may never see her after the first ses- sion. The therapeutic environment itself, with its emphasis on trust and dis- closure, may be overwhelming to the PPD client. Furthermore, it would not be typical of a PPD client to present with complaints of paranoia (Turkat, Keane, & Thompson-Pope, 1990). Some (or perhaps many) PPD individu- als may be falling through the cracks of the mental health system because they do not readily disclose problems with “paranoia” and may initially fail to be identified as possessing significant PPD traits. In addition, PPD indi- viduals who are not delusional may draw less clinical attention than para- noid psychotic individuals who require more frequent and longer hospitalizations, antipsychotic medication, and more consistent follow-up. Furthermore, PPD individuals often drop out of therapy early or do not fol- low through with treatment plans (Turkat, 1985; Oldham & Skodol, 1994). Therefore, accurate and efficient assessment of significant and relevant PPD features is essential in diagnosis and selection of a treatment modality that takes into account the great difficulty a PPD client may have with disclosure as well as with the therapeutic relationship.
The Paranoid Personality Disorder Features Questionnaire (PPDFQ).
The PPDFQ (Useda, 2002) is a dimensional measure that assesses the six main traits associated with PPD as described by the clinical and theoretical literature on PPD (Cameron, 1943, 1963; Kraepelin, 1921; Kretschmer, 1925; Millon, 1969, 1981; Shapiro, 1965; Sheldon, 1940; Sheldon & Stevens, 1942; Turkat, 1985) as well as the DSM-IV’s (APA, 1994)
  
56
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Associated Features section on PPD: Mistrust/suspiciousness, antagonism, introversion, hypersensitivity, hypervigilance, and rigidity. Thus the PPDFQ appears to assess the fundamental traits underlying PPD in a more balanced and proportionate manner than do the DSM-IV’s own PPD diagnostic crite- ria, which, as has already been noted, overemphasize the cognitive features of PPD. The PPDFQ provides additional diagnostic information (i.e., the presence and degree of impairment associated with specific maladaptive variants of the core personality traits of PPD) compared to other self-report questionnaires of PPD currently in use (e.g., MMPI PPD profiles [Merritt, Balogh, & Kok, 1998]).
PPDFQ items for each of these six traits were originally written to represent three modes of expression (i.e., cognitive, affective, and interpersonal/ behavioral) of an underlying trait. Many items focused on the participant’s assessment of his or her interpersonal relationships and interpersonal situa- tions as well as on his or her interpretations of others’ behaviors. For example, the item “I think most other people are hostile” was developed to represent the cognitive mode of antagonism. The item “I make an effort to pick up on every detail of another person’s behavior” was written to reflect the behavioral mode of hypervigilance. The item response format was a five-point Likert scale assessing level of agreement (0 = strongly disagree; 1 = disagree; 2 = neutral, neither disagree nor agree; 3 = agree; 4 = strongly agree). The measure was designed to assess functioning over the previous 2 years.
Initial findings on the reliability and validity of the PPDFQ in a norma- tive college student sample (N = 106) are encouraging (Useda, 2002). The test-retest reliability of the six PPDFQ scores over a 6-week interval was good, and the hypothesized relationships between the PPDFQ subscales and the five-factor model of personality as well as Livesley’s dimensional model of personality pathology were supported. Further validation of the PPDFQ in clinical samples is clearly indicated. It is hoped that the PPDFQ will prove a useful tool that will improve the validity of dimensional assessments of PPD in future studies.
What Kinds of Treatments Are Likely
to Be Most Effective for People With PPD?
The treatment literature on PPD is limited to single-case studies. We could locate no report in the literature of a clinical trial of any treatment for PPD— psychotherapeutic or psychopharmacological. Perhaps one reason for this is that PPD patients are perceived by some clinicians to be untreatable. Clearly, their mistrust, antagonism, introversion, rigidity, and other features present challenges for psychotherapists, given that therapy is usually predicated on one’s ability to form a trusting relationship with the therapist and to exam- ine one’s own assumptions about oneself, others, and the world.
A thorough discussion of possible treatment approaches to use with PPD patients is beyond the scope of this chapter. However, a few general guidelines
  
Paranoid Personality Disorder 57

can be suggested. First, the goal of therapy with PPD patients is to help them recognize and accept their own feelings of vulnerability; heighten their feelings of self-worth and reduce their feelings of shame; help them develop a more bal- anced, trusting view of others; and reduce their reliance on counterproductive self-protective strategies, such as bullying, threatening, and intimidating others and keeping others at a distance. A variety of therapeutic approaches could be employed to accomplish these goals (e.g., cognitive-behavioral, psychody- namic). However, regardless of theoretical orientation, it is essential that the patient’s mistrust and self-protective mechanisms be confronted directly in an empathic but clear and straightforward manner.
It is an old adage that you cannot “talk a paranoid person out of his para- noia.” However, many individuals with PPD have some capacity to take per- spective on their own suspicious cognitions. An approach of “collaborative empiricism” can be very helpful in this regard, in which the therapist invites the patient to join in a process of examining his or her beliefs in the light of objective evidence. Thought records can be used to help the patient identify and modify his or her maladaptive cognitions by weighing the evidence sup- porting them and contradicting them. In conducting this sort of inquiry, it is important to acknowledge that the patient’s suspicions about others often contain a kernel of truth.
For example, one of us (D. P. B.) treated a PPD patient for 3 years using schema therapy (Bernstein, 2005; Young et al., 2003), an integrative form of psychotherapy that combines cognitive, behavioral, psychodynamic object relations, and existential/humanistic approaches. The patient had largely unfounded fears that his coworkers didn’t respect him and that his boss was looking for an excuse to fire him. While there was little objective evidence to support his belief that others disrespected him or that his own job was in jeopardy, his workplace environment did appear to be a ruthless one in which senior staff deliberately fostered competition among coworkers, and many of his colleagues worried about their job security. The patient appeared to be relieved that his therapist validated the realistic aspects of his perceptions, rather than treating his beliefs as “crazy.” Moreover, recognizing that many of his coworkers might also be feeling insecure about their jobs helped the patient to accept his own feelings of vulnerability. The patient was then able to engage in a process of collaborative empiricism with his therapist, in which they weighed the evidence supporting and contradicting his beliefs. The patient’s “evidence” that others disrespected him was based mainly on ambiguous social interactions in which colleagues had appeared unfriendly or hadn’t solicited his opinion during meetings. After examining the evidence critically, the patient was able to recognize that his colleagues’ behavior could be open to a variety of alternative explanations. Moreover, the patient came to see that his own self-protective tendency to keep others at a distance was probably responsible for some of the unfriendliness he was experienc- ing. Similarly, his own tendency to keep quiet during meetings for fear of appearing stupid was probably responsible for the fact that others didn’t solicit his opinions.
  
58
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

As a behavioral intervention, the therapist recommended that the patient take some of his colleagues out to lunch. After doing so, the patient noticed that his colleagues were more friendly and relaxed around him, contradict- ing his view that they didn’t like him. Shortly thereafter, he received a glow- ing evaluation from his supervisor. Rather than being relieved, the patient reacted to this good news with mistrust: he couldn’t believe that his super- visor could actually hold him in such high regard, and insisted that the supervisor must be secretly criticizing him behind his back! After some dis- cussion, the patient was able to recognize that his reaction was based on a core defectiveness schema—the belief that he was irrevocably flawed, unlik- able and unlovable. It was this core belief that was responsible for his fre- quent perception that others disrespected him. It was also responsible for his conviction that his wife was planning to leave him, despite the abundant evi- dence that she cared deeply for him and was satisfied with their relationship. Thus, the empathic but persistent confrontation of the patient’s mistrustful beliefs and self-defeating coping mechanisms led over time to greater feelings of self-worth and self-acceptance, less mistrustful attitudes, greater ability to “reality test” his own suspicious beliefs, and more effective ways of relating to others.
Summary___________________________________________
PPD is a severe personality disorder that has received far less empirical atten- tion than it deserves, given its prevalence in clinical populations and its neg- ative consequences, such as disruptive behavior and interpersonal distress, unnecessary litigation, psychiatric comorbidity, and violence. In this chapter, we have recommended that the DSM-IV criteria for PPD be substantially revised to increase their validity and have discussed new directions for PPD research and treatment. We hope that this chapter will stimulate researchers and clinicians to pursue these new avenues with the goal of improving understanding and treatment of this difficult disorder.
References _________________________________________
American Psychiatric Association. (1952). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of men- tal disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Asarnow, R., Nuechterlein, K., Fogelson, D., Subotnik, K., Payne, D., Russell, A., et al. (2001). Schizophrenia and schizophrenia-spectrum personality disorders in the first-degree relatives of children with schizophrenia: The UCLA Family Study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 581–588.
  
Paranoid Personality Disorder 59

Baron, M., Gruen, R., Rainer, J. D., Kane, J., Asnis, L., & Lord, S. (1985). A family study of schizophrenic and normal control probands: Implications for the spec- trum concept of schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 147–454.
Beck, A. T., Freeman, A. T., & Associates (1990). Cognitive therapy of personality disorders. New York: Guilford Press.
Bernstein, D. P. (2005). Schema therapy for personality disorders. In S. Strack (Ed.), Handbook of personology and psychopathology (pp. 462–477). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Bernstein, D. P., Cohen, P., Velez, C. N., Schwab-Stone, M., Siever, L., & Shinsato, L. (1993). Prevalence and stability of the DSM-III-R personality disorders in a community-based survey of adolescents. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1237–1243.
Bernstein, D. P., Useda, J. D., & Siever, L. J. (1993). Paranoid personality disorder: Review of the literature and recommendations for DSM-IV. Journal of Personality Disorders, 7, 53–62.
Bernstein, D. P., Useda, J. D., & Siever, L. J. (1995). Paranoid personality disorder. In W. J. Livesley (Ed.), The DSM-IV personality disorders (pp. 45–57). New York: Guilford Press.
Bleuler, E. (1922). Die Probleme der Schizoidie und der Syntonie [The problem of schizoidness and syntony]. Zeitschrift fur die Gesamte Neurolagie und Psychiatrie, 78, 373–388.
Bögels, S. M., & Mansell, W. (2004). Attention processes in the maintenance and treatment of social phobia: Hypervigilance, avoidance, and self-focused atten- tion. Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 827–856.
Bond, M., & Perry, C. (2004). Long-term changes in defense styles with psychody- namic psychotherapy for depressive, anxiety, and personality disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 1665–1671.
Cameron, N. (1943). The paranoid pseudo-community. American Journal of Sociology, 49, 32–38.
Cameron, N. (1963). Personality development and psychopathology. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Cecero, J., Nelson, J., & Gillie, J. (2004). Tools and tenets of schema therapy: Toward the construct validity of the Early Maladaptive Schema Questionnaire—Research Version (EMSQ-R). Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 11, 344–357.
Coryell, W. H., & Zimmerman, M. (1989). Personality disorder in the families of depressed, schizophrenic, and never-ill probands. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 496–502.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1990). Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality. Journal of Personality Disorders, 4, 362–371.
Drake, R. E., & Vaillant, G. E. (1985). A validity study of Axis II of DSM-III. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 553–558.
Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L., Squires-Wheeler, E., Adamo, U. H., Basset, A. S., Cornblatt, B. A., Kestenbaum, C. J., et al. (1995). The New York High-Risk Project: Psychoses and Cluster A personality disorders in offspring of schizophrenic parents at 23 years of follow-up. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52, 857–865.
Giesen-Bloo, J., Arntz, A., van Dyck, R., Spinhoven, P. & van Tilburg, W. (2004, November). Schema focused vs. transference focused psychotherapy for border- line personality disorder: Results of a multicenter trial. Paper presented at the 2nd European Congress for Psychotherapy, Amsterdam, Netherlands.
60
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Grilo, C., Sanislow, C., Gunderson, J., Pagano, M., Yen, S., Zanarini, M., et al. (2004). Two-year stability and change of schizotypal, borderline, avoidant, and obsessive-compulsive personality disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 767–775.
Holmes, D. (1978). Projection as a defense mechanism. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 677–688.
Johnson, J., Cohen, P., Brown, J., Smailes, E., & Bernstein, D. (1999). Childhood maltreatment increases risk for personality disorders during early adulthood. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 600–606.
Johnson, J., Smailes, E., Cohen, P., Brown, J., & Bernstein, D. (2000). Associations between four types of childhood neglect and personality disorder symptoms dur- ing adolescence and early adulthood: Findings of a community-based longitudi- nal study. Journal of Personality Disorders, 14, 171–187.
Kendler, K. S., & Gruenberg, A. M. (1982). Genetic relationship between paranoid personality disorder and the “schizophrenic spectrum” disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 1185–1186.
Kendler, K., Gruenberg, A., & Strauss, J. (1982). An independent analysis of the Copenhagen sample of the Danish adoption study of schizophrenia: V. The rela- tionship between childhood social withdrawal and adult schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 39, 1257–1261.
Kendler, K., & Hays, P. (1981). Paranoid psychosis (delusional disorder) and schiz- ophrenia: A family history study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 547–551. Kendler, K. S., Masterson, C. C., & Davis, K. (1985). Psychiatric illness in first- degree relatives of patients with paranoid psychosis, schizophrenia and medical
illness. British Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 524–531.
Kety, S. S., Rosenthal, D., Wender, P. H., & Schulsinger, F. (1968). The types and
prevalence of mental illness in the biological and adoptive families of adopted schizophrenics. In D. Rosenthal & S. S. Kety (Eds.), The transmission of schiz- ophrenia (pp. 345–362). Oxford, UK: Pergamon Press.
Koenigsberg, H., Harvey, P., Mitropoulou, V., New, A., Goodman, M., Silverman, J., et al. (2001). Are the interpersonal and identity disturbances in the borderline personality disorder criteria linked to the traits of affectivity and impulsivity? Journal of Personality Disorders, 15, 358–370.
Kolko, D. (2002). Child physical abuse. In L. Berliner & J. Myers (Eds.), The APSAC handbook on child maltreatment (2nd ed., pp. 21–54). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Kraepelin, E. (1921). Manic-depressive insanity and paranoia. Edinburgh, UK:
Livingstone.
Kretschmer, E. (1925). Physique and character. London: Kegan Paul.
Leung, N., Thomas, G., & Waller, G. (2000). The relationship between parental bond-
ing and core beliefs in anorexic and bulimic women. British Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 39, 205–213.
Lingiardi, V., Lonati, C., Delucchi, F., Fossati, A., Vanzulli, L., & Maffei, C. (1999).
Defense mechanisms and personality disorders. Journal of Nervous and Mental
Disease, 187, 224–228.
Livesley, W. J., Jang, K. L., & Vernon, P. A. (1998). Phenotypic and genetic structure of
traits delineating personality disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 941–948. Maier, W., Lichterman, D., Minges, J., & Heun, R. (1994). Personality disorders among the relatives of schizophrenic patients. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 20,
481–493.
Paranoid Personality Disorder 61

Merritt, R., Balogh, D., & Kok, C. (1998). DSM-IV Cluster A personality disorder diagnoses among young adults with a 2-7-8 MMPI profile. Assessment, 5, 273–285.
Miller, M. B., Useda, J. D., Trull, T. J., Burr, R. M., & Minks-Brown, C. (2001). Paranoid, schizoid and schizotypal personality disorders. In H. E. Adams & P. B. Sutker (Eds.), The comprehensive handbook of psychopathology (3rd ed., pp. 535–559). New York: Plenum.
Millon, T. (1969). Modern psychopathology: A biosocial approach to maladaptive learning and functioning. Philadelphia: Saunders.
Millon, T. (1981). Disorders of personality: DSM-III Axis II. New York: Wiley. Millon, T., & Davis, R. (1996). Disorders of personality: DSM-IV and beyond. New
York: Wiley.
Morey, L. C. (1988). Personality disorders in DSM-III and DSM-III-R: Convergence,
coverage, and internal consistency. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145,
573–577.
Nicolson, R., Brookner, F., Lenane, M., Gochman, P., Ingraham, L., Egan, M., et al.
(2003). Parental schizophrenia spectrum disorders in childhood-onset and adult-
onset schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 490–495.
Oldham, J. M., & Skodol, A. E. (1994). Do patients with paranoid personality dis- order seek psychoanalysis? In J. M. Oldham & S. Bone (Eds.), Paranoid: New psychoanalytic perspectives (pp. 151–166). Madison, CT: International
Universities Press.
Paris, J., Zweig-Frank, H., Bond, M., & Guzder, J. (1996). Defense styles, hostility,
and psychological risk factors in male patients with personality disorders.
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 184, 153–158.
Ray, I. (1863/1968). Mental hygiene. New York: Hafner.
Saulsman, L., & Page, A. (2004). The five-factor model and personality disorder
empirical literature: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 23,
1055–1085.
Schmidt, N., Joiner, T., Young, J., & Telch, M. (1995). The Schema Questionnaire:
Investigation of psychometric properties and the hierarchical structure of a mea-
sure of maladaptive schemas. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19, 295–321. Shapiro, D. (1965). Neurotic styles. New York: Basic Books.
Sheldon, W. H. (1940). The varieties of human physique: An introduction to consti-
tutional psychology. New York: Harper.
Sheldon, W. H., & Stevens, S. S. (1942). The varieties of temperament: A psychol-
ogy of individual differences. New York: Harper.
Siever, L., & Davis, K. (1991). A psychobiological perspective on the personality dis-
orders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 1647–1658.
Siever, L., & Davis, K. (2004). The pathophysiology of schizophrenia disorders:
Perspectives from the spectrum. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 398–413. Spitzer, R. L., Endicott, J., & Gibbon, M. (1979). Crossing the border into border- line personality and borderline schizophrenia: The development of criteria.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 36, 17–24.
Trull, T. J. (1992). DSM-II-R personality disorders and the five-factor model of
personality: An empirical comparison. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101,
553–560.
Turkat, I. D. (1985). Paranoid personality disorder. In I. D. Turkat (Ed.), Behavioral
  
case formulation (pp. 155–198). New York: Plenum.
62
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Turkat, I. D., Keane, S. P., & Thompson-Pope, S. K. (1990). Social processing errors among paranoid personalities. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 12, 263–269.
Useda, J. D. (2002, April). The construct validity of the Paranoid Personality Disorder Features Questionnaire (PPDFQ): A dimensional assessment of para- noid personality disorder. Dissertation Abstracts International, 62, 9B. (UMI No. 4240)
Vaillant, G. E. (1994). Ego mechanisms of defense and personality psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 44–50.
Waller, G., Meyer, C., & Ohanian, V. (2001). Psychometric properties of the long and short version of the Young Schema Questionnaire: Core beliefs among bulimic and comparison women. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25, 137–147.
Waller, G., Meyer, C., Ohanian, V., Elliott, P., Dickson, C., & Sellings, J. (2001). The psychopathology of bulimic women who report childhood sexual abuse: The mediating role of core beliefs. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 189, 700–708.
Warner, M., Morey, L., Finch, J., Gunderson, J., Skodol, A., Sanislow, C., et al. (2004). The longitudinal relationship of personality traits and disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 217–227.
Widiger, T. A., & Frances, A. (2002). Toward a dimensional model for the person- ality disorders. In P. T. Costa, Jr., & T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality (2nd ed., pp. 23–44). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Widiger, T. A., & Rogers, J. H. (1989) Prevalence and comorbidity of personality disorders. Psychiatric Annals, 19, 132–136.
Widiger, T. A., & Trull, T. J. (1998). Performance characteristics of the DSM-III-R personality disorder criteria sets. In T. A. Widiger, A. J. Frances, H. A. Pincus, R. Ross, M. B. First, W. W. Davis, et al. (Eds.), DSM-IV sourcebook (Vol. 4, pp. 357–373). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Widiger, T. A., Trull, T. J., Clarkin, J. F., Sanderson, C., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (2002). A description of the DSM-IV personality disorders with the five-factor model of personality. In P. T. Costa, Jr., & T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality (2nd ed., pp. 89–99). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Wiggins, J. S., & Pincus, A. L. (1989). Conceptions of personality disorders and dimensions of personality. Psychological Assessment, 1, 305–316.
Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1997). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Winokur, G. (1985). Familial psychopathology in delusional disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 26, 241–248.
Young, J., Klosko, J., & Weishaar, M. (2003). Schema therapy: A practitioner’s guide. New York: Guilford Press.
Zimmerman, M. (1994). Diagnosing personality disorders: A review of issues and research methods. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 225–245.

Schizoid 4 Personality Disorder
Vijay A. Mittal Emory University
Oren Kalus Ulster County Mental Health
David P. Bernstein Maastricht University
Larry J. Siever Mount Sinai School of Medicine
________________________________________ Introduction
Schizoid personality disorder (SCD) is one of the DSM’s (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2000) three “odd/eccentric” cluster personal- ity disorders (along with schizotypal and paranoid personality disorders), which are characterized by phenomenological similarities to schizophrenia. SCD is distinguished from the other two personality disorders in this cluster by the prominence of social, interpersonal, and affective deficits (i.e., nega- tive symptoms) in the absence of psychotic-like cognitive/perceptual distor- tions (i.e., positive symptoms).
Despite a rich and extensive clinical tradition regarding the schizoid char- acter, its pre-DSM-III (APA, 1980) status was handicapped by considerable
 
63
64
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

heterogeneity and lack of clearly operationalized diagnostic criteria. The architects of DSM-III attempted to subdivide and sharpen the boundaries of this heterogeneous diagnosis by adding schizotypal and paranoid personal- ity disorders to the “odd” cluster and moving avoidant personality disorder to the “anxious” cluster. The narrowing of the schizoid personality disorder diagnosis that resulted from these changes raised further questions, however, about its diagnostic boundaries and about whether the diagnosis is a valid separate entity. Evidence of extensive criterion overlap as well as comorbid- ity with other personality disorders (particularly schizotypal and avoidant) has been of particular concern in this regard. The low prevalence rates of DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000) schizoid personality disorder have further com- plicated attempts to address these issues empirically. The scarcity of data on DSM-IV-TR schizoid personality disorder has remained a significantly lim- iting factor in resolving these concerns and in considering the status of SCD as we approach DSM-V.
Historical Background
Bleuler (1924) used the term “schizoid” to describe a tendency to turn inward and away from the external world, the absence of emotional expres- siveness, simultaneous contradictory dullness and sensitivity, and pursuit of vague interests. From the 1950s until the mid 1970s, the term was used to describe schizophrenia-like spectrum disorders (Miller, Useda, Trull, Burr, & Minks-Brown, 2001) and encompassed the conceptions that are now delineated into separate Cluster A personality disorders (Wolff, 1998).
Although most historical clinical descriptions of schizoid personality disorder are consistent with DSM-IV-TR criteria, there appear to be some discrepancies. In addition to describing the familiar negative symptoms of schizoid personality disorder outlined in the DSM, many clinicians described the presence of contradictory affective and cognitive states in schizoid personality disorder that were not recognized in DSM-III (some of these features may have been absorbed into other personality disor- ders, such as schizotypal and avoidant). Kretschmer (1925), for example, differentiated two types of schizoid characteristics—the hyperaesthetic and the anaesthetic—that contrasted inner sensitivity with overt insensi- tivity. Rather than separating these contrasting behavioral tendencies into two distinct diagnostic groups, as DSM-III did with the schizoid and avoidant categories, Kretschmer suggested that these characteristics may coexist in the same person. Several clinicians have suggested that the schizoid individual’s apparent outward insensitivity and indifference often belie marked inner sensitivity. This association highlights a problem with one of the DSM-III-R’s (APA, 1987) schizoid personality disorder criteria, which is based on an inferred inner state–subjective indifference
  
Schizoid Personality Disorder 65
 
to rejection or criticism. In an effort to eliminate this ambiguity, this criterion was revised in DSM-IV to emphasize a more objective behav- ioral description and to increase specificity for the context in which the relative lack of emotional expression takes place (APA, 1994).
Another area in which the traditional literature differs from DSM-IV- TR concerns the schizoid’s sexuality. Observations by Terry and Rennie (1938) of compulsive masturbation in SCD individuals are consistent with the DSM criterion of absent sexual relationships but not with the absence of sexual desire. Other clinical features either not reported or deemphasized in DSM-IV-TR include autistic thinking, fragmented self- identity, and symptoms of derealization/depersonalization. Numerous clinicians (particularly psychodynamically oriented ones) have stressed the fragmented personality structure and the use of such primitive defen- sive mechanisms as splitting. Guntrip (1969) and other clinicians reported the frequent presence of depersonalization, derealization, absence of feel- ing, and disembodiment in SCD individuals. The psychoanalytic literature also makes extensive references to the “primitive character structure” of the schizoid and in particular to an identity disturbance that may contrast with the more dramatic and affectively charged identity disturbance reported in borderline personality disorder patients. Bleuler (1954) and other clinicians also emphasized the phenomenological similarities between schizoid personality and schizophrenia, which anticipated cur- rent questions concerning the relationship of all three Cluster A diagnoses to schizophrenia.
The Diagnosis of Schizoid __________________________________________ Personality Disorder
Daryl is a 28-year-old male who lives in an apartment above his parents’ garage. Because he tends to avoid interacting with his family, his parents felt that he would feel happy about the move to his own space. He appeared indifferent to the change. Daryl works as a computer programmer in a small firm and is in dan- ger of losing his job. His supervisor is becoming frustrated because Daryl seems indifferent to feedback or criticism. His coworkers describe him as a “loner” and report being disconcerted by his apparent lack of emotion. Daryl’s mother com- plains that he never smiles or frowns at anything. When she tries to include him in family activities, he appears cold and detached. Daryl has little interest in mak- ing friends and has never been in a romantic relationship. He has never been excited by the prospect of sexual intercourse. Although he spends most of his free time building models of airplanes, he does not overtly enjoy this activity. When complimented about his airplanes, Daryl appears not to notice or to care.
66 PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Prevalence _________________________________________
It has been noted that schizoid personality is among the least frequently observed of the personality disorders (Miller et al., 2001). This low prevalence has likely contributed to the dearth of information surrounding the demo- graphic characteristics of the disorder. DSM-IV-TR notes that SCD may be first apparent in late childhood. Because this is a period in which cooperative play is in ascendance, the social isolation associated with SCD becomes more salient. In addition, the disorder is also more common in men than in women (APA, 2000). See Table 4.1 for the DSM-IV-TR criteria for SCD.
The DSM-IV-TR does not provide data regarding SCD prevalence. This omission may be due to the uncommon appearance of schizoid individuals in clinical settings, or to the fact that current conceptions of SCD identify only the most severe cases of the disorder (Wolff, 1998). Considerable vari- ation in the prevalence rates is apparent across clinical settings. Estimates of the prevalence of SCD in the general population based on community sur- veys (Reich, Yates, & Nduaguba, 1989), nonpsychiatric controls (Drake & Vaillant, 1985), and relatives of psychiatric patients (Zimmerman & Coryell, 1990) have ranged from 0.5% to 7%.
Prevalence rates vary considerably depending on the DSM version. Studies using DSM-III-R criteria generally report higher prevalence rates than those using DSM-III criteria. For example, Morey and Heumann
Table 4.1 DSM-IV-TR criteria for diagnosing schizoid personality disorder
Criteria
A.
(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7)
B.
Description
A pervasive pattern of detachment from social relationships and a restricted range of expression of emotions in interpersonal settings, beginning by early adulthood and present in a variety of contexts, as indicated by four (or more) of the following.
Neither desires nor enjoys close relationships, including being part of a family Almost always chooses solitary activities
Has little, if any, interest in having sexual experiences with another person Takes pleasure in few, if any, activities
Lacks close friends or confidants other than first-degree relatives Appears indifferent to the praise or criticism of others
Behavior or appearance that is odd, eccentric, or peculiar
Does not occur exclusively during the course of Schizophrenia, a Mood Disorder With Psychotic Features, another Psychotic Disorder, or a Pervasive Developmental Disorder and is not due to the direct physiological effects of a general medical condition.

SOURCE: Reprinted with permission from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision, (Copyright 2000). American Psychiatric Association.
Note. If criteria are met prior to the onset of Schizophrenia, add “Premorbid,” e.g., “Schizoid Personality Disorder (Premorbid).”
Schizoid Personality Disorder 67

(1988) compared DSM-III with DSM-III-R SCD diagnoses in the same group of 291 personality-disordered patients, reporting a substantially higher preva- lence using DSM-III-R criteria (1.4% versus 11.0%). These differences reflect changes incorporated into DSM-III-R that provided a richer and potentially more sensitive description and attempted to reduce the risk of oversimplification (Akhtar, 1987). For example, the use of a polythetic system that does not require any single feature added further flexibility and may have increased the sensitivity of the diagnosis.
Based on DSM-IV criteria, results from an epidemiological catchment area study found prevalence rates between 0.7% and 0.9% (Samuels et al., 2002). Prevalence rates are also dependent on classification systems. For example, a Swedish community sample study utilizing the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10; World Health Organization, 1992) system found SCD prevalence to be as high as 4.5% (Ekselius, Tillfors, Furmark, & Fredrikson, 2001).
______________________________ Psychometric Properties
The internal consistency of measures of schizoid personality disorder is poor; a recent study using the Diagnostic Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders (DIPD-IV) reported a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of .47 and mean intercrite- rion correlation of .11 (Grilo et al., 2001). By a large margin (histrionic PD being the next lowest, alpha = .64), this is the poorest internal consistency of any personality disorder (Grilo et al., 2001). Other studies have reported slightly higher levels of internal consistency, with alpha ranging between .63 (Farmer & Chapman, 2002) and .68 (Ottosson, Ekselius, Grann, & Kullgren, 2002). Nevertheless, schizoid personality disorder had the lowest consistency of any of the personality disorders in each of these studies.
Because of the low base rate of SCD, studies aiming to determine sensitivity, specificity, and predictive power of individual criteria have been marked by an insufficient sample size for conducting appropriate analyses (Farmer & Chapman, 2002). Some past research has examined criterion performance for the DSM- III-R conceptualization of SCD. (Due to criterion changes in the DSM-IV-TR, these data will be reported only for those items which have remained constant throughout subsequent revisions.) The results of three studies examining the performance of the DSM-III-R criteria (Millon & Tringone, 1989 [N = 26]; Morey & Heumann, 1988 [N = 32]; Freiman & Widiger, 1989 [N = 8]) were divergent, although some trends were apparent. Of the criteria reflecting impaired capacity for interpersonal relationships, only “neither desires nor enjoys close relationships . . .” demonstrated high sensitivity (.62–.87) and speci- ficity (.86–.93) and was considered prototypical (78/100) by clinicians. The criterion “almost always chooses solitary activities” showed high sensitivity
  
68
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

(.73–.88) and was judged prototypical (76/100) but had moderate specificity (.78–.88). The criterion “lacks close friends or confidants . . .” also demon- strated high sensitivity (.69–.72), but it had the lowest specificity (.55–.68) of all the schizoid criteria. The low specificity of criteria indicates that these features are shared with other personality disorders.
In contrast to the criteria referring to interpersonal relationships, the criteria “has little, if any, desire to have sexual experiences . . .” and “appears indifferent to the praise and criticism of others” demonstrated low sensitivity (.62–.75; .00–.34), mid to low prototypicality (71/100; 55/100), but high specificity (.78–1; .93–.95). These criteria may define a subgroup of patients dominated by deficits in both affective responsivity and capacity for pleasure. It was concluded that retaining these criteria in subsequent versions of the DSM, despite their low sensitivity, might be justified by their possible ability to identify a subset of atypical cases. There are currently no data concerning the DSM-IV-TR criteria “takes pleasure in few, if any, activities” and “behavior or appearance that is odd, eccentric, or peculiar.”
Compatibility of DSM and ICD-10 Criteria _____________
The revisions to SCD introduced in DSM-III-R and further modified in DSM-IV have not produced satisfactory levels of agreement between DSM and ICD-10. A study examining prevalence rates in a Swedish community sample and comparing these rates between ICD-10 and DSM-IV found that differences were most striking for the classification of SCD (ICD-10 = 4.5%; DSM-IV = 0.9%; Ekselius et al., 2001). The kappa was .32, which is a low level of agreement in view of the fact that the next lowest value was .50 for antisocial/dyssocial personality (Ekselius et al., 2001). This conclusion is fur- ther supported by a study examining the concordance of personality disor- ders between DSM-IV and ICD-10, which found schizoid to exhibit the poorest agreement between systems (kappa = .37; Ottosson et al., 2002). This discordance is attributable to arbitrary thresholds (Ottosson et al., 2002) and additional ICD-10 criteria that do not have corresponding DSM- IV items (e.g. ., “marked difficulty in recognizing and adhering to social con- vention, resulting in eccentricity of behavior” [World Health Organization, 1992, p. F60.1]).
Comorbidity ________________________________________
Rates of comorbidity of SCD with other personality disorders are listed in Table 4.2. The highest co-occurrence is with schizotypal personality disor- der, perhaps because of the high overlap between the two criteria sets
   
Schizoid Personality Disorder 69

Table 4.2 Comorbidity of schizoid personality disorder with other Axis II disorders
PRN SZT ATS BDL HST NAR AVD DPD OCP
Percentage of criterion group receiving schizoid diagnosis
DahI(1986) 0 80 40 20 20 0 60 0 0
Morey(1988) 47 38 3 19 9 28 53 19 16
Freiman& 62 62 25 38 0 38 88 0 0 Widiger (1989)
Skodoletal. 40 60 0 60 0 20 80 20 20 (1988)
Millon& 4 27 0 0 0 8 23 15 8 Tringone (1989)
Farmer& 5 27 0 8 0 0 10 – 3 Chapman (2002)
Note. Key to personality disorder abbreviations: PRN, paranoid; SZT, schizotypal, ATS, antisocial; BDL, borderline; HST, histrionic; NAR, narcissistic; AVD, avoidant; DPD, dependent; OCP, obsessive compulsive. The blank for DPD on the Farmer and Chapman (2002) study is due to an insufficient sample size.
(e.g., social isolation, restricted affect). Avoidant personality disorder also demonstrated high comorbidity with SCD. Lesser degrees of comorbidity were demonstrated with paranoid, antisocial, and borderline personality disorders. Although SCD is sometimes considered a member of the schizo- phrenia spectrum, sharing some overlap with other Cluster A disorders, it has also been evaluated in the context of Asperger’s syndrome (Wolff, 1998). The sections that follow will discuss SCD’s overlap with avoidant personality disorder, its relationship with Asperger’s disorder, and the role of SCD in the schizophrenia spectrum.
____________ Schizoid and Avoidant Personality Disorders
Kretschmer (1925) distinguished between two disorders: anaesthetic (with- drawn due to indifference) and hyperaesthetic (withdrawn due to an over- stimulation of outside influences). This distinction, which parallels the DSM-IV-TR distinction between schizoid and avoidant personality types, has engendered numerous controversies (Miller et al., 2001). Despite their phenotypic similarities, avoidant personality is listed as a Cluster C anxious disorder, whereas schizoid is in Cluster A, the odd and eccentric group.
Some studies suggest that SCD can be distinguished from avoidant per- sonality disorder (Trull, Widiger, & Frances, 1987) on the basis of intimacy

70
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

needs and sensitivity to rejection. However, contrasting historical descrip- tions suggesting that sensitivity and insensitivity coexist in schizoid person- ality disorder and more recent studies suggesting that anxiety and other clinical symptoms occur in both disorders (Overholser, 1989) call for addi- tional investigation.
Although some researchers have argued that the etiology of the social withdrawal symptom presentation is sufficient to draw a line between SCD and avoidant personality disorder (see Chapter 10), the research literature has demonstrated poor discriminant validity between the two disorders. A recent study revealed that of those persons given a diagnosis of SCD, 40% met criteria for avoidant personality disorder (Farmer & Chapman, 2002). Another study examining comorbidity of personality disorders found that schizoid and avoidant personality disorders were correlated at a significant rate (r = .51; Solano & De Chavez, 2000). In addition, person- ality measures have demonstrated difficulty in distinguishing between the two disorders. An analysis of discriminant validity utilizing three measures, the International Personality Disorder Examination (Loranger, Susman, Oldham, & Russakoff, 1987), the Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire (Hyler & Rieder, 1994), and the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory (Millon, Millon, & Davis, 1994), found that avoidant and schizoid person- ality disorders were not clearly distinguishable from each other due to high intercorrelations (Blackburn, Donnelly, Logan, & Renwick, 2004).
Schizoid and Asperger’s Syndrome ____________________
A recent body of literature points to a possible link between SCD and Asperger’s syndrome. There is significant overlap in the phenomenological criteria for both disorders: solitary activity, lack of empathy, emotional detachment, increased sensitivity, paranoid ideation, unusual styles of com- munication, and rigidity of mental set (Wolff, 1998). In a study of parents of autistic children, Wolff (1998) found a heightened level of schizoid per- sonality traits in the parents compared with matched control pairs. Both dis- orders additionally share nonverbal behavior deficits that often interfere with interpersonal relationships.
Despite these similarities, researchers have noted differences between the two disorders. Wolff (1998) asked, “If there is an overlap between Asperger’s and SCD, how is it possible that autism and schizophrenia rarely aggregate in the same families, or occur so rarely in the same person?” (p. 124). The clin- ical presentation of the two disorders is quite different. Asperger’s disorder, or autism, becomes evident between 2 and 3 years of age when imaginative play is in ascendance. In contrast, schizoid children do not appear to be lacking in fantasy proneness; to the contrary, schizoids sometimes appear to have trouble distinguishing make-believe from reality (Wolff, 1998).
   
Schizoid Personality Disorder 71

Whereas autism is usually apparent in early childhood, schizoid traits are usually first apparent in middle childhood, when the development of social skills and such activities as team sports are more common. Although the disorders share similar symptoms, it is likely that they stem from separate etiologies.
Dimensions and Boundaries ________________________ in the Schizophrenia Spectrum
There is considerable item overlap and comorbidity between SCD and schizotypal personality disorder. SCD shares deficit symptoms with schizo- typal personality disorder, specifically those contributing to an asociality due to deficits in interpersonal skills and affect expression (Siever, Kalus, & Keefe, 1993). One group of researchers reported that schizoid personality correlated highly (r = .65) with schizotypal personality (Solano & De Chavez, 2000). Other researchers found that of those persons given a diag- nosis of SCD, 80% met criteria for schizotypal personality disorder (Farmer & Chapman, 2002). A longitudinal study following 141 schizoid adoles- cents discovered that three fourths of this sample met DSM-III criteria for schizotypal personality disorder (Wolff, 1991). Although SCD shares several social deficit symptoms with schizotypal personality disorder, the two disor- ders can be distinguished by the absence of positive symptoms (e.g., magical thinking) in schizoid personality disorder.
Because elevated rates of all three disorders in Cluster A are found in the families of schizophrenic patients relative to the general population, all three disorders may reflect a shared genetic predisposition (Miller et al., 2001). One line of studies, examining the history of mental illness in the relatives of schizophrenic individuals, suggests that the boundaries of schizophrenia- related disorders may extend beyond schizotypal personality disorder to include schizoid and paranoid personality disorders (Baron et al., 1985; Gunderson, Siever, & Spaulding, 1983). Cluster A disorders are often seen in the biological relatives of patients with schizophrenia. In a controlled family study of inpatients with schizophrenia, it was found that Cluster A personality disorders occurred at a rate of 2.1% in probands in comparison to a rate of 0.3% in matched control families (Maier, Lichtermann, Minges, & Heun, 1994). In addition, the premorbid histories of individuals with schizophrenia often include paranoid, schizoid, and schizotypal personality disorder diagnoses. Furthermore, the high-risk offspring of schizophrenic patients followed longitudinally are later distinguishable from normal con- trols only by the prevalence of all Cluster A disorders, not by each separately (Erlenmeyer-Kimling et al., 1995). A retrospective study in which family members of schizophrenic patients were interviewed using the Structured
   
72
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II; First, Gibbon, Spitzer, Williams, & Benjamin, 1997) found that 27.5% of the sample met criteria for premorbid SCD (Solano & De Chavez, 2000). Nevertheless, retrospective designs are vulnerable to hindsight bias, which may adversely affect the validity of reports. A prospective study is necessary to replicate and extend these findings.
In contrast, other studies suggest that a relationship with schizophre- nia extends to schizotypal personality disorder but not to SCD (Baron et al., 1985). For example, Maier, Lichtermann, Minges, and Heun (1994) examined psychiatric illnesses in the relatives of schizophrenic patients. They found that of the Cluster A disorders, SCD occurred the least frequently in probands (between 0.3% and 0.7%). Given that schizotypal personality disorder, another Cluster A disorder believed to overlap etiologically with schizophrenia, occurred in 2.1% of relatives, it seems likely that the relationship between SCD and schizophrenia is tenuous at best.
As noted, the historical definition of SCD may add to the confusion sur- rounding the methodology of studies aimed at determining the boundaries of SCD. Some studies examining the genetic boundaries of the schizophrenia spectrum may also have been confounded by a failure to distinguish between SCD and schizotypal personality disorder. Evidence that negative rather than positive symptoms are associated with increased heritability in schizo- phrenia (Dworkin & Lenzenweger, 1984) would theoretically support a familial/genetic link between schizophrenia and SCD, because the latter is largely expressed through mild negative symptoms.
Assessment _________________________________________
Although there are no measures designed to explicitly assess SCD, a number of comprehensive structured interviews include scales assessing the disorder. For example, the SCID-II (First et al., 1997) is a widely used instrument in the assessment of personality disorders. Interrater reliability estimates for SCD using this measure (kappa = .90 for categorical assessments and intraclass cor- relation = .93 for dimensional assessments) have been formed (Maffei et al., 1997). A similar interview, the Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders (SIDP-IV; Pfohl, Blum, & Zimmerman, 2001), also demonstrated good psychometric properties for SCD. For example, a recent study revealed that the SIDP-IV found higher levels of SCD in familial-high-risk siblings of schizophrenia patients versus normal controls (Auther, 2003).
In addition, there are several self-report questionnaires that assess SCD. The DSM-IV and ICD-10 Personality Questionnaire (DIP-Q) is a self-report measure designed to measure DSM-IV and ICD-10 personality disorders using 140 true/false questions (Ottosson, Grann, & Kullgren, 2000). Although this measure is useful because it offers diagnoses based
  
Schizoid Personality Disorder 73

on both diagnostic systems, a study examining the test-retest reliability reported a kappa value of only .42 for SCD (Ottosson, Grann, & Kullgren, 2000). In terms of internal consistency, the study reported Cronbach’s alpha statistics of .68 and .69 for the DSM-IV and ICD-10, respectively. More research is necessary to evaluate the validity of this instrument for assessing SCD.
Another potentially useful assessment tool focuses on a specific symptom dimension of SCD. Chapman, Chapman, and Raulin (1976) developed the Social Anhedonia Scale (SocAnh), a 40-item self-report measure that gauges level of indifference to other people. A high score on the scale, which has an internal consistency between .80 and .90 (Chapman et al., 1976), has been found to correlate with schizophrenia spectrum disorders (Kwapil, 1998). Nevertheless, there are no known data concerning this scale’s ability to dif- ferentiate among Cluster A disorders.
______________________________________________ Treatment
There are few reported treatments of SCD, partly because such patients are theoretically unlikely to request treatment; aloof and “loner” behavior ten- dencies that characterize the disorder also would probably make the seeking of treatment unlikely (Stone, 1993). In addition, clinical reports indicate that few schizoid individuals see indifference to interpersonal contact and avoid- ance of others as problematic (Miller et al., 2001). When schizoid persons do seek help, it is rarely for prolonged periods of time (Stone, 1993). In fact, it is often a comorbid Axis I disorder (Miller et al., 2001), an acute stress, familial pressure, or a shift in life circumstances (Siever & Kendler, 1987) that leads the schizoid individual to appear in mental health settings.
There are no well-controlled studies of treatment efficacy for SCD. However, a few researchers have suggested target areas for treatment pend- ing further research. For example, Beck and his colleagues (1990) suggest increasing social contact, learning skills useful for identifying emotions in the self as well as others, and using group therapy as a tool for practicing role playing and modeling appropriate behavior. Major changes and modifica- tions of character structure are considered unlikely, probably because of the- orized constitutionally determined limitations in affective response and expression (Millon, 1981). Therapy should probably be aimed at achieving modest reductions in social isolation and at promoting more effective adjust- ment to new circumstances. It is important to note that these therapeutic techniques have not been tested in controlled trials. The role of drug therapy for SCD remains an open question.
SCD seems to persist across the lifetime. A longitudinal study following a sample of schizoid children and comparing them with demographically matched normal controls demonstrated an increase in treatment for psychiatric disorders and a decrease in occupational functioning and rate of involvement in
  
74 PERSONALITY DISORDERS

intimate relationships (Wolff, 1991). However, the course of illness is not entirely bleak; rates of independent living and employment for these individu- als were not different from those of control subjects (Wolff, 1991).
Toward DSM-V _____________________________________
The current DSM classification system relies on an approach in which criteria are counted and then held in comparison to a cutoff value. For such personal- ity disorders as SCD, in which the signs and symptoms appear to be highly subjective, this approach may be particularly problematic. Because a number of symptoms are vague and subjective, the threshold for a criterion is inevitably somewhat arbitrary. For example, the wording of criterion 2 (“almost always . . .”) and criterion 4 (“takes pleasure in few . . .”) leaves considerable room for clinical interpretation. In addition, a number of the criteria for SCD, such as criterion 6 (“appears indifferent . . .), rely heavily on behavioral obser- vations (see Table 4.1). These criteria leave room for errors based on the con- text of assessment and biases resulting from the rater’s experience.
Future research using taxometric methods (Meehl, 1995) should deter- mine whether SCD is qualitatively or quantitatively different from normal- ity. These methods estimate accurate base rates, locate optimal cuts on indicators, and provide a classification of individuals as accurate as indica- tors will permit. They also help determine whether a disorder, such as SCD, is underpinned by a taxon (category in nature) rather than a dimension. If taxometric methods prove SCD to be qualitatively distinct, then future con- ceptions of the disorder should focus on identifying a discrete biological eti- ology, such as a dominant gene, a configural set of genes, or prenatal or perinatal insult. Such a finding could also imply that SCD is a member of the schizophrenia spectrum, which appears to be qualitatively distinct from nor- mality. This state of affairs may seem paradoxical, given that the schizophre- nia spectrum is marked by dimensional variation. Nevertheless, it’s useful to recall that within a taxon one can find dimensional variation attributable to polygenic factors, environmental influences, and the like. Taxometric meth- ods could also help to establish whether SCD differs in kind or degree from avoidant personality disorder, a condition with which it overlaps substantially.
Alternatively, taxometric methods could suggest that SCD is underpinned by a latent dimension rather than a taxon. A dimensional approach to conceptu- alizing SCD has been gaining increased support (Matthews, Saklofske, Costa, Deary, & Zeidner, 1998). Dimensional models treat personality as a multivari- ate space and view personality dysfunction as extreme constellations of this space. One example is offered by researchers working to translate personality disorders into a five-factor model (FFM; Widiger, Trull, Clarkin, Sanderson, & Costa, 1994). Widiger and his colleagues (1994) presented promising data for
   
Schizoid Personality Disorder 75

the use of the FFM in conceptualizing SCD. They found that low levels of extra- version, low levels of neuroticism, especially on facets such as self-consciousness, and low levels of openness to experience best characterized SCD. A follow-up study (Trull & Widiger, 1997) provided further validity for this conceptualiza- tion by demonstrating that SCD was negatively correlated with measures of extraversion. A recent study determined that once comorbid symptomatology is removed from Cluster A disorders, SCD is characterized by low levels of such extraversion facets as positive emotions, warmth, and gregariousness (Trull, Widiger, & Burr, 2001).
A number of other dimensional models show promise for further concep- tions of SCD. One criticism that surrounds the five-factor model is that it is based on personality constructs described by laypersons and omits complex personality features seen in clinical settings (Shedler & Westen, 2004). One alternative dimensional model has been derived from a card-sorting method: the Shedler-Westen Assessment Procedure-200 (SWAP-200). Researchers interviewed a large sample of psychiatrists and clinical psychologists who characterized specific personality disorders seen in their clinical work using a set of 200 personality-descriptive statements (Shedler & Westen, 2004). A Q (within-subject) factor analysis of these data yielded 12 dimensions: schizoid orientation, psychological health, psychopathy, hostility, narcis- sism, emotion dysregulation, dysphoria, obsessionality, thought disorder, oedipal conflict, dissociation, and sexual conflict statements (Shedler & Westen, 2004). The schizoid orientation dimension (Shedler & Westen, 2004) may be particularly suitable for describing and classifying SCD. For example, the highest factor loading (.58)—“appears to have little need for human contact; is genuinely indifferent to the presence of others” (p. 1749)—bears some similarities to several DSM-IV-TR criteria for SCD, but the next-highest loading (.57)—“tends to think in concrete terms and interpret things in overly literal ways; has limited ability to appreciate metaphor, analogy, or nuance” (p. 1749)—offers a new clinical perspective on SCD. By highlighting clinically relevant facets of the disorder, the SWAP- 200 offers to enrich clinical description and potentially add criteria that differentiate SCD from schizotypal and avoidant personality disorders.
________________________________________ Conclusions
Given the dearth of empirical information, many aspects of SPD remain poorly understood. Despite a rich clinical history, appropriate DSM place- ment of the diagnosis remains unclear. Information on prevalence is incon- sistent at best, and more discriminating diagnostic features are sorely needed. Most DSM-IV-TR SCD criteria with high specificity demonstrate unsatisfactory sensitivity, and those with high sensitivity generally have low specificity. There are no good treatment outcome data, and information on
   
76
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

the course of illness is limited. Distinguishing SCD from other phenomeno- logically similar personality disorders in Cluster A and from avoidant per- sonality disorder remains a key concern. The question of whether SCD is part of the schizophrenia spectrum disorders requires additional investiga- tion. Future research should incorporate biological markers (e.g., deviant smooth eye tracking, attentional deficits) that have been observed in schizo- typal personality disorder and schizophrenia (Siever et al., 1993). In addi- tion, family history studies and designs including less seriously affected individuals are necessary to determine the extent of genetic overlap between SCD and both Asperger’s disorder and schizophrenia (Wolff, 1998).
References _________________________________________
Akhtar, S. (1987). Schizoid personality disorder: A synthesis of developmental, dynamic, and descriptive features. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 41, 499–518.
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of men- tal disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of men- tal disorders (3rd ed., revised.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of men- tal disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of men- tal disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.
Auther, A. M. (2003). Social anhedonia and schizophrenia-spectrum personality traits in genetic high-risk adolescents and young adults. Dissertation Abstracts International, 63, 7B. (UMI No. 3464)
Baron, M., Gruen, R., Rainer, J. D., Kane, J., Asnis, L., & Lord, S. (1985). A family study of schizophrenic and normal control probands: Implications for the spec- trum concept of schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 447–454.
Beck, A. T., Freeman, A. T., Pretzer, J., Davis, D. D., Fleming, B., Ottaviani, R., et al. (1990). Cognitive therapy of personality disorders. New York: Guilford Press.
Blackburn, R., Donnelly, J. P., Logan, C., & Renwick, S. J. D. (2004). Convergent and discriminative validity of interview and questionnaire measures of personality dis- order in mentally disordered offenders: A multitrait-multimethod analysis using confirmatory factor analysis. Journal of Personality Disorders, 18(2), 129–150.
Bleuler, E. (1924). Textbook of psychiatry (A. A. Brill, Trans.). New York: Macmillan.
Bleuler, M. (1954). The concept of schizophrenia [Letter to the editor]. American Journal of Psychiatry, 111, 382–383.
Chapman, L. J., Chapman, J. P., & Raulin, M. L. (1976). Scales for physical and social anhedonia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85, 374–382.
Dahl, A. (1986). Some aspects of the DSM-III personality disorders illustrated by a consecutive sample of hospitalized patients. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 73(Suppl. 228), 61–66.
Drake, R., & Vaillant, G. (1985). A validity study of Axis II of DSM-III. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 553–558.
   
Schizoid Personality Disorder 77

Dworkin, R., & Lenzenweger, M. (1984). Symptoms and the genetics of schizophre- nia: Implications for diagnosis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 1541–1546.
Ekselius, L., Tillfors, M., Furmark, T., & Fredrikson, M. (2001). Personality disorders in the general population: DSM-IV and ICD-10 defined prevalence as related to sociodemographic profile. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 311–320.
Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L., Squire-Wheeler, E., Hildoff, U., Bassett, A. S., Cornblatt, B. A., Kestenbaum, C. J., et al. (1995). The New York High-Risk Project: Psychoses and Cluster A personality disorders in offspring of schizophrenic patients at 23 years of follow-up. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52, 827–865.
Farmer, R. F., & Chapman, A. L. (2002). Evaluation of DSM-IV personality disorder criteria as assessed by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 43, 285–300.
First, M. B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., & Benjamin, L. S. (1997).
Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis II personality disorders.
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Freiman, K., & Widiger, T. A. (1989). [Co-occurrence and diagnostic efficiency sta-
tistics]. Unpublished raw data.
Grilo, C. M., McGlashan, T. H., Morey, L. C., Gunderson, J. G., Skodol, A.E.,
Tracie, S. M., et al. (2001). Internal consistency, intercriterion overlap and diagnostic efficiency of criteria sets for DSM-IV schizotypal, borderline, avoidant and obsessive- compulsive personality disorders. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 104, 264–272.
Gunderson, J. G., Siever, I. J., & Spaulding, F. (1983). The search for a schizotype: Crossing the border again. Archives of General Psychiatry, 40, 15–22.
Guntrip, H. (1969). Schizoid phenomena, object relations, and the self. New York: International Universities Press.
Hyler, S. E., & Rieder, R. O. (1994). Personality diagnostic questionnaire–4+. New York: Authors.
Kretschmer, E. (1925). Physique and character. London: Kegan Paul.
Kwapil, T. R. (1998). Social anhedonia as a predictor of the development of schizo-
phrenia-spectrum disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 558–565. Loranger, A. W., Susman, V. L., Oldham, J. M., & Russakoff, L. M. (1987). Personality Disorder Examination: A preliminary report. Journal of Personality
Disorders, 1, 1–13.
Maffei, C., Fossati, A., Agostoni, I., Barrao, A., Bagnato, M., Deborah, D., et al.
(1997). Interrater reliability and internal consistency of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II), Version 2.0. Journal of Personality Disorders, 11(3), 279–284.
Maier, W., Lichtermann, D., Minges, J., & Heun, R. (1994). Personality disorders among the relatives of schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 20(3), 481–493.
Matthews, G., Saklofske, D., Costa, P. T., Jr., Deary, I. J., & Zeidner, M. (1998). Dimensional models of personality: A framework for systematic clinical assess- ment. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 14(1), 36–49.
Meehl, P. E. (1995). Bootstraps and taxometrics. American Psychologist, 50(4), 266–275. Miller, M. B., Useda, D., Trull, T. J., Burr, M., & Minks-Brown, C. (2001). Paranoid, schizoid, and schizotypal personality disorder. In H. E. Adams & P. B. Sutker (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of psychopathology (3rd ed.).
New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.
78
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Millon, T. (1981). Disorders of personality: DSM-III, Axis II. New York: Wiley Interscience.
Millon, T., Millon, C., & Davis, R. (1994). MCMI-III manual. Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems.
Millon, T., & Tringone, R. (1989). [Co-occurrence and diagnostic efficiency statis- tics]. Unpublished raw data.
Morey, L. (1988). Personality disorders in DSM-III and DSM-IIIR: Convergence, age, and internal consistency. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 573–577.
Morey, L., & Heumann, K. (1988). [Co-occurrence and diagnostic efficiency statis- tics]. Unpublished raw data.
Ottosson, H., Ekselius, L., Grann, M., & Kullgren, G. (2002). Cross-system concor- dance of personality disorder diagnosis of DSM-IV and diagnostic criteria for research of ICD-10. Journal of Personality Disorders, 16(3), 283–292.
Ottosson, H., Grann, M., & Kullgren, G. (2000). Test-retest reliability of a self- report questionnaire for DSM-IV and ICD-10 personality disorder. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 16, 53–58.
Overholser, J. C. (1989). Differentiation between schizoid and avoidant personali- ties: An empirical test. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 34(8), 785–790.
Pfohl, B., Blum, N., & Zimmerman, M. (2001). Structured interview for DSM–IV personality (SIDP-IV). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Reich, J., Yates, W., & Nduaguba, M. (1989). Prevalence of DSM-III personality disor- ders in the community. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 24, 12–16. Samuels, J., Eaton, W. W., Bienvenu, J., III, Brown, C. H., Costa, P. T., Jr., & Nestadt, G. (2002). Prevalence and correlates of personality disorders in a com-
munity sample. British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 536–542.
Shedler, J., & Westen, D. (2004). Dimensions of personality pathology: An alterna- tive to the five-factor model. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 1743–1754. Siever, L. J., Kalus, O. F., & Keefe, R. S. E. (1993). The boundaries of schizophre-
nia. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 16(2), 217–244.
Siever, L. J., & Kendler, K. S. (1987). Schizoid/schizotypal/paranoid personality dis-
orders. In J. O. Cavenar (Ed.), Psychiatry. New York: Basic Books.
Skodol, A. F., Rosnick, L., Kellman, D., Oldham, J., & Hyler, S. (1988). Validating structured DSM-III-R personality disorder assessments with longitudinal data.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 1297–1299.
Solano, J. J. R., & De Chavez, M. G. (2000). Premorbid personality disorders in
schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 44, 137–144.
Stone, M. H. (1993). Long-term outcome in personality disorders. British Journal
of Psychiatry, 162, 299–313.
Terry, G. C., & Rennie, T. (1938). Analysis of paraergesia. American Journal
of Orthopsychiatry, 9, 817–918.
Trull, T. J., & Widiger, T. A. (1997). Structured interview for the five-factor model
of personality (SIFFM): Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological
Assessment Resources.
Trull, T. J., Widiger, T. A., & Burr, R. (2001). A structured interview for the assess-
ment of the five-factor model of personality: Facet-level relations to the Axis II
personality disorders. Journal of Personality, 69(2), 175–198.
Trull, T. J., Widiger, T. A., & Frances, A. (1987). Covariation of criteria sets for avoidant, schizoid, and dependent personality disorders. American Journal
of Psychiatry, 144(6), 767–771.
Schizoid Personality Disorder 79

Widiger, T. A., Trull, T. J., Clarkin, J. F., Sanderson, C., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1994). A description of the DSM-III-R and DSM-IV personality criteria sets. In P. T. Costa, Jr., & T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality (pp. 41–56). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Wolff, S. (1991). “Schizoid” personality in childhood and adult life: III. The child- hood picture. British Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 629–635.
Wolff, S. (1998). Schizoid personality in childhood: The links with Asperger syn- drome, schizophrenia spectrum disorders, and elective mutism. In E. Schopler & Z. B. Gary (Eds.), Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism? Current issues in autism (pp. 123–138). New York: Plenum.
World Health Organization. (1992). International classification of diseases (10th revision). Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
Zimmerman, M., & Coryell, W. H. (1990). Diagnosing personality disorders in the community: A comparison of self-report and interview measures. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47, 527–531.

Schizotypal 5 Personality Disorder
Annie M. Bollini Emory University
Elaine F. Walker Emory University
________________________________________ Introduction
Many of the diagnostic entities in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM; American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2000) are derived from a combination of clinical experience, untested theoretical assumptions, and consensus. This is especially true of the Axis II disorders. But the diagnostic entity of schizotypal personality disorder (SPD) is largely grounded in empirical research. It is the product of research findings accu- mulated from behavior genetics and longitudinal studies conducted over sev- eral decades.
In this chapter, we begin by briefly reviewing the historical origins of SPD and presenting a typical case of an adolescent who meets criteria for SPD. Next, we discuss recent approaches to the assessment of schizotypal signs and syndromes, and then we turn to the accumulating empirical literature on the course and correlates of SPD. Finally, we draw on these findings to spec- ulate on the validity and reliability of the SPD construct as it is currently con- ceptualized and offer directions for future research.
_______________________________ Historical Background
Although it is true that research (Kety, Rosenthal, Wender, & Schulsinger, 1968; Meehl, 1962, 1973a, 1973b) guided the conceptualization of SPD as
 
81
82
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

a diagnostic entity, clinical observations served as the initial impetus for that research. These observations were largely drawn from the literature on schiz- ophrenia. In fact, even the earliest descriptive writings on dementia praecox, and subsequently those on schizophrenia, contained comments on behav- ioral abnormalities observed in some biological relatives of patients. Bleuler (1924, 1969) and Kraepelin (1919/1971) noted that some first-degree rela- tives of probands, though not psychotic, had odd behavioral propensities that were enduring and similar to the symptoms of schizophrenia.
Concomitant with these trends in the psychiatric literature, research psy- chologists were advancing new ideas about the origins of vulnerability for schizophrenia, and this work also contributed to conceptualizations of SPD. In a seminal article, Meehl (1962) coined the term “schizotaxia” to refer to a constitutionally based behavioral syndrome that reflects the genetic liability for schizophrenia. On the biological level, Meehl described this vulnerability as a syndrome involving a subtle “neurointegrative” defect (i.e., problems with associated neural systems) with genetic origins. Meehl proposed that schizotaxia did not always progress to clinical schizophrenia. He hypothesized that, depending on environmental circumstances, some schizotaxic individuals remain stable over time, whereas others progress to either “schizotypy” or schizophrenia. Stressful social environments were presumed to be capable of triggering the transition to schizophrenia. This notion parallels the diathesis- stress model of schizophrenia spectrum disorders that has dominated the field over the past several decades (Gottesman, 1991; Meehl, 1973a, 1973b; Walker & Diforio, 1997).
With the publication of Meehl’s paper in 1962, the terms schizotaxia and schizotypy entered the literature on schizophrenia and set the stage for sys- temic investigations of the presence of subclinical psychotic signs in non- clinical populations. Loren and Jean Chapman have been at the forefront of this field of research. These investigators and their colleagues began by obtaining retrospective self-reports of premorbid signs from schizophrenia patients (Freedman & Chapman, 1973). Patients reported that prior to the clinical onset of their illness, they experienced increased thought blocking, mental fatigue, inability to focus attention, visual illusions, misidentification of people, acute auditory perceptual abnormalities (i.e., heightened sensitiv- ity to noises/sounds), and impaired perception of speech. The Chapmans subsequently coined the term “psychosis proneness” to refer to a predispo- sition, or diathesis, for psychosis, similar to the concept of schizotaxia. Drawing on the diathesis-stress model, the Chapmans assumed that psy- choses, both schizophrenia and affective, arise from the interaction between environmental stress and a predisposition, or proneness, to psychotic think- ing. Along with their colleagues, the Chapmans developed paper-and-pencil screening inventories to measure several key dimensions of psychosis prone- ness: the Physical Anhedonia Scale, the Perceptual Aberration Scale, the Magical Ideation Scale, the Impulsive Nonconformity Scale, and the Social Anhedonia Scale (Chapman & Chapman, 1980; Chapman, Chapman,
  
Schizotypal Personality Disorder 83
 
& Miller, 1982; Chapman, Chapman, & Raulin, 1976). They sought to test their assumptions about psychosis proneness by administering these scales to large numbers of college students who would be followed longitudinally. When subjects were followed 10 years after completing the scales, the authors found a significantly elevated rate of psychotic disorders in those designated as psychosis prone based on their scores obtained while they were in college (Chapman, Chapman, Kwapil, Eckblad, & Zinser, 1994).
Meehl’s (1962) assumptions about the behavioral manifestations of vul- nerability to schizophrenia were also reflected in research on the genetic ori- gins of schizophrenia. The early behavior genetics studies of schizophrenia, primarily using family history and adoption methods, established that a behavioral syndrome, which included oddities in social behavior, percep- tions, and ideation, was more common in the relatives of patients than in the general population (for a review, see Gottesman, 1991). As described in greater detail later, one of the earliest and most seminal studies was con- ducted by Kety and his colleagues (1968; Kety, Rosenthal, Wender, Schulsinger, & Jacobsen, 1976), who used reviews of hospital records and interviews with biological and adoptive relatives of schizophrenia patients. The investigators discovered an elevated rate of what they labeled “border- line” or “possible schizophrenia” (i.e., the presence of some clinically mean- ingful symptoms of schizophrenia, but too few to qualify for an Axis I diagnosis) in the biological relatives of patients.
_________________________________________________ Case Study
The following case study is a description of a young man who meets criteria for SPD. It depicts some of the symptoms, behaviors, and cognitive deficits that are characteristic of an individual with SPD.
Martin is a 17-year-old male high school student. His mother brought him to a university psychological center to participate in a research study about adolescent development because she was motivated to obtain the incentive: a free psychological assessment. She told the interviewer that she has been con- cerned about Martin’s emotional and social functioning over the last 2 to 3 years and offered that his behavior reminded her of her eldest brother, who had a schizophrenia diagnosis when he was a youth.
As part of the study, Martin participated in a structured clinical interview. He was distant and reserved and made little eye contact with the interviewer. When asked a question, he frequently asked the interviewer to repeat or rephrase it, and he seemed to have difficulty providing examples to supple- ment his experiences. When answering a question, he was often vague or went off on tangents. He described feeling nervous around people, especially new people, and he frequently found himself preoccupied with questioning their
(Continued)
84
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

e i
a
(Continued)
intentions. He said he had no friends and preferred to do activities alone; he spent most of his time playing video games. In addition, he talked about his experiences with telepathy. He believed he was able to read some people’s thoughts, especially those of his family. On several occasions, he had felt as if one of his parents had stolen thoughts from his head, but he explained to the interviewer that he had learned to deal with this problem by avoiding his parents. On occasion, he noticed objects and flashes of light out of the corner of his eye, but then he realized there was nothing there and told the interviewer it was probably in his head. During the interview, he seemed completely unconcerned about any aspects of his life other than his parents’ reluctance to buy him a newer version of his current gaming system.
Martin’s story is typical of a person who meets criteria for SPD. He is xperiencing perceptual abnormalities, has unusual beliefs, and is socially solated. The next sections describe the diagnostic system used to identify people with SPD and detail the genetic, developmental, and behavioral
spects of the disorder and its relation to schizophrenia.
The Diagnosis of SPD ________________________________
In the late 1970s, the accumulating body of evidence gave rise to the syn- drome labeled “schizotypal personality disorder,” which entered the psychi- atric nomenclature. Spitzer and his collaborators (Spitzer, Endicott, & Gibbon, 1979) proposed formal criteria for the new diagnostic category for the DSM-III (APA, 1980), and these were included in all subsequent revi- sions of the DSM. It is classified as an Axis II personality disorder, and thus is assumed to reflect an enduring set of behavioral propensities. Despite revi- sions of the DSM (i.e., DSM-III-R [APA, 1987], DSM-IV [APA, 1994], and DSM-IV-TR [APA, 2000]), the diagnostic criteria for SPD have remained relatively constant. At this point, there is not sufficient research on the lon- gitudinal course of symptoms and functioning in people who meet current DSM criteria for SPD to warrant making modifications to these criteria in the next DSM revision. Once sufficient data are available, however, there may be a reasonable basis for revision. As it is currently defined, SPD involves both behavioral deficits, or negative signs, and behavioral abnormalities, or positive signs.
The DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for SPD include attenuated versions of the defining features of schizophrenia. Thus, for example, individuals with SPD report unusual sensory experiences that may be troubling, but the expe- riences do not qualify as hallucinations because their validity is questioned by the individual. Likewise, although unusual ideas may be present, they are not delusional in nature.
 
Schizotypal Personality Disorder 85

Schizotaxia did not become part of the nomenclature. Instead, this concept has been used by researchers to refer to the constitutional liability to schizo- phrenia. The results of nearly four decades of research, however, suggest that schizotaxia, too, may reflect a meaningful clinical condition, as well as an underlying liability for more severe schizophrenia-related conditions. Faraone, Green, Seidman, and Tsuang (2001) have recently reformulated the concept of schizotaxia, and their conceptualization differs from Meehl’s (1962, 1973) in three ways. They assume that (1) the etiology of schizotaxia derives from both genetic factors and biological consequences of adverse environmental factors (e.g., pregnancy or delivery complications), whereas Meehl assumed the etiol- ogy was solely genetic; (2) schizotaxia reflects a multifactorial polygenic etiol- ogy, while Meehl proposed a single, major gene; and (3) schizotaxia is a stable condition in most cases, so neither schizotypy nor schizophrenia is the only, or most likely, outcome. These investigators are now beginning to conduct research aimed at identifying the components of schizotaxia based on studies of first-degree relatives of schizophrenia and SPD patients.
Exploring the Dimensions and Boundaries of SPD
As discussed in other chapters, there has been an ongoing debate about the relative merits of categorical versus dimensional approaches to personality. Both of these perspectives are reflected in the literature on SPD. The categor- ical approach is used in clinical practice, as well as in some clinical research studies in which SPD is conceptualized as a diagnostic entity with multiple indicators. In both realms, the DSM criteria are applied for diagnosis. For clinical research on SPD, structured interviews are typically employed so that information is obtained in a consistent manner. There are several structured clinical interviews for the personality disorders, such as the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II; First, Gibbon, Spitzer, Williams, & Benjamin, 1997) and the Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality (SIDP-IV; Pfohl, Blum, & Zimmerman, 1994). There is one clinical interview specifically designed to assess SPD: the Structured Interview for Schizotypy (SIS; Kendler & Lister-Sharp, 1989).
The dimensional approach to schizotypy is reflected in a rapidly accumulat- ing body of literature. As the phenomenology of SPD became more widely rec- ognized in the clinical and research communities, investigators began to explore the measurement of the key features presumed to define the syndrome. Assuming that these features reflect relatively enduring behavioral and psychological dimensions and thus vary along a continuum, several self-report scales (e.g., the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire) were developed to index schizotypal traits. A pivotal assumption guiding this work is that schizotypal signs are a man- ifestation of the genetic liability to schizophrenia and will occur at a higher fre- quency and intensity in the biological relatives of schizophrenia patients. Research findings consistent with this assumption have been obtained for some of the presumed signs of schizotypy (Kendler, Gruenberg, & Strauss, 1981).
  
86
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

The published measures of schizotypy vary in the extent to which they rely on the individual’s self-report and in the emphasis they place on various features of SPD. The SIS was originally developed for behavior genetics research (Kendler & Lister-Sharp, 1989). It relies on both self-report and examiner observations. The SIS contains two sections and distinguishes between symptoms and signs. The first part is a structured interview with questions intended to yield information about schizotypal symptoms and social relationships. Based on the results, the examiner rates the subject, using seven-point scales, on 19 dimensions, including childhood social isola- tion, ideas of reference, magical thinking, and impulsivity. The second part of the SIS contains 16 scales and involves ratings of schizotypal signs based on the interviewer’s observations of the individual’s behavior. Examples of these scales are rapport, attention seeking, grooming, and enjoyment of the interview. Average intraclass correlations in the original studies were of the order of .8 for symptoms and .7 for signs. Advantageous features of the SIS are (1) questions that aid the interviewer in making contextual assessments of the pathological nature of certain symptoms (e.g., suspiciousness, ideas of reference) and (2) symptom probes designed to make responding in the affir- mative appear nondeviant.
The SIS was revised by Vollema and Ormel (2000), primarily to stan- dardize the rating procedures. In this revision (SIS-R), a four-point scale was introduced to provide more explicit criteria (i.e., frequency, duration, and level of conviction) for rating symptoms and signs. The SIS-R was adminis- tered to a sample of psychiatric patients with personality disorders, and the results showed that most schizotypal symptoms can be assessed reliably with the SIS-R, but only four of the nine individual schizotypal signs (e.g., goal- directed behavior, loosening of associations, amount of speech, and oddness) can be reliably assessed (Vollema & Ormel, 2000).
As described previously, the Chapman scales for measuring psychosis proneness represented the first systematic attempt to index subclinical signs of risk for psychotic disorders. This line of research has been extended in subsequent measures designed to index the DSM criteria for SPD. Notable among these is the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ), which is a relatively brief self-report instrument that provides subscale scores for each of the nine DSM schizotypal personality disorder criteria (Raine, 1991). The SPQ taps both positive tendencies (i.e., distortions of normal functioning, such as hallucinations, bizarre delusions, and disorganized thoughts) and neg- ative tendencies (i.e., diminution of normal functioning, such as restricted range of emotion, social withdrawal, decreased goal-directed behavior) and includes items pertaining to ideas of reference, suspiciousness, magical think- ing, and unusual perceptual experiences. The SPQ was developed for the study of schizotypy in the general population and as a screening device for SPD. This instrument has high test-retest reliability (.82) over a 2-month period and high convergent validity (r = .65–.81), but low, though signifi- cant, discriminant validity (r = .19–.37). Factor analysis of the SPQ yields
  
Schizotypal Personality Disorder 87

three factors: cognitive-perceptual, interpersonal, and disorganized. This fac- tor structure has been documented in both nonpatient and patient samples (Raine et al., 1994; Vollema & Hoijtink, 2000).
Another example of a self-report measure is the Kings Schizotypy Questionnaire (KSQ). The KSQ is a forced-choice (yes/no) self-report measure of schizotypy developed by Williams (1993). The KSQ contains items based on the DSM-III criteria for SPD as well as on self-reported subjective experi- ences described by psychiatrically well relatives of schizophrenia patients. In the development of the questionnaire, high internal consistency and test- retest reliability were established (Williams, 1993), and this instrument was found to differentiate schizophrenia from nonschizophrenia individuals at a high rate (Jones et al., 2000). Factor analysis confirmed that the KSQ tapped the major dimensions of schizotypy, including both positive and negative signs (Claridge et al., 1996; Williams, 1993).
Prevalence of SPD
Specifying base rates for personality disorders has proven difficult. Diagnosing any personality disorder can take several hours of interviewing. Moreover, people who meet criteria for these disorders are likely to be dis- inclined to initiate evaluation, treatment, or research participation. This may be especially true of individuals who meet criteria for SPD, as they manifest signs of social withdrawal. Because their social interactions are reduced, they are less likely to be clinically detected. For these reasons, estimates of the base rate of SPD are probably conservative.
The DSM-IV reports that the prevalence of SPD in the general population is 3% (APA, 1994). This figure is based on studies of SPD yielding estimates between 1% and 3%. In first-degree relatives of normal controls, the SPD prevalence rate was found to be 1% (Maier, Lichtermann, Klingler, Heun, & Hallmayer, 1992). This figure is presumed to be an underestimate of the pop- ulation rate due to the fact that the probands were screened for psychiatric disorder. A large-scale study of consecutive psychiatric admissions to a com- munity hospital found that 2% met criteria for SPD (Loranger, 1990). Finally, based on phone interviews, another study found that 3% of the respondents met criteria for SPD (Zimmerman & Coryell, 1990). Because the 3% rate obtained via phone interview is based on the most representative sample, it is probably the most valid estimate of the population rate. However, this 3% figure should be considered conservative, as this estimate is based on a random sample of individuals who were willing and able to respond to a telephone survey about their mental health. Some with SPD would not agree to such an interview or have a telephone. Nonetheless, at 3%, SPD would be one of the more common personality disorders, because the prevalence of all personality disorders combined is estimated to be about 11% (Lenzenweger, 1999).
  
88
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

The estimated prevalence of SPD is much higher, possibly three to four times higher, than the prevalence of schizophrenia. As described in the following sections, the majority of those who meet criteria for SPD do not develop any psychotic disorder, although the risk for schizophrenia is significantly elevated in individuals with SPD (Miller, Byrne, et al., 2002; Yung et al., 1998).
The Genetic Link Between Schizotypal
Personality Disorder and Schizophrenia ________________
The genetic relation between schizophrenia and SPD has been the subject of investigation for several decades. The concept of SPD hinges on its genetic link with schizophrenia, and as currently defined, SPD and schizophrenia share common features such as odd beliefs, social impairment, and abnor- malities in perceptual experiences and emotional expression. However, SPD criteria specify an attenuated version of the psychotic symptoms necessary for a schizophrenia diagnosis. How strong is the genetic link between the two disorders?
To address this question, researchers have conducted family, twin, and adoption studies. Most examine the rate of schizophrenia or SPD in first- and second-degree family members of probands (i.e., index individuals with SPD or schizophrenia). Findings from these diverse approaches converge to lend substantial support to the notion that SPD and schizophrenia share a genetic liability.
As noted earlier, Kety and his colleagues (1968) published a landmark report from the Danish Adoption Study that fueled interest in schizotypy. They conducted assessments of the biological relatives of adoptees (probands) who were diagnosed with schizophrenia in young adulthood. The study also included the biological relatives of a group of normal adoptees. In most cases, the adoptees who were the subject of this research had been placed in their adoptive homes in infancy. The investigators found a significantly higher rate of schizophrenia in the biological relatives of the probands with schizophrenia (5.3%) than in the relatives of those probands without the ill- ness (1.8%), confirming a genetic contribution to the etiology of schizo- phrenia. In addition, they observed an elevated rate (15.9%) of milder psychiatric syndromes in the biological relatives of the adopted schizophre- nia patients compared to relatives of nonschizophrenia probands (4.9%). These syndromes were characterized by subclinical versions of some of the defining symptoms of schizophrenia. Thus a subgroup of the relatives of probands manifested interpersonal deficits, odd ideas, unusual perceptual experiences, and behavioral abnormalities.
The findings from the Danish Adoption Study not only provided further evidence for the genetic etiology of schizophrenia but also suggested the exis- tence of less severe behavioral manifestations of the genetic vulnerability.
   
Schizotypal Personality Disorder 89

This and subsequent reports led to the notion of “schizophrenia spectrum disorders” and set the stage for the development of diagnostic criteria for SPD, which were published in 1979 (Spitzer, Endicott, & Gibbon, 1979).
In an effort to validate the diagnostic criteria proposed for SPD, Kendler and his colleagues (1981) used the SIS to rediagnose a subset of the biologi- cal relatives from the Kety et al. (1968) investigation. Consistent with pre- dictions, they found a higher rate of SPD (10.5%) in biological relatives of schizophrenic adoptees than in controls (1.5%). In contrast, the rates of SPD in the adoptive relatives of both the schizophrenia and control adoptees were very low (2%–3%) and strikingly similar. This report yielded the first empir- ical support for the validity of the newly developed criteria for SPD.
The majority of the investigations aimed at understanding the genetic relation between SPD and schizophrenia have used the family method. Studies of the family members of SPD probands have revealed significantly elevated rates of schizophrenia (2%–4%) and other schizophrenia spectrum disorders (14–15%) when compared to the relatives of individuals with other personality disorders (Battaglia, Bernardeschi, Franchini, Bellodi, & Smeraldi, 1995; Siever et al., 1990). Similarly, studies of the biological rela- tives of schizophrenia patients replicate the findings from the Danish Adoption Study. The rate of SPD is significantly higher in family members of schizophrenia patients than in relatives of normal controls (Gershon, DeLisi, Hamovit, & Nurnberger, 1988; Kendler & Gruenberg, 1984). Along the same lines, a meta-analysis using data from three large-scale family studies revealed that when data on relatives were aggregated, the odds ratios (OR) for schizophrenia (OR = 16.2), SPD (OR = 5.0), and nonaffective psy- chosis (OR = 4.0) were higher for relatives of schizophrenia probands than for normal control probands (Kendler & Gardner, 1997).
Adoption and family studies have established a genetic link between schiz- ophrenia and SPD. The mode of transmission of the genetic vulnerability, however, has not yet been established, although the prevailing assumption is that it is polygenic (Gottesman, 1991). Thus, it is assumed that multiple, addi- tive, and/or interactive genes, rather than a single major genetic locus, confer the risk for schizophrenia. This raises the question of whether SPD and schiz- ophrenia are behavioral expressions of the same or different polygenotypes. Perhaps the SPD relatives of schizophrenia probands possess only a subset of the genes required for the schizophrenia phenotype. The full complement of “risk genes” (i.e., complete polygenotype) may be required for schizophrenia. A related question concerns the heritability of the polygenotype. Does its presence always lead to schizophrenia, or, as implied by the prevailing diathe- sis-stress model, do environmental factors interact to determine the nature of the phenotypic expression? It is possible that the polygenotype can be expressed as either SPD or schizophrenia, depending on environmental fac- tors. Research on twins has the potential to shed light on this issue.
Twin studies are a powerful methodological tool for exploring the genetic contribution to psychiatric syndromes. The results of twin studies provide
  
90
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

support for the notion that the phenotypic expression of the genetic vulnera- bility to schizophrenia can vary, with SPD being one of the variants (Torgersen, 1994). Several studies have shown that SPD occurs more fre- quently in the monozygotic (MZ) co-twins of schizophrenia patients than in the dizygotic (DZ) co-twins of patients. For example, Farmer, McGuffin, and Gottesman (1987) examined the Maudsley twin sample and obtained the maximum MZ/DZ concordance ratio (7.68) when diagnostic criteria were broadened to include “spectrum disorders,” namely, SPD, affective dis- order with mood-incongruent delusions, and atypical psychosis, in addition to schizophrenia. In contrast, adding paranoid disorder (paranoia) and all other affective disorder categories reduced the ratio. Similarly, Torgersen and Kringlen (1991) interviewed MZ and DZ twin pairs with schizophrenia probands and found a significantly elevated rate of SPD in the co-twins. But the concordance rates were elevated for both MZ and DZ pairs, and the MZ/DZ concordance ratio was not large, indicating that shared environ- mental factors, including the prenatal environment, also play a role in the development of SPD. Further, when Torgersen (1984) examined co-twins of probands with SPD, he found that no MZ co-twins met diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia. This finding is consistent with data on the base rates of SPD and schizophrenia, as well as the assumption that shared environmen- tal factors play a role in the expression of both SPD and schizophrenia.
Taken together, the findings on MZ twins indicate that the same polygeno- type can result in schizophrenia or SPD. Consistent with this notion, Risch and Baron’s (1984) quantitative genetic modeling analysis of data on twin pairs with one schizophrenia proband revealed that the best fit was obtained with broader diagnostic criteria that included both schizophrenia and SPD, rather than schizophrenia alone. When the broad phenotype was used, all single-locus models without polygenic background were excluded, but the results were consistent with polygenic inheritance. Further, the polygenic model was a better fit for the data when SPD was included in the phenotype, again suggesting that SPD is an alternative manifestation of the liability to schizophrenia.
Of course, schizophrenia and SPD vary in their symptom expression. It is of interest to know whether the risk rate for either disorder in relatives varies as a function of the proband’s symptom profile. Some findings indicate that the negative signs of schizophrenia and schizotypy are more heritable than the positive signs. Dworkin and Lenzenweger (1984) studied twin pairs with schizophrenia probands and found that concordance rates in the MZ pairs were higher when probands had a greater number of negative symptoms, but there was no evidence of a similar relationship for positive symptoms. The authors concluded that negative symptoms may be indicative of a more her- itable genotype. Similarly, when Torgersen (1984) examined the MZ and DZ concordance rates for DSM Cluster A disorders, SPD showed significant heritability, higher than that of borderline personality disorder. But the genetic influences on SPD were significantly more pronounced for the social
  
Schizotypal Personality Disorder 91

withdrawal and paranoid/suspicious features than they were for the “psychotic-like” ideational and perceptual anomalies. Along these lines, a recent report by MacDonald and colleagues (MacDonald, Pogue-Geile, Debski, & Manuck, 2001) examined the occurrence of schizotypal signs in MZ and DZ twins using the Chapman scales. The findings indicated that social anhedonia (negative sign) and perceptual aberration (positive sign) were more strongly influenced by genes than were indices of ideational abnormality (positive sign). Finally, when the KSQ was administered to large samples of schizophrenia patients, their healthy first-degree relatives, and unrelated controls, it did not distinguish the relatives from controls, although schizophrenia patients scored higher on the KSQ (Jones et al., 2000). The authors conjectured that their results might be due to defensive responding and/or self-selection bias among relatives, or the fact that the KSQ does not assess negative signs. Taken together, these studies indicate that the negative signs of SPD may be under greater genetic control than the positive signs (Faraone et al., 2001).
In contrast, other findings suggest that positive signs evidence greater heri- tability. Baron, Gruen, and Romo-Gruen (1992) found that the risk for both schizophrenia and SPD was higher in first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients with predominantly positive symptoms than it was in relatives of patients with mainly negative symptoms. Kremen et al. (1998) compared SPQ scores of normal controls to the scores of the biological relatives of schizophre- nia patients. Relatives scored higher on positive schizotypy, particularly the cognitive-perceptual factor of the SPQ, when compared to controls. No differ- ences were found for the negative and disorganization schizotypy dimensions. Yaralian et al. (2000) also found that the relatives of schizophrenia patients had elevated scores on the cognitive-perceptual factor of the SPQ, particularly for the “unusual perceptual experiences” and “ideas of reference” subscales.
These results suggest that previous failures to demonstrate elevated scores on positive symptoms of schizotypy may be a function of the measure. The earlier studies used measures other than the SPQ, such as the Perceptual Aberration Scale, which may not be as sensitive as the SPQ to individual dif- ferences in the positive signs of schizotypy. As mentioned, the SPQ assesses four positive features of SPD: ideas of reference, magical thinking, unusual perceptual experiences, and suspiciousness, and these items contributed to the differentiation of biological relatives from controls in both the Yaralian et al. (2000) and Kremen et al. (1998) studies. Some of these items are not assessed by the Perceptual Aberration Scale, which may account for the discrepancy between studies that do and do not find group differences for positive schizo- typy. Clearly, more research is needed to identify the key dimensions of schizotypy that are genetically influenced and linked to schizophrenia.
In summary, there is substantial support from family, adoption, and twin studies that schizophrenia and SPD share a common genetic liability. But the MZ concordance rates for both schizophrenia and SPD indicate that envi- ronmental factors also play a key role (Gottesman, 1991).
  
92
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Obstetrical complications (OCs) are considered to be one such environmen- tal factor. OCs occur at higher rates in the medical histories of schizophrenia patients than in nonpsychiatric samples (Cannon, Barr, & Mednick, 1991; Schulsinger, Parnas, Mednick, Teasdale, & Schulsinger, 1987). Bakan and Peterson (1994) found that college undergraduates with SPD traits experienced substantially more OCs during their births. Artificial labor induction and birth defects were the most commonly reported OCs in the high SPD trait group.
Environment may also play a key role in the expression of a vulnerability to SPD. Although there are no reports on the role of psychosocial factors in the expression of SPD, there is a large body of literature documenting that psychosocial stressors influence the onset and course of schizophrenia. In par- ticular, stress (Ventura, Neuchterlein, Hardesty, & Gitlin, 1992) and other environmental factors, such as the amount of expressed emotion (i.e., critical comments, negative emotional overinvolvement) in families, have been found to exacerbate symptoms and precipitate relapse (Butzlaff & Hooley, 1998). More recent research suggests that characteristics of the patient’s illness, such as symptom severity and presence of negative symptoms, elicit more critical comments and greater emotional overinvolvement in mothers (King, 2000). Thus the relationship between negative expressed emotion in family members and poor outcome in patients appears to be bidirectional.
The Developmental Link Between
SPD and Schizophrenia ______________________________
SPD and schizophrenia also share a developmental link. Specifically, SPD signs, and even the full SPD syndrome, often predate the onset of schizo- phrenia. Retrospective and follow-back studies have demonstrated that most people who develop schizophrenia as adults experience social problems, such as having fewer friends and poor social adjustment (Done, Crow, Johnstone, & Sacker, 1994) well before manifesting the illness (Walker, Grimes, Davis, & Smith, 1993; Walker & Lewine, 1990). Further, prospec- tive research has shown that the transition rate to schizophrenia is much higher in SPD individuals than in mentally healthy controls and those with other psychological disorders.
Follow-back studies have revealed that, even in childhood, preschizophre- nia children, especially adolescents, are rated by their teachers as showing more social maladjustment (Done et al., 1994), less responsiveness in social sit- uations, and fewer instances of positive emotion (Walker et al., 1993; Walker & Lewine, 1990). In adolescence, there is typically a gradual increase in adjust- ment problems, including social withdrawal, irritability, negative affect, and noncompliance (Walker, Baum, & Diforio, 1998). These behaviors are part of the criteria for SPD and suggest the presence of subthreshold as well as clini- cally significant SPD prior to the development of schizophrenia.
  
Schizotypal Personality Disorder 93

Evidence of a developmental progression from SPD to schizophrenia is also provided by prospective studies. There has been a resurgence of interest in the clinical progression of SPD, with the goal of identifying more sensitive and spe- cific predictors of schizophrenia. This renewed emphasis on prediction stems, in part, from evidence that a longer duration of untreated psychosis leads to worse disease prognosis and course (McGlashan, 1999). Thus targeting vul- nerable individuals for intervention prior to the clinical manifestation of schiz- ophrenia may prevent illness onset or reduce severity. A substantial proportion of young adults who meet DSM criteria for SPD subsequently develop schizo- phrenia. Several recent short-term longitudinal studies have found that about 20% to 40% of young adults with SPD develop schizophrenia within 1 to 5 years (Miller, Byrne, et al., 2002; Yung et al., 1998). Thus the proportion of schizotypals who develop schizophrenia is high in light of the population base rate of schizophrenia, which is 1% to 2% (Gottesman, 1991).
In a study using the SIS, biological relatives of schizophrenia patients were assessed at 16 to 25 years of age, then followed over time to monitor their psychiatric status (Miller, McGlashan, et al., 2002). The goal was to deter- mine which symptom dimension ratings from the SIS best predicted clinical course. Within 3 years, 7/78 (9%) of the participants who had no Axis I dis- order at entry into the study developed a psychotic disorder. The best pre- dictor from the SIS was social withdrawal, such that 5/12 (42%) scoring above the cutoff on this scale developed a psychiatric illness and 64/66 (97%) of those below the cutoff remained healthy. Thus, social withdrawal showed reasonably high sensitivity and specificity for predicting psychotic outcome over a short period of time; these findings are particularly note- worthy because schizophrenia has a heterogeneous clinical picture that does not always include social withdrawal. In contrast, for odd behavior, there was high sensitivity but low specificity; 6/7 (86%) of the onset cases scored high, as did 28/71 (39%) of those who remained healthy. A combination of four factors—social withdrawal, psychotic-like symptoms, socioemotional dysfunction, and odd behavior—was the best predictor, with 6/9 (67%) of the participants who scored above the cutoff point falling ill and 68/69 (99%) of those scoring below it remaining well. This combination of signs and symptoms is consistent with a diagnosis of SPD.
Functional and Biological ______________ Parallels Between SPD and Schizophrenia
There is a vast literature documenting various cognitive and physical abnor- malities in patients with schizophrenia. To explore the validity of the SPD cri- teria and shed light on etiologic mechanisms, investigators have sought to determine whether similar abnormalities are present in SPD. The results of this research generally indicate that those who meet criteria for SPD show
  
94
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

functional deficits and physical abnormalities that parallel those observed in schizophrenia, although the magnitude of the deficits typically is less severe in SPD.
Cognitive Functioning
Cognitive impairment is well established in schizophrenia and involves generalized deficits, especially pronounced deficiencies in verbal memory, attention, and executive functions, such as vigilance, working memory, and planning (Cornblatt, Green, & Walker, 1999; Heinrichs & Zakzanis, 1998). Individuals who meet criteria for SPD show a similar pattern (Voglmaier, Seidman, Salisbury, & McCarley, 1997). For example, Voglmaier et al. (1997, 2000) found that unmedicated SPD patients manifested general cog- nitive impairment, including more pronounced deficits in verbal fluency, learning, and retention. Along the same lines, other investigators have shown that SPD is associated with poorer verbal and spatial working memory (Farmer et al., 2000; Harvey, Powchick, Mohs, & Davidson, 1995; Keefe et al., 1995; Park & McTigue, 1997; Park, Pueschel, Sauter, Rentsch, & Hell, 1999; Saykin et al., 1991).
In addition to impairments in verbal functions, adults who meet criteria for SPD show deficits in sustained attention (Condray & Steinhauer, 1992; Nuechterlein et al., 2002). Attentional deficits have also been found in col- lege students who score high on schizotypal traits (Lenzenweger, Cornblatt, & Putnick, 1991).
Findings have been mixed with respect to executive functioning. Some researchers have reported that individuals with SPD manifest deficits in aspects of executive functioning related to concept formation, abstract reasoning, and perseverative errors when compared to normal controls (Diforio, Walker & Kestler, 2000; Lyons, Merla, Young, & Kremen, 1991; Spaulding, Garbin, & Dras, 1989). In contrast, others have found that indi- viduals with subthreshold and clinical SPD perform similarly to normal controls on measures of executive functions that tap vigilance (Condray & Steinhauer, 1992; Raine et al., 1992). Executive functioning comprises sev- eral cognitive abilities (e.g., planning, sequencing, monitoring), and the dis- crepancy among these studies may be due to the use of different measures.
Motor and Physical Correlates of SPD
Consistent with the assumption that schizophrenia has neurodevelopmental origins, investigators have found physical correlates of the illness, including motor and dermatoglyphic abnormalities (i.e., abnormal patterns on the top layer of skin on the hands) and minor physical anomalies. Like schizophrenia patients (Cornblatt et al., 1999), individuals with SPD manifest abnormalities in motor functions. They show more abnormal spontaneous movements (Walker,
  
Schizotypal Personality Disorder 95

Lewis, Loewy, & Palvo, 1999) and less motor proficiency (Neumann & Walker, 1999) than non-SPD individuals. Recent investigations of SPD have also revealed an elevated rate of minor physical anomalies and dermatoglyphic abnormalities, namely, asymmetries in finger ridge counts (Weinstein, Diforio, Schiffman, Walker, & Bonsall, 1999). These results are consistent with the assumption that SPD and schizophrenia share an underlying neural substrate.
Psychophysiology
There is a substantial literature documenting abnormal psychophysiolog- ical responses in both schizophrenia and SPD. Parallel abnormalities have been found in prepulse inhibition, heart rate, electrodermal activity, and other autonomic nervous system–controlled responses.
Prepulse inhibition (PPI) of the startle response refers to the decrease in the startle response (measured with eye blink or skin conductance) to the onset of the startling stimulus (e.g., air puff, aversive noise) when it is preceded by the presentation of a “warning,” or prestimulus. PPI is pre- sumed to be a measure of sensorimotor gating—the ability to suppress, or gate, sensory, motor, or cognitive stimuli. Compared to normal and psy- chiatric controls, schizophrenia patients display less PPI of the startle response, suggesting they are hyperresponsive (Kumari, Soni, & Sharma, 1999; Weike, Bauer, & Hamm, 2000). Consistent with this pattern, schizotypal individuals show deficits in PPI when compared to healthy controls (Cadenhead, Geyer, & Braff, 1993). Along the same lines, P50 suppression paradigms assess the effectiveness of inhibition of the brain electrical response to the second of two auditory stimuli presented in rapid succession. The suppression of the second click is reduced in both schizo- phrenia (Clementz, Geyer, & Braff, 1997; Freedman et al., 1997) and SPD patients (Cadenhead, Light, Geyer, & Braff, 2000).
Reflexive eye movement responses are also aberrant in both schizophre- nia and SPD. Saccadic eye movement inhibition is measured with an anti- saccade task, in which the participant is asked to make a saccade in the opposite direction of a target. Investigations of this ability reveal that patients with schizophrenia (Fukushima, Fukushima, Morita, & Yamashita, 1990) and SPD (Ross et al., 1998) show a higher proportion of antisaccade errors (i.e., saccade toward the target rather than in the opposite direction as instructed) than normals. These findings suggest abnormal eye movement responses in these groups.
On measures of electrodermal activity, specifically skin conductance (SC), schizophrenia patients show a pattern characterized by extremes of either nonresponding or hyperresponding, with a significantly larger proportion of patients than normals being nonresponders (Bernstein et al., 1982; Dawson & Neuchterlein, 1987; Gruzelier & Raine, 1994). About 40% of schizo- phrenic participants show electrodermal nonresponding (Ohman, 1981), and this pattern has been linked to the presence of more negative symptoms
  
96
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

(Alm, Lindstrom, Ost, & Ohman, 1984; Bernstein et al., 1981). Results of SC investigations in SPD have been mixed, although most show differences between schizotypal and normal participants (Gruzelier & Raine, 1994; for a review, see Raine, Lencz, & Benishay, 1995). Most studies show a pattern marked by inconsistency of SC responding across trials in SPD participants, such that they show less SC responsivity on some trials and greater respon- sivity on others compared to normal controls (Wilkins, 1988).
In general, it appears that SPD is associated with abnormalities in PPI, P50, saccadic eye movements, and SC responding similar to those observed in schizophrenia. These findings provide additional support for the etiologic relation between the two disorders and indicate that central nervous system regulation is disrupted in both.
Structural Brain Abnormalities
Investigations of brain morphology in schizophrenia have revealed that patients show volumetric reductions in multiple brain regions, including the frontal and temporal lobes and hippocampus, as well as enlargement of the ventricles (Gur et al., 2000; Kumra et al., 2000; Lawrie & Abukmeil, 1998; Mitelman, Shihabuddin, Brickman, Hazlett, & Buchsbaum, 2003). There is support for similar, though less pronounced, structural abnormalities in schizotypal individuals.
For example, SPD patients have larger ventricular (i.e., cerebrospinal fluid) volumes, reduced cortical gray matter volumes (Dickey et al., 2000), and smaller temporal lobes compared to normal controls (Downhill et al., 2001), especially in superior temporal gyrus gray matter volume (Dickey et al., 1999). Other investigations of temporal lobe volume indicate that medial and inferior temporal gyrus volumes are reduced in SPD, as in schiz- ophrenia (Siever et al., 2000). However, schizophrenia patients show more severe medial temporal lobe abnormalities than SPD individuals (Pearlson et al., 1997). One study showed that SPD individuals demonstrated reduced gray matter volume in the prefrontal cortex compared to both psychiatric and normal control groups (Raine et al., 2002), although some have not replicated this finding (Siever et al., 2002).
In an investigation of ventricular volume in schizophrenia and SPD, schiz- ophrenia patients had the largest left anterior and temporal horns, followed by SPD patients, and then normal controls (Buchsbaum, Yang, et al., 1997). Thalamic volumes also are abnormal in schizophrenia and SPD. Decreased thalamic volumes in the pulvinar region have been reported in both groups (Byne et al., 2001). Additional reductions in the mediodorsal nucleus have been observed in schizophrenia patients, but not in SPD individuals (Siever et al., 2000). These results suggest that the severity of morphological brain abnormalities found in SPD falls between schizophrenia and normality.
Overall, these findings indicate that structural brain abnormalities are a feature associated with SPD. Morphological abnormalities are found in the frontal and temporal regions as well as in the thalamus and ventricles.
  
Schizotypal Personality Disorder 97

Abnormalities in brain morphology may have functional consequences. For example, there is substantial support for the idea that decreased hippocampal volume is related to poorer memory functioning (Walder, Walker, & Bollini, in press). Thus, brain morphological abnormalities linked with SPD may con- tribute to functional deficits. Furthermore, some studies indicate that SPD brain abnormalities are less pronounced than those observed in schizophre- nia. These findings lend further support to the notion that SPD is related to schizophrenia, but the degree of neuropathology appears to be attenuated.
Functional Brain Abnormalities
In addition to structural abnormalities, schizotypal individuals manifest functional brain abnormalities. However, these abnormalities do not con- sistently parallel those documented in schizophrenia. For example, in a positron-emission tomography (PET) study of SPD in which schizophrenia and normal groups performed a list-learning task, the SPD patients showed higher metabolic rates in the medial frontal and medial temporal areas than normals (Buchsbaum et al., 2002). They also manifested higher metabolic rates in Brodmann area 10 than both normals and unmedicated schizophre- nia patients. While performing this same task, SPD individuals displayed increased glucose metabolic rates in the ventral putamen, which suggests decreased dopaminergic activity in this area. However, schizophrenia patients displayed the opposite pattern—namely, decreased glucose meta- bolic rates—in this area compared to normal controls. This finding was interpreted as support for the notion of anomalous prefrontal cortex activ- ity in schizophrenia and SPD, which is diminished in schizophrenia and com- pensated for in SPD (Shihabuddin et al., 2001). In contrast to these findings, SPD and normal participants showed similar activation of the thalamic nuclei while performing an auditory learning task (Hazlett et al., 1999).
Studies of resting brain activity also suggest a distinction between SPD and schizophrenia. In an electroencephalogram (EEG) study of frontal activity in unmedicated schizophrenia, SPD, and depression patients, those with schizo- phrenia showed an increase in delta activity throughout the cortex, with greatest activity in the anterior cingulate gyrus and temporal lobes (i.e., the fusiform gyrus; Mientus et al., 2002). SPD and depressed individuals, in con- trast, showed significantly fewer delta waves in the anterior cingulate and right temporal lobe. Thus the schizophrenia and SPD groups showed differ- ent patterns of activation. In another EEG study, unmedicated schizophrenia patients showed increased delta activity in the frontal lobe while resting, whereas SPD patients did not show this abnormality (Wuebben & Winterer, 2001). Yet there is evidence that SPD and normal subjects differ in frontal activity during executive functioning tasks. In a single-photon emission com- puted tomography (SPECT) study of regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF), SPD individuals showed greatest activation in the middle frontal gyrus, whereas normal controls displayed mostly precentral gyrus activation (Buchsbaum, Trestman, et al., 1997).
  
98
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

When compared to normals, schizotypal individuals show abnormal brain activation, particularly in the frontal and temporal brain regions, while resting and during some cognitive tasks. Schizophrenia patients show abnor- mal activation in similar brain regions; however, the nature of their dys- functional patterns appears qualitatively different. This finding may reflect differences in the neural bases of the disorder, but it also is possible that antipsychotic medication contributes to these differences. Furthermore, it has been found that decreased or atypical activation of brain circuitry dur- ing cognitive performance is related to performance deficits (Heckers et al., 1999). Thus abnormal activation patterns may have functional conse- quences, and these patterns may contribute to the cognitive deficits charac- teristic of SPD and schizophrenia.
Neurochemistry
Neurochemical abnormalities have been central to hypotheses about the neuropathophysiology of schizophrenia. Among the neurotransmitters, dopamine has received the most attention for its relation to psychotic symp- tomatology. Researchers have found higher concentrations of dopamine and homovanillic acid (HVA), a dopamine metabolite, in schizophrenia patients’ brains (Davis, Kahn, Ko, & Davidson, 1991), and a relation between these higher concentrations and more severe psychotic symptoms (Davis et al., 1985; Davis et al., 1991). Similarly, there is some evidence that SPD indi- viduals show elevated cerebrospinal fluid HVA concentrations compared to individuals with other personality disorders (Siever et al., 1993). Furthermore, Siever and his colleagues (1993) reported a positive relation between these concentrations and psychotic-like schizotypal symptoms. These findings provide evidence of elevated dopamine in SPD that mimics the pattern found in schizophrenia and lends further support to the notion that the two illnesses are neurochemically related.
Given the assumption that stress can exacerbate schizophrenia symptoms, investigators have examined biological indicators of the stress response. Secretion of the neurohormone cortisol increases in response to stress. There is substantial evidence that, compared to healthy controls, schizophrenia patients show increased baseline and post-dexamethasone cortisol release (Altamura, Guercetti, & Percudani, 1989; Copolov et al., 1989; Walker & Diforio, 1997). Along the same lines, there is evidence of heightened cortisol in schizotypal individuals. SPD adolescents manifest elevated salivary cortisol levels com- pared to normal controls (Walker, Walder, & Reynolds, 2001; Weinstein et al., 1999). Interestingly, higher cortisol levels in SPD individuals have been linked with more motor abnormalities (Neumann & Walker, 1999) and more severe positive symptoms (Diforio, 1999). These findings suggest hyperactivity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, the neural system that gov- erns cortisol release, in both schizophrenia and SPD. This may be the neural
  
Schizotypal Personality Disorder 99

mechanism that mediates the heightened sensitivity to stress that is assumed to be linked with risk for schizophrenia (Walker & Diforio, 1997).
____________________________________ Treatment of SPD
There is a dearth of literature on treatments for SPD. Both psychopharmacology and psychotherapy are currently used to treat schizotypal individuals who come into contact with health care professionals and are distressed to the degree that they require intervention. To date, there are no known published reports of con- trolled studies that have examined the effect of psychotherapy on schizotypal symptoms. In the absence of guiding research, therapeutic approaches focus on providing support, social skills training, and psychoeducation (Stone, 1985). Interestingly, the schizophrenia literature is increasingly showing that cognitive- behavioral therapy (CBT) is relatively efficacious for treating some aspects of schizophrenia, including positive symptoms and depression (Turkington, Dudley, Warman, & Beck, 2004). Given the biological and developmental con- nection between schizophrenia and SPD and the fact that SPD symptoms appear to be an attenuated version of the Axis I disorder, it is likely that CBT also would be beneficial for treating SPD. Future studies should examine the effects of CBT on SPD symptoms. At this point, however, there are no empirical findings that implicate one therapeutic approach over another for treating SPD.
Similarly, few reports have been published on the efficacy of psychophar- macological treatments for SPD. The pharmacological approach to date has been use of atypical antipsychotic medication, which is the treatment of choice for schizophrenia. In a recent review of psychopharmacological treat- ments in personality disorders, Markovitz (2004) reported that only a few studies have examined antipsychotic medication effects in a comorbid schizotypal and borderline personality disordered group and have found SPD symptom improvement. Only one known study to date has examined these effects in a predominantly SPD group; the research team found signif- icantly reduced positive and negative symptoms of SPD within the first 2 months of atypical antipsychotic medication treatment (Koenigsberg et al., 2003). Thus there is modest empirical support for the use of atypical antipsy- chotics for treating SPD.
___________________________ Summary and Conclusions
Since its formal inception as a diagnostic entity in 1979, SPD has been the subject of extensive research. We now have evidence indicating that it is a valid and reliable syndrome. It is clear that it has genetic links with schizo- phrenia and is often a precursor of schizophrenia and other psychotic disor- ders. Findings from twin studies suggest that schizophrenia and SPD can be alternative expressions of the same polygenotype. An important implication
   
100
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

of this idea is that environmental factors are likely playing a role in deter- mining the expression of the constitutional vulnerability to schizophrenia.
Consistent with the findings on genetics, SPD is often accompanied by some of the same functional deficits and biological abnormalities observed in schiz- ophrenia patients. Thus, both disorders appear to involve brain morphologi- cal abnormalities and dysfunction. Nonetheless, there are some differences between schizophrenia and SPD, especially in the area of brain activation. These differences are important because (1) they may hold clues about the neu- ropathophysiology of schizophrenia and (2) they may inform us about the nature of the environmental factors capable of influencing the phenotypic expression of the polygenotype(s) associated with risk for schizophrenia.
It is expected that the pace of research on SPD will increase more rapidly in the near future. The chief reason for the expected increase is the evidence that SPD is one of the best known predictors of schizophrenia. Consequently, researchers are initiating longitudinal studies of SPD to identify which clini- cal features, in combination with other variables, yield the greatest predictive power. Once the sensitivity and specificity of prediction are adequate to jus- tify preventive intervention, it is expected that clinical trials aimed at pre- venting the transition from SPD to schizophrenia will ensue (McGlashan, 2001). It is most likely that medication will be the chief intervention strategy for SPD, although psychotherapeutic interventions may also be examined. Nevertheless, some have noted that SPD is a clinical condition that should be treated independent of its association with the Axis I disorder. In cases in which the individual is experiencing distress as a consequence of the symp- toms of SPD, it is reasonable to argue that treatment is indicated. Thus another noteworthy future trend will be research on the treatment of SPD.
References _________________________________________
Alm, T., Lindstrom, L., Ost, L.-G., & Ohman, A. (1984). Electrodermal nonrespond- ing in schizophrenia: Relationships to attentional, clinical, biochemical, computed tomographical and genetic factors. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 1, 195–208.
Altamura, C., Guercetti, G., & Percudani, M. (1989). Dexamethasone suppression test in positive and negative schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 30, 69–75. American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (3rd ed., revised). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of men-
tal disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of men-
tal disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.
Bakan, P., & Peterson, K. (1994). Pregnancy and birth complications: A risk factor
 
for schizotypy. Journal of Personality Disorders, 8, 299–306.
Schizotypal Personality Disorder 101

Baron, M., Gruen, R. S., & Romo-Gruen, J. M. (1992). Positive and negative symp- toms: Relation to familial transmission of schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 610–614.
Battaglia, M., Bernardeschi, L., Franchini, L., Bellodi, L., & Smeraldi, E. (1995). A family study of schizotypal disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 21, 33–45.
Bernstein, A. S., Frith, C. D., Gruzelier, J. H., Patterson, T., Straube, E., Venables, P. H., et al. (1982). An analysis of the skin conductance orienting response in samples of American, British, and German schizophrenics. Biological Psychiatry, 14, 155–211.
Bernstein, A. S., Taylor, K. W., Starkey, P., Juni, S., Lubowsky, J., & Paley, H. (1981). Bilateral skin conductance, finger pulse volume, and EEG orienting response to tones of differing intensities in chronic schizophrenics and controls. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 169, 513–528.
Bleuler, E. (1924). Textbook of psychiatry (A. A. Brill, Trans.). New York: Macmillan. Bleuler, E. (1969). Lehrbuch der Psychiatrie, 11 Auflage; umgearbeitet von M Bleuler.
Berlin, Germany: Springer.
Buchsbaum, M. S., Nenadic, I., Hazlett, E. A., Spiegal-Cohen, J., Fleischman, M. B.,
Akhavan, A., et al. (2002). Differential metabolic rates in prefrontal and tem- poral Brodmann areas in schizophrenia and schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophrenia Research, 54, 141–150.
Buchsbaum, M. S., Trestman, R. L., Hazlett, E., Seigel, B. V., Jr., Schafer, C. H., Luu- Hsia, C., et al. (1997). Regional cerebral blood flow during the Wisconsin Card Sort Test in schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophrenia Research, 27, 21–28.
Buchsbaum, M. S., Yang, S., Hazlett, E., Siegel, B. V., Jr., Germans, M., Haznedar, M., et al. (1997). Ventricular volume and asymmetry in schizotypal personality disorder and schizophrenia assessed with magnetic resonance imaging. Schizophrenia Research, 27, 45–53.
Butzlaff, R. L., & Hooley, J. M. (1998). Expressed emotion and psychiatric relapse. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55(6), 547–552.
Byne, W., Buchsbaum, M. S., Kemether, E., Hazlett, E. A., Shinwari, A., Mitropoulou, V., et al. (2001). Magnetic resonance imaging of the thalamic mediodorsal nucleus and pulvinar in schizophrenia and schizotypal personality disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 133–140.
Cadenhead, K. S., Geyer, M. A., & Braff, D. (1993). Impaired startle prepulse inhi- bition and habituation in schizotypal personality disordered patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1862–1867.
Cadenhead, K. S., Light, G. A., Geyer, M. A., & Braff, D. L. (2000). Sensory gating deficits assessed by the P50 event-related potential in subjects with schizotypal personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 55–59.
Cannon, T. D., Barr, C. E., & Mednick, S. A. (1991). Genetic and perinatal factors in the etiology of schizophrenia. In E. F. Walker (Ed.), Schizophrenia: A life- course developmental perspective (pp. 9–31). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Chapman, L. J., & Chapman, J. P. (1980). Scales for rating psychotic and psychotic- like experiences as continua. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 6, 476–489.
Chapman, L. J., Chapman, J. P., Kwapil, T. R., Eckblad, M., & Zinser, M. C. (1994). Putatively psychosis-prone subjects 10 years later. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 171–183.
Chapman, L. J., Chapman, J. P., & Miller, E. N. (1982). Reliabilities and intercor- relations of eight measures of proneness to psychosis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 187–195.

102
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Chapman, L. J., Chapman, J. P., & Raulin, M. L. (1976). Scales for physical and social anhedonia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85, 374–382.
Claridge, G., McCreery, C., Mason, O., Bentall, R., Boyle, G., & Slade, P. (1996). The factor structure of “schizotypal” traits: A large replication study. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 35, 103–115.
Clementz, B., Geyer, M., & Braff, D. (1997). P50 suppression among schizophrenia patients and normal comparison subjects: A methodological analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 41, 1035–1044.
Condray, R., & Steinhauer, S. R. (1992). Schizotypal personality disorder in indi- viduals with and without schizophrenic relatives: Similarities and contrasts in neurocognitive and clinical functioning. Schizophrenia Research, 7, 33–41.
Copolov, D. L., Rubin, R. T., Stuart, G. W., Poland, R. E., Mander, A. J., Sashidharan, S. P., et al. (1989). Specificity of the salivary cortisol dexamethasone suppression test across psychiatric diagnoses. Biological Psychiatry, 25, 879–893.
Cornblatt, B. A., Green, M. F., & Walker, E. F. (1999). Schizophrenia: Etiology and neurocognition. In T. Millon (Series Ed.) & P. H. Blaney & R. D. Davis (Vol. Eds.), Oxford textbooks in clinical psychology: Vol. 4. Oxford textbook of psychopathology (pp. 277–310). London: Oxford University Press.
Davis, K. L., Davidson, M., Mohs, R. C., Kendler, K. S., Davis, B. M., Johns, C. A., et al. (1985). Plasma homovanillic acid concentration and the severity of schiz- ophrenic illness. Science, 227(4694), 1601–1602.
Davis, K. L., Kahn, R. S., Ko, G., & Davidson, M. (1991). Dopamine in schizo- phrenia: A review and reconceptualization. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 1474–1486.
Dawson, M. E., & Neuchterlein, K. H. (1987). The role of autonomic dysfunctions within a vulnerability-stress model of schizophrenic disorders. In D. Magnusson & A. Ohman (Eds.), Psychopathology: An interactional perspective (pp. 41–58). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Dickey, C. C., McCarley, R. W., Voglmaier, M. M., Niznikiewicz, M. A., Seidman, L. J., Hirayasu, Y., et al. (1999). Schizotypal personality disorder and MRI abnor- malities of temporal lobe gray matter. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 1392–1402.
Dickey, C. C., Shenton, M. E., Hirayasu, Y., Fischer, I., Vogimaier, M. M., Niznikiewicz, M. A., et al. (2000). Large CSF volume not attributable to ventricular volume in schizotypal personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 48–54.
Diforio, D. R. (1999). Cortisol secretion and executive functioning as predictors of symptom development in adolescents at-risk for schizophrenia. Dissertation Abstracts International, 59, 8B. (UMI No. 4458)
Diforio, D., Walker, E. F., & Kestler, L. P. (2000). Executive functions in adolescents with schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophrenia Research, 42, 125–134.
Done, D. J., Crow, T. J., Johnstone, E. C., & Sacker, A. (1994). Childhood antecedents of schizophrenia and affective illness: Social adjustment at ages 7 and 11. British Medical Journal, 309, 699–703.
Downhill, J. E., Jr., Buchsbaum, M. S., Hazlett, E. A., Barth, S., Roitman, S. L., Nunn, M., et al. (2001). Temporal lobe volume determined by magnetic resonance imag- ing in schizotypal personality disorder and schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 48, 187–199.
Dworkin, R. H., & Lenzenweger, M. F. (1984). Symptoms and the genetics of schiz- ophrenia: Implications for diagnosis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 1541–1546.
Faraone, S. V., Green, A. I., Seidman, L. J., & Tsuang, M. T. (2001). Schizotaxia: Clinical implications and new directions for research. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 27, 1–18.
Schizotypal Personality Disorder 103

Farmer, A. E., McGuffin, P., & Gottesman, I. I. (1987). Twin concordance for DSM- III schizophrenia: Scrutinizing the validity of the definition. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 634–641.
Farmer, C. M., O’Donnell, B. F., Niznikiewicz, M. A., Voglmaier, M. M., McCarley, R. W., & Shenton, M. E. (2000). Visual perception and working memory in schizotypal personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 781–786.
First, M. B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., & Benjamin, L. S. (1997). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis II personality disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Freedman, B., & Chapman, L. J. (1973). Early subjective experiences in schizo- phrenic episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 82, 46–54.
Freedman, R., Coon, H., Myles-Worsley, M., Orr-Urtreger, A., Olincy, A., Davis, A., et al. (1997). Linkage of a neurophysiological deficit in schizophrenia to a chro- mosome 15 locus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 94, 587–592.
Fukushima, J., Fukushima, K., Morita, N., & Yamashita, I. (1990). Further analysis of the control of voluntary saccadic eye movements in schizophrenic patients. Biological Psychiatry, 28, 943–958.
Gershon, E. S., DeLisi, L. E., Hamovit, J., & Nurnberger, J. I. (1988). A controlled family study of chronic psychoses: Schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 45, 328–336.
Gottesman, I. (1991). Schizophrenia genesis: The origins of madness. New York: Freeman. Gruzelier, J., & Raine, A. (1994). Bilateral electrodermal activity and cerebral mecha- nisms in syndromes of schizophrenia and the schizotypal personality. International
Journal of Psychophysiology, 16, 1–16.
Gur, R. E., Cowell, P. E., Latshaw, A., Turetsky, B. I., Grossman, R. I., Arnold, S.
E., et al. (2000). Reduced dorsal and orbital prefrontal gray matter volumes in
schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 761–768.
Harvey, P. D., Powchick, P., Mohs, R. C., & Davidson, M. (1995). Memory func- tions in geriatric chronic schizophrenic patients: A neuropsychological study.
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 7, 207–212. Hazlett, E. A., Buchsbaum, M. S., Byne, W., Wei, T-C., Spiegel-Cohen, J., Geneve, C., et al. (1999). Three-dimensional analysis with MRI and PET of the size, shape, and function of the thalamus in the schizophrenia spectrum. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 1190–1199.
Heckers, S., Goff, D., Schacter, D. L., Savage, C. R., Fischman, A. J., Alpert, N. M.,
et al. (1999). Functional imaging of memory retrieval in deficit vs. nondeficit
schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 1117–1123.
Heinrichs, R. W., & Zakzanis, K. K. (1998). Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia:
A quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology, 12, 426–445.
Jones, L. A., Cardno, A. G., Murphy, K. C., Sanders, R. D., Gray, M. Y., McCarthy, G., et al. (2000). The Kings Schizotypy Questionnaire as a quantitative measure
of schizophrenia liability. Schizophrenia Research, 45, 213–221.
Keefe, R. S. E., Roitman, S. E., Harvey, P. D., Blum, C. S., Dupre, R. L., Davidson, M., et al. (1995). A pen-and-paper human analogue of a monkey prefrontal cor- tex activation task: Spatial working memory in patients with schizophrenia.
Schizophrenia Research, 17, 25–35.
Kendler, K. S., & Gardner, C. O. (1997). The risk for psychiatric disorders in rela-
tives of schizophrenic and control probands: A comparison of three independent
studies. Psychological Medicine, 27, 411–419.
Kendler, K. S., & Gruenberg, A. M. (1984). An independent analysis of the Danish
adoption study of schizophrenia: VI. The relationship between psychiatric
104
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

disorders as defined by DSM-III in relatives and adoptees. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 41, 555–564.
Kendler, K. S., Gruenberg, A. M., & Strauss, J. S. (1981). An independent analysis
of the Copenhagen sample of the Danish Adoption Study of Schizophrenia.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 982–984.
Kendler, K. S., & Lister-Sharp, D. J. (1989). The Structured Interview for Schizotypy
(SIS): A preliminary report. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 15, 559–571.
Kety, S. S., Rosenthal, D., Wender, P. H., & Schulsinger, F. (1968). The types and prevalence of mental illness in the biological and adoptive families of adopted
schizophrenics. Journal of Psychiatry Research, 6(Suppl. 1), 345–362.
Kety, S. S., Rosenthal, D., Wender, P. H., Schulsinger, F., & Jacobsen, B. (1976). Mental illness in the biological and adoptive families of adopted individuals who
have become schizophrenic. Behavior Genetics, 6, 219–225.
King, S. (2000). Is expressed emotion cause or effect in the mothers of schizophrenic
young adults? Schizophrenia Research, 45(1–2), 65–78.
Koenigsberg, H. W., Reynolds, D., Goodman, M., New, A. S., Mitropoulou, V.,
Trestman, R. L., et al. (2003). Risperidone in the treatment of schizotypal per-
sonality disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 64(6), 628–634.
Kraepelin, E. (1919/1971). Dementia praecox and paraphrenia (G. M. Robertson,
Ed.; R. M. Barclay, Trans.). New York: Krieger.
Kremen, W. S., Buka, S. L., Seidman, L. J., Goldstein, J. M., Koren, D., & Tsuang,
M. T. (1998). IQ decline during childhood and adult psychotic symptoms in a community sample: A 19-year longitudinal study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 672–677.
Kumari, V., Soni, W., & Sharma, T. (1999). Normalization of information process- ing deficits in schizophrenia with clozapine. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 1046–1051.
Kumra, S., Giedd, J. N., Vaituzis, A. C., Jacobsen, L. K., McKenna, K., Bedwell, J., et al. (2000). Childhood-onset psychotic disorders: Magnetic resonance imaging of volumetric differences in brain structure. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1467–1474.
Lawrie, S. M., & Abukmeil, S. S. (1998). Brain abnormality in schizophrenia: A sys- tematic and quantitative review of volumetric magnetic resonance imaging studies. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 110–120.
Lenzenweger, M. F. (1999). Schizotypic psychopathology: Theory, evidence, and future directions. In T. Millon (Series Ed.) & P. H. Blaney & R. D. Davis (Vol. Eds.), Oxford textbooks in clinical psychology: Vol. 4. Oxford textbook of psy- chopathology (pp. 605–627). London: Oxford University Press.
Lenzenweger, M. F., Cornblatt, B. A., & Putnick, M. (1991). Schizotypy and sus- tained attention. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 84–89.
Loranger, A. (1990). The impact of DSM-III on diagnostic practice in a university hospital: A comparison of DSM-II and DSM-III in 10,914 patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47, 672–675.
Lyons, M. J., Merla, M. E., Young, L., & Kremen, W. S. (1991). Impaired neuropsy- chological functioning in symptomatic volunteers with schizotypy: Preliminary findings. Biological Psychiatry, 30, 424–426.
MacDonald, A. W., Pogue-Geile, M. F., Debski, T. T., & Manuck, S. (2001). Genetic and environmental influences on schizotypy: A community-based twin study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 27, 47–58.
Schizotypal Personality Disorder 105

Maier, W., Lichtermann, D. Klingler, T., Heun, R., & Hallmayer, J. (1992). Prevalences of personality disorders (DSM-III-R) in the community. Journal of Personality Disorders, 6, 187–192.
Markovitz, P. J. (2004). Recent trends in the pharmacotherapy of personality disor- ders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 18(1), 90–101.
McGlashan, T. H. (1999). Duration of untreated psychosis in first-episode schizo- phrenia: Marker or determinant of course? Biological Psychiatry, 46, 899–907. McGlashan, T. H. (2001). Psychosis treatment prior to psychosis onset: Ethical
issues. Schizophrenia Research, 51, 47–54.
Meehl, P. E. (1962). Schizotaxia, schizotypy, schizophrenia. American Psychologist,
17, 827–838.
Meehl, P. E. (1973a). Psychodiagnosis: Selected papers. New York: Norton.
Meehl, P. E. (1973b). Some methodological reflections on the difficulties of psychoan-
alytic research. Psychological Issues Monographs, 8(2, Monograph 30), 104–117. Mientus, S., Gallinat, J., Wuebben, Y., Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Mulert, C., Frick, K., et al. (2002). Cortical hypoactivation during resting EEG in schizophrenics but not in depressives and schizotypal subjects as revealed by low resolution electromag- netic tomography (LORETA). Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 116, 95–111. Miller, P., Byrne, M., Hodges, A., Lawrie, S. M., Owens, D. G. C., & Johnstone, E. C. (2002). Schizotypal components in people at high risk of developing schiz- ophrenia: Early findings from the Edinburgh high-risk study. British Journal
of Psychiatry, 180, 179–184.
Miller, T. J., McGlashan, T. H., Rosen, J. L., Somjee, L., Markovich, P. J., Stein, K.,
et al. (2002). Prospective diagnosis of the initial prodrome for schizophrenia based on the Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes: Preliminary evidence of interrater reliability and predictive validity. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 863–865.
Mitelman, S. A., Shihabuddin, L., Brickman, A. M., Hazlett, E. A., & Buchsbaum, M. S. (2003). MRI assessment of gray and white matter distribution in Brodmann’s areas of the cortex in patients with schizophrenia with good and poor outcomes. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 2154–2168.
Neumann, C. S., & Walker, E. F. (1999). Motor dysfunction in schizotypal person- ality disorder. Schizophrenia Research, 38, 159–168.
Nuechterlein, K. H., Asarnow, R. F., Subotnik, K. L., Fogelson, D. L., Payne, D. L., Kendler, K. S., et al. (2002). The structure of schizotypy: Relations between neu- rocognitive and personality disorder features in relatives of schizophrenic patients in the UCLA Family Study. Schizophrenia Research, 54, 121–130.
Ohman, A. (1981). Electrodermal activity and vulnerability to schizophrenia: A review. Biological Psychology, 12, 87–145.
Park, S., & McTigue, K. (1997). Working memory and the syndromes of schizotypal personality. Schizophrenia Research, 26, 213–220.
Park, S., Pueschel, J., Sauter, B. H., Rentsch, M., & Hell, D. (1999). Spatial working memory deficits and clinical symptoms in schizophrenia: A 4-month follow-up study. Biological Psychiatry, 46, 392–400.
Pearlson, G., Barta, P., Powers, R., Memon, R., Richards, S., Aylward, E., et al. (1997). Medial and superior temporal gyral volumes and cerebral asymmetry in schizophrenia versus bipolar disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 41, 1–14.
Pfohl, B., Blum, N., & Zimmerman, M. (1994). Structured interview for DSM–IV personality disorders. Iowa City: University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics.
  
106
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Raine, A. (1991). The SPQ: A scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 17, 555–564.
Raine, A., Lencz, T., & Benishay, D. S. (1995). Schizotypal personality and skin con- ductance orienting. In A. Raine, T. Lencz, & S. A. Mednick (Eds.), Schizotypal personality (pp. 219–249). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Raine, A., Lencz, T., Yaralian, P., Bihrle, S., LaCasse, L., Ventura, J., et al. (2002). Prefrontal structural and functional deficits in schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 28, 501–513.
Raine, A., Reynolds, C., Lencz, T., Scerbo, A., Triphon, N., & Kim, D. (1994). Cognitive-perceptual, interpersonal, and disorganized features of schizotypal personality. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 20, 191–201.
Raine, A., Triphon, N., Kim, D., Hesler, A., Bird, L., Lencz, T., et al. (1992).
Schizotypal personality: Factor structure, sex differences, psychiatric differences, genetics, psychophysiology, and neuropsychology. Paper presented at the 1992 Western Psychological Association meeting, Portland, OR.
Risch, N., & Baron, M. (1984). Segregation analysis of schizophrenia and related disorders. American Journal of Human Genetics, 36, 1039–1059.
Ross, R. G., Harris, J. G., Olincy, A., Radant, A., Adler, L. E., & Freedman, R. (1998). Familial transmission of two independent saccadic abnormalities in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 30, 59–70.
Saykin, A. J., Gur, R. C., Gur, R. E., Mozley, P. D., Mozley, L. H., Resnick, S. M., et al. (1991). Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia: Selective impair- ment in memory and learning. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 618–624.
Schulsinger, F., Parnas, J., Mednick, S., Teasdale, T. W., & Schulsinger, H. (1987). Heredity-environment interaction and schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 21, 431–436.
Shihabuddin, L., Buchsbaum, M. S., Hazlett, E. A., Silverman, J., New, A., Brickman, A. M., et al. (2001). Striatal size and glucose metabolic rate in schizo- typal personality disorder and schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 877–884.
Siever, L. J., Amin, F., Coccaro, E. F., Trestman, R., Silverman, J., Horvath, T. B., et al. (1993). CSF homovanillic acid in schizotypal personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 149–151.
Siever, L. J., Buchsbaum, M. S., Shihabuddin, L., Downhill, J., Byne, W. M., & Hazlett, E. A. (2000). Cortical and subcortical volumes in patients with schizo- typal personality disorder [Abstract]. Biological Psychiatry, 47, 120S.
Siever, L. J., Koenigsberg, H. W., Harvey, P., Mitropoulou, V., Laruelle, M., Abi- Dargham, A., et al. (2002). Cognitive and brain function in schizotypal person- ality disorder. Schizophrenia Research, 54, 157–167.
Siever, L. J., Silverman, J. M., Horvath, T. B., Howard, K., Coccaro, E., Keefe, R. S. E., et al. (1990). Increased morbid risk for schizophrenia-related disorders in relatives of schizotypal personality disordered patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47, 634–640.
Spaulding, W., Garbin, C. P., & Dras, S. R. (1989). Cognitive abnormalities in schiz- ophrenic patients and schizotypal college students. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 177, 717–728.
Spitzer, R. L., Endicott, J., & Gibbon, M. (1979). Crossing the border into border- line personality and borderline schizophrenia: The development of criteria. Archives of General Psychiatry, 36, 17–24.
Schizotypal Personality Disorder 107

Stone, M. (1985). Schizotypal personality: Psychotherapeutic aspects. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 11, 576–589.
Torgersen, S. (1984). Genetic and nosological aspects of schizotypal and borderline personality disorders: A twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41, 546–554.
Torgersen, S. (1994). Personality deviations within the schizophrenia spectrum. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 384(Suppl.), 40–44.
Torgersen, S., & Kringlen, E. (1991). Twin concordance for DSM-III-R schizophre- nia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 83, 395–401.
Turkington, D., Dudley, R., Warman, D. M., & Beck, A. T. (2004). Cognitive- behavioral therapy for schizophrenia: A review. Journal of Psychiatric Practice, 10(1), 5–16.
Ventura, J., Nuechterlein, K. H., Hardesty, J. P., & Gitlin, M. (1992). Life events and schizophrenic relapse after withdrawal of medication. British Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 615–620.
Voglmaier, M. M., Seidman, L. J., Niznikiewicz, M. A., Kickey, C. C., Shenton, M. E., & McCarley, R. W. (2000). Verbal and nonverbal neuropsychological test performance in subjects with schizotypal personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 787–793.
Voglmaier, M. M., Seidman, L. J., Salisbury, D., & McCarley, R. W. (1997). Neuropsychological dysfunction in schizotypal personality disorder: A profile analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 41, 530–540.
Vollema, M. G., & Hoijtink, H. (2000). The multidimensionality of self-report schizotypy in a psychiatric population: An analysis using multidimensional Rasch models. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 26, 565–575.
Vollema, M. G., & Ormel, J. (2000). The reliability of the Structured Interview for Schizotypy—Revised. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 26, 619–629.
Walder, D. J., Walker, E. F., & Bollini, A. M. (in press). The hippocampus, hypo- thalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, cognition and symptoms in schizophrenia. In D. Barch (Ed.), Cognitive and affective neuroscience of psychopathology. New York: Oxford University Press.
Walker, E., Grimes, K., Davis, D., & Smith, A. (1993). Childhood precursors of schizophrenia: Facial expressions of emotion. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1654–1660.
Walker, E., Lewis, N., Loewy, R., & Palvo, S. (1999). Motor dysfunction and risk for schizophrenia. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 509–523.
Walker, E. F., Baum, K. M., & Diforio, D. (1998). Developmental changes in the behavioral expression of vulnerability for schizophrenia. In M. F. Lenzenweger & R. H. Dworkin (Eds.), Origins and development of schizophrenia: Advances in experimental psychopathology (pp. 469–491). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Walker, E. F., & Diforio, D. (1997). Schizophrenia: A neural diathesis-stress model. Psychological Review, 104, 667–685.
Walker, E. F., & Lewine, R. J. (1990). Prediction of adult-onset schizophrenia from childhood home movies of the patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 1052–1056.
Walker, E. F., Walder, D. J., & Reynolds, F. (2001). Developmental changes in cor- tisol secretion in normal and at-risk youth. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 721–732.
  
108
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Weike, A. I., Bauer, U., & Hamm, A. O. (2000). Effective neuroleptic medication removes prepulse inhibition deficits in schizophrenia patients. Biological Psychiatry, 47, 61–70.
Weinstein, D. D., Diforio, D., Schiffman, J., Walker, E., & Bonsall, R. (1999). Minor physical anomalies, dermatoglyphic asymmetries, and cortisol levels in adoles- cents with schizotypal personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 617–623.
Wilkins, S. (1988). Behavioral and psychophysiological aspects of information pro- cessing in schizotypics. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of York, UK. Williams, M. (1993). The psychometric assessment of schizotypal personality.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of London.
Wuebben, Y., & Winterer, G. (2001). Hypofrontality: A risk-marker related to schiz-
ophrenia? Schizophrenia Research, 48, 207–217.
Yaralian, P. S., Raine, A., Lencz, T., Hooley, J. M., Bihrle, S. E., Mills, S., et al. (2000).
Elevated levels of cognitive-perceptual deficits in individuals with a family history
of schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophrenia Research, 46, 57–63.
Yung, A. R., Phillips, L. J., McGorry, P. D., McFarlane, C. A., Francey, S., Harrigan, S., et al. (1998). Prediction of psychosis: A step towards indicated prevention of
schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172(Suppl. 33), 14–20. Zimmermann, M., & Coryell, W. (1990). Diagnosing personality disorders in the community: A comparison of self-report and interview measures. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 47, 527–531.

Antisocial 6 Personality Disorder and Psychopathy
Christopher J. Patrick University of Minnesota

Among the various disorders of personality, antisocial personality disorder (APD) and the related syndrome of psychopathy stand as unique in terms of the toll they exact on society. As a function of this impact, these syndromes have long held the fascination of the public at large as well as that of investigators in the scientific community. The goal of this chapter is to review, in a relatively brief but at the same time broad and integrative fashion, what is currently known about these disorders. In doing so, I will highlight overlapping elements of these clinical phenomena as well as impor- tant elements that differentiate them. In addition, I will endeavor to link these phenomena to broader constructs in the domains of personality and psychopathology, and in turn to underlying psychological processes that may provide the basis for a clearer, mechanistic understanding of the unique and distinctive features of these clinical syndromes.
The chapter begins with a historical overview of the constructs of psy- chopathy and antisocial personality. Highlighted in this section is the fact that although the APD construct has achieved prominence in the psychiatric liter- ature during the past 25 years, the construct of psychopathy actually pre- ceded it historically and—from the perspective to be advanced here—can be considered an overarching construct that incorporates APD. Following the historical overview, I provide a summary of concepts and recent empirical findings regarding the APD construct. The emphasis is on placing APD into
Author’s note: Preparation of this chapter was supported by grants MH52384, MH65137, and MH072850 from the National Institute of Mental Health, grant R01 AA12164 from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, and funds from the Hathaway endowment at the University of Minnesota.
109
110
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

a broader conceptual framework that encompasses other impulse control disorders (e.g., conduct disorder, alcohol dependence, and drug dependence) with which APD is closely associated. Specifically, drawing on recent research findings, I will argue that the adult antisocial deviance embodied in the APD construct represents one manifestation of a broader disposition toward disin- hibitory (externalizing) problems that can be viewed as more basic from an etiologic standpoint. The next section of the chapter is devoted to contem- porary conceptualizations of psychopathy and relevant empirical findings. Special emphasis is devoted to the idea of psychopathy as a fundamentally variegated construct, encompassing somewhat paradoxical elements of behavioral pathology on one hand and positive psychological adjustment on the other. The last major section of the chapter considers relations between the constructs of psychopathy and APD from the standpoint of a dual-process model of the etiologic mechanisms underlying these disorders. Following this, I include a brief section on the treatment of these disorders.
Historical Overview of the
Psychopathy and APD Constructs _____________________
Early Conceptualizations of Psychopathy
The earliest accounts of the syndrome that came to be known as psy- chopathy emphasized extreme behavioral deviance in the context of intact reasoning and communicative abilities. Over 200 years ago, Philippe Pinel (1801/1962) documented examples of patients “who at no period gave evi- dence of any lesion of understanding, but who were under the dominion of instinctive and abstract fury, as if the faculties of affect alone had sustained injury” (p. 9). Patients of this kind engaged repeatedly in impulsive acts inju- rious to themselves and others despite recognizing at a verbal/conceptual level the irrationality of such acts. The label Pinel applied to this syndrome was manie sans delire (“insanity without delirium”). As indicated in the fore- going quotation, Pinel hypothesized that the underlying impairment in such cases was an inability to control emotion (affect) as opposed to some deficit in reason or understanding.
Around the same time, the American physician Benjamin Rush (1812) made note of similar cases, but postulated moral weakness (i.e., an impair- ment in the capacity to experience shame or guilt in relation to contemplated actions and their potential consequences) as the root cause. In contrast with Pinel’s notion of a defect in affective control, Rush’s etiologic perspective car- ried with it a social-evaluative component (i.e., individuals of this sort do bad things because they are morally deranged). In his account of the syndrome, Rush also highlighted the manipulative, deceitful nature of such individuals. A perspective similar to Rush’s was advanced by the British psychiatric expert J. C. Pritchard (1835), who coined the term “moral insanity” in reference to
  
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 111

such cases—connoting the idea that the behavioral deviance in such cases stems from a deficit in the intrinsic sense of decency, fairness, and responsi- bility that normal individuals possess. However, Pritchard departed from Rush (as well as Pinel) by applying the label “moral insanity” so broadly as to encompass most conditions considered mental disorders at this time (e.g., drug or alcohol addiction; sexual deviations of various kinds, including homosexuality; mood disorders) except those classifiable today as mental retardation or schizophrenia. The result was a broadening of the construct that persisted in large part until Cleckley (1941; see below).
The term “psychopathic” was introduced by German psychiatrist J. L. Koch (1891) as an alternative to Pritchard’s term “morally insane.” Specifically, Koch proposed the label “psychopathic inferiority” to denote conditions of a chronic nature that in his view reflected an underlying organic (physical, brain-based) cause. Like Pritchard, Koch applied this term to a much broader array of clinical conditions than would be encompassed by current conceptualizations of psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder—including neurotic conditions and some forms of mental retarda- tion as well as “character disorders” of various sorts (i.e., conditions that would now be regarded as “personality disorders”). In the seventh edition of his classic volume Psychiatrie: Ein Lehrbuch (“Psychiatry: A Textbook”), Koch’s contemporary Emil Kraepelin (1904) used the term “psychopathic personalities” for a narrower range of conditions that he characterized as chronic and constitutional in origin. These included impulse control prob- lems, sexual perversions, obsessional syndromes, and other “degenerative” personalities. Included among the last of these conditions were four groups of individuals who would be regarded as antisocial and/or psychopathic according to current definitions: (1) “morbid liars and swindlers” (charm- ing, deceitful, fraudulent, and lacking in loyalty to others); (2) “criminals by impulse” (driven by impulsive urges to commit crimes such as theft, fire set- ting, and sexual assault); (3) “professional criminals” (deliberately calculat- ing and self-serving); and (4) “morbid vagabonds” (inadequate, aimless, and irresponsible). In the next (eighth) edition of his text, Kraepelin (1915) revised his typology of pathological personalities, dropping the “profes- sional criminal” subgroup and adding four other types: excitable, eccentric, antisocial, and quarrelsome. The latter two of these (marked by callousness- destructiveness and alienation-hostility, respectively) intersect most clearly with modern conceptualizations of antisocial personality.
Adolf Meyer (1904) introduced these German conceptualizations to the American psychiatric community. Following Koch, Meyer used the term “constitutional inferiority” to describe what he regarded as chronic characterological disorders. However, in contrast with Koch (but like Kraepelin), he explicitly excluded neurotic conditions from this class of dis- order. The distinction Meyer drew between these chronic syndromes and neurotic conditions persisted in American psychiatric circles, but his descriptive label for the former (“constitutional inferiority”) was super- seded in time by Kraepelin’s more evaluatively neutral term, “psychopathic
  
112
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

personality.” The term “sociopathic” was introduced by the German psy- chiatrist Karl Birnbaum (1909) as a challenge to Kraepelin’s characteriza- tion of these chronic conditions as constitutional in origin. Birnbaum was part of a revisionist movement, persisting through the 1930s, that viewed most forms of mental disorder (including so-called “psychopathic person- alities”) as arising primarily from social-environmental factors. However, despite reflecting fundamentally different perspectives on etiology, the terms psychopathic and sociopathic came to be used interchangeably over time (e.g., whereas Cleckley [1941, 1976] preferred the term “psychopathic personality,” the first edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders [DSM], published by the American Psychiatric Association [APA] in 1952, employed the term “sociopathic personality disturbance”).1
Influential figures in this area during the 1920s and 1930s included Kurt Schneider (in Germany) and Eugen Kahn (in America). Both espoused a decid- edly broad perspective on the construct of psychopathy. In the third edition of his volume Die Psychopathischen Persönlichkeiten (“The Psychopathic Personalities”; 1934), Schneider identified 10 distinctive subtypes of psy- chopaths, several of which (e.g., “hyperthymic,” “depressive,” “insecure,” and “asthenic”) fall clearly outside modern conceptualizations of psychopathy and antisocial personality. Likewise, Kahn’s (1931) list of psychopathic subgroups (16 in all) included several patently neurotic conditions (e.g., “nervous,” “anx- ious,” “sensitive,” “depressive”), as well as other assorted types that bear minimal resemblance to contemporary notions of psychopathy or APD (e.g., “compulsive,” “moody,” “sexually perverse,” “eccentric”).
Cleckley’s “Mask of Sanity”
American psychiatrist Hervey Cleckley countered this historical inclination toward diagnostic overinclusiveness with the publication of his seminal vol- ume The Mask of Sanity, which appeared in its initial edition in 1941. Drawing on his own clinical experiences with patients at the Veteran’s Administration Hospital in Augusta, Georgia—one of the largest psychiatric hospitals in the United States at the time his book was written—Cleckley pre- sented a variety of vivid case examples to illustrate the personality and behav- ioral characteristics of individuals he viewed as psychopathic. In contrast with prior writers who defined “psychopathic personality” as a term encompass- ing various forms of delinquency, impulse control problems, addictions, and sexual deviations (as well as, in some cases, neurotic conditions and organic brain syndromes), Cleckley explicitly rejected this broad conceptualization. Instead, he characterized psychopathy as a highly distinctive clinical syn- drome with a unique underlying etiology, which he regarded as affective in nature (i.e., a core deficit in emotional reactivity). Based on the material pro- vided in his case descriptions, Cleckley proposed a list of 16 specific criteria he believed could be used to identify psychopathic individuals.
  
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 113

Perhaps the most essential element in Cleckley’s conceptualization, embod- ied in the title of his book, is the idea that psychopathy entails the juxtaposition of severe underlying pathology against the overt appearance of robust mental health. In contrast with other psychiatric patients who present as confused, agitated, dysphoric, withdrawn, or otherwise disturbed, psychopaths appear poised, sociable, and generally well adjusted upon initial contact. It is only through ongoing observation across a range of situations that the psychopath’s characteristic deviancy becomes evident. Table 6.1 illustrates this by organizing Cleckley’s (1976) 16 diagnostic criteria for psychopathy into three distinctive categories. The first category consists of indicators of positive psychological adjustment (i.e., good intelligence and social charm, absence of delusions/ irrationality, absence of nervousness, and suicide rarely carried out). With these indicators, Cleckley was referring not merely to the absence of salient mental disturbance but also to the presence of resiliency and good adjustment:
The surface of the psychopath . . . shows up as equal to or better than normal and gives no hint at all of a disorder within. Nothing about him suggests oddness, inadequacy, or moral frailty. His mask is that of robust mental health. (p. 383)
Table 6.1 Cleckley’s (1976) 16 diagnostic criteria for psychopathy, grouped by conceptual category

Conceptual Category
Positive adjustment
Chronic behavioral deviance
Emotional-interpersonal deficits
Criterion Number and Label
1. Superficial charm and good “intelligence”
2. Absence of delusions and other signs of irrational thinking
3. Absence of “nervousness” or psychoneurotic manifestations
14. Suicide rarely carried out
7. Inadequately motivated antisocial behavior
8. Poor judgment and failure to learn by experience 4. Unreliability
13. Fantastic and uninviting behavior with drink and sometimes without
15. Sex life impersonal, trivial, and poorly integrated
16. Failure to follow any life plan
5. Untruthfulness and insincerity
6. Lack of remorse or shame
10. General poverty in major affective reactions
9. Pathologic egocentricity and incapacity for love
11. Specific loss of insight
12. Unresponsiveness in general interpersonal relations

114
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

However, at odds with this outward appearance of good mental health is the presence of severe behavioral maladjustment: “Yet he has a disorder that often manifests itself in conduct far more seriously abnormal than that of the schizophrenic” (Cleckley, 1976, p. 383).
The psychopath, however perfectly he mimics man theoretically, that is to say, when he speaks for himself in words, fails altogether when he is put into the practice of actual living. His failure is so complete and so dramatic that it is difficult to see how such a failure could be achieved by anyone less defective than a downright madman. (p. 370)
In Cleckley’s diagnostic scheme, this component of the syndrome is cap- tured by indicators of manifest behavioral deviance (see Table 6.1, second section), including impulsive antisocial acts, irresponsibility (unreliability), promiscuity, and an absence of any clear life plan. In addition to items reflecting positive psychological adjustment and overt behavioral deviance, Cleckley’s criteria for psychopathy included a third category reflecting the emotional underresponsiveness and absence of genuine social relationships that he viewed as central to the disorder.
Cleckley’s conceptualization of psychopathy was influential because he provided a precise definition of the syndrome that had been lacking to that point in history. His vivid case illustrations persuaded even the casual reader that the clinical entity he was describing truly existed; practitioners in psy- chiatric and forensic settings who routinely encountered individuals of this type could readily identify with his account of the disorder. His conceptual- ization placed core emphasis on the emotional-interpersonal features that dis- tinguished psychopathic individuals from other criminal and antisocial types. Thus along with descriptions of psychopaths who engaged regularly in anti- social acts and experienced frequent encounters with the law, Cleckley pre- sented examples of “successful psychopaths” who maintained careers as scholars, physicians, and businessmen. Cleckley’s view was that the presence of the core underlying emotional-interpersonal deviation was more important in defining the presence of the disorder than marked antisocial deviance.2
Moreover, Cleckley’s 16 diagnostic criteria for psychopathy (derived directly from his case descriptions) provided an objective means of identify- ing the disorder that made it amenable to systematic empirical study. David Lykken’s (1957) pioneering study of anxiety responses in psychopaths employed Cleckley’s criteria as the basis for diagnoses, as did Robert Hare’s seminal investigations beginning in the early 1960s. Cleckley’s conceptual- ization remains highly influential to the present day because it served as the foundation for Hare’s Psychopathy Checklist—Revised (PCL-R; Hare, 1991, 2003), which has become the dominant assessment instrument for the diagnosis of psychopathy in research studies as well as for clinical assess- ment. However, it be should be noted that the PCL-R appears to depart importantly from Cleckley’s conceptualization, particularly because it omits
  
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 115

direct indicators of good mental health. This point is discussed in further detail later.
Emergence of the Concept
of Antisocial Personality Disorder
The roots of the modern psychiatric concept of APD can be traced to the first edition of the DSM (DSM-I; APA, 1952). The formal classification sys- tem for psychiatric disorders embodied in the DSM grew out of efforts by the federal government to develop statistics for illness and death among residents of mental hospitals in the United States. DSM-I was modeled loosely after the sixth revision of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD; World Health Organization, 1948), which for the first time included a section devoted to the classification of mental disorders. This initial edition of the DSM contained a category of mental disorders termed “sociopathic person- ality disturbance”; following earlier conceptualizations of psychopathy, this designation included a broad range of syndromes encompassing sexual devi- ations of various kinds, addictions, and delinquency. Included among the dis- orders in this category was a syndrome referred to as “sociopathic personality disturbance: antisocial reaction,” intended to capture the aggressive, crimi- nally deviant individual who repeatedly violates the norms and laws of soci- ety. (The use of the term “reaction” throughout DSM-I is attributable to the lingering influence of Adolph Meyer, who viewed mental disorders as reac- tions of the personality to biological, social, and psychological factors.)
The second edition of the DSM (APA, 1968) was developed to align even more closely with the version of the ICD in place at that time, ICD-8. In DSM-II, the term “reaction” was eliminated as a descriptor for disorders. Sexual deviations, addictions, and delinquent personality types were grouped under a category entitled “Personality Disorders and Certain Other Non- Psychotic Mental Disorders.” Within this category, the term “antisocial per- sonality” was used for a syndrome corresponding to psychopathy. The diagnostic features of the syndrome closely resembled those proposed by Cleckley and included weak socialization, incapacity for loyalty, selfishness, callousness, irresponsibility, and absence of guilt. A serious limitation of DSM-II (as of DSM-I) was that the basis for diagnostic classifications con- sisted of prototypical descriptions of each disorder rather than specific, behavior-oriented diagnostic criteria. As a result, the reliability of clinical and research diagnostic classifications using DSM-II was generally poor.
The problem of diagnostic reliability was confronted in the next edition of the DSM, DSM-III (APA, 1980), by the development of more explicit, behavior- oriented criterion sets for disorders patterned after those developed for use in research studies by Feighner et al. (1972) and Spitzer, Endicott, and Robins (1978). The criteria for antisocial personality disorder in the DSM-III were strongly influenced by the work of Lee Robins (1966), who conducted
  
116
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

groundbreaking research on the development of “sociopathy” by following up a large sample of individuals (N = 524) seen as children in a treatment clinic for juvenile delinquents. Following Cleckley, Robins’s initial criteria for sociopathy included items relating to lack of guilt, remorse, and shame, but (due in part to problems in assessing them reliably) these criteria failed to differentiate signifi- cantly between sociopaths and nonsociopaths in her study, and thus were dis- carded as indicators in the criterion sets developed subsequently by Feighner et al. and Spitzer et al. Consequently, the criteria for APD adopted within DSM- III focused exclusively on behavioral indicants of deviance in childhood and adulthood, including such things as truancy, delinquency, stealing, vandalism, irresponsibility, aggressiveness, impulsivity, recklessness, and lying. As a func- tion of this change, the DSM-III diagnosis of antisocial personality proved to be highly reliable. Nevertheless, influential investigators in the area (e.g., Frances, 1980; Hare, 1983; Millon, 1981) were quick to challenge the diagnostic valid- ity of the DSM-III criteria for APD on the grounds that they excluded many of the features Cleckley deemed central to psychopathy, including superficial charm, absence of anxiety, lack of remorse or empathy, and general poverty of affect. Some effort was made to respond to these criticisms in the revised third edition of the DSM (DSM-III-R; APA, 1987) by the addition of lack of remorse (i.e., “feels justified in having hurt, mistreated, or stolen from another,” p. 346) as an adult criterion for APD. Some changes were also made in the child crite- ria for APD (i.e., conduct disorder component) in DSM-III-R, but these changes did not affect the representation of “core features” of psychopathy in the APD criterion set (i.e., in DSM-III-R, as in DSM-III, all of the child criteria remained focused on overt behavioral deviance).
As part of the groundwork for DSM-IV (APA, 1994), specialized field tri- als were conducted to evaluate proposed revisions to the criterion sets for various disorders. One of these (Widiger et al., 1996) focused on APD. Two proposals were evaluated in the field trial for APD, one entailing increased representation of core affective-interpersonal features of psychopathy (e.g., lack of empathy, inflated self-appraisal, glib/superficial interactive style) in the criteria for APD, and the other involving simplification of the existing criterion set without substantial changes to the diagnosis. Diagnostic data from four major clinical samples (inpatients at a general psychiatric institu- tion; drug treatment and homeless shelter residents; methadone maintenance program outpatients; and incarcerated offenders) were examined, consisting of scores on the existing DSM-III-R criteria along with scores on two alter- native criterion sets that provided greater coverage of affective-interpersonal features: a shortened (10-item) version of Hare’s (1991) PCL-R, and the research criteria for “dyssocial personality disorder” developed for ICD-10. The field trial data revealed high correspondence among diagnoses based on the three criterion sets for all samples except the prison sample, where the PCL-R criterion set showed some evidence of incremental validity over the other two criterion sets in predicting psychopathy-related variables. However, it was concluded that enhanced validity within this one specific
  
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 117

sample was insufficient to warrant a major shift in the criteria for APD, and consequently, changes based on this field trial were limited to simplification of the adult indicators. Specifically, two indicators (“parental irresponsibil- ity” and “failure to sustain a monogamous relationship”) were dropped, and two others (“employment-related irresponsibility” and “financial irresponsi- bility”) were combined into a single “irresponsibility” item, resulting in a reduction of the number of adult indicators (from 10 in DSM-III-R to seven in DSM-IV). In addition, some minor changes in the child criteria for APD were instituted based on the results of a separate field trial for conduct dis- order (i.e., two items, “staying out at night” and “intimidating others” were added, resulting in 15 indicators in DSM-IV versus 13 in DSM-III-R).
Thus due to the field trial results, no effort was made to increase the representation of core affective-interpersonal features of psychopathy in the criteria for APD in DSM-IV. As a result of this decision, criticisms of the construct validity of the APD criteria by investigators in the psychopathy area have persisted (e.g., Hare & Hart, 1995). The issue of the correspon- dence between the construct of APD within DSM-IV and the construct of psychopathy embodied in Hare’s (1991, 2003) PCL-R is discussed more in subsequent sections.
The DSM Construct of __________________________ Antisocial Personality Disorder
Clinical Features
Table 6.2 summarizes the diagnostic criteria for APD listed in the text revision of DSM-IV (DSM-IV-TR; APA, 2000), the most recent version of the DSM. Like the criteria for the other disorders in the DSM, the criteria for APD are polythetic—meaning that individuals can achieve a diagnosis of APD in different ways, as long as they meet the criteria for inclusion (i.e., current age at least 18, occurrence of antisocial behavior not limited to episodes of mania or schizophrenic psychosis) and fulfill a sufficient number of the designated child and adult criteria for the disorder (i.e., three or more of 15 possible symptoms of child conduct disorder before age 15, leading to significant social, academic, or occupational impairment, and three or more of seven possible adult features since age 15).
As indicated in Table 6.2, the child criteria for APD include aggressive and destructive behaviors on one hand, and deceitfulness/theft and nonaggressive rule breaking on the other. Formal factor-analytic investigations of the structure of the child APD criteria (e.g., Frick et al., 1991; Tackett, Krueger, Sawyer, & Graetz, 2003) have confirmed that the aggressive and nonaggressive indicators define separate, albeit correlated, factors. Tackett et al. (2003) reported that these two conduct disorder factors showed discriminative associations with
  
118
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Table 6.2 Diagnostic criteria for DSM-IV-TR antisocial personality disorder
Criterion Category
A. Adult antisocial behavior (3 or more of the following since age 15):
B. Age criterion
C. Child conduct disorder (3 or more of the following before age 15, resulting in impaired social, academic, or occupational function):
Summary Description of Criterion
D. Comorbidity criterion
1. Repeated participation in illegal acts
2. Deceitfulness
3. Impulsiveness or failure to make plans in advance 4. Hostile-aggressive behavior
5. Engagement in actions that endanger self or others 6. Frequent irresponsible behavior
7. Absence of remorse
Current age at least 18
Aggression toward people or animals:
1. Frequent bullying, threatening, or intimidation of others 2. Frequent initiation of physical fights
3. Use of dangerous weapons
4. Physical cruelty toward people
5. Physical cruelty toward animals
6. Theft involving victim confrontation 7. Forced sexual contact
Destruction of property:
8. Deliberate fire setting with intent to cause damage 9. Deliberate destruction of property
Deceptiveness or stealing:
10. Breaking/entering (house, building, or vehicle) 11. Frequent lying to acquire things or to avoid duties 12. Nontrivial theft without victim confrontation
Serious rule violations:
13. Frequent violations of parental curfew, starting before age 13
14. Running away from home
15. Frequent truancy, starting before age 13
Antisocial behavior does not occur exclusively during episodes of schizophrenia or mania

SOURCE: Based on information from the American Psychiatric Association, 2000.
aggressive behavior syndrome and delinquent behavior syndrome, respectively, as defined by scores on the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach, 1991).
An implication of this work is that there may be distinct variants of child antisocial deviance with different etiologic underpinnings. Along these lines, Moffitt (1993) proposed a distinction between adolescence-limited and life course–persistent subgroups of delinquent individuals. The former was distin- guished by a later onset and predominantly nonaggressive forms of deviancy and rule breaking, the latter by early age of onset, aggressive-destructive as well as nonaggressive delinquent behaviors, and continuation of child and adolescent deviancy into adulthood. Moffitt postulated that the early-onset, aggressive
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 119

subtype of delinquency may have a stronger underlying neurobiological basis (see also Lynam, 1997). Recently, Tackett, Krueger, Iacono, and McGue (2005) reexamined the structure of conduct disorder symptoms in a large sam- ple of male twins recruited from the community, permitting an analysis of dif- ferential etiologic contributions to aggressive versus nonaggressive subfactors. The results of this analysis indicated that these two components of conduct dis- order have common as well as distinctive etiologic underpinnings. Additive genetic influences and nonshared environment (i.e., experiences unique to the individual) contributed significantly to both components, the proportion of symptom variance attributable to genes being somewhat higher for the aggressive component (35%) than for the nonaggressive component (28%). In addition, a significant contribution of shared environment (i.e., environ- mental influences common to two siblings growing up in the same house- hold) was found for the nonaggressive component only.
The adult criteria for APD include one specific indicator of aggression (irritabil- ity and aggressiveness), three clearly nonaggressive indicators (deceitfulness, impulsivity, and irresponsibility), and three nonspecific indicators (failure to conform to norms with respect to lawful behaviors, reckless disregard for safety of self or others, and lack of remorse). Perhaps because the adult APD criteria do not divide as readily into aggressive and nonaggressive indicators, no parallel factor-analytic work has been done to examine the extent to which distinctive components of this sort underlie adult indicators of APD. Nonetheless, there is evidence in the literature that aggressive forms of adult antisocial behavior have unique neurobiological correlates. For example, a number of published studies have reported evidence of reduced levels of the neurotransmitter serotonin (indexed by concentrations of the serotonin metabolite 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid [5-HIAA] in cerebrospinal fluid) in antisocial individuals who exhibit severe episodes of impulsive aggressive behavior (e.g., Linnoila et al., 1983; Virkkunen et al., 1994; for a review, see Minzenberg & Siever, 2006). Evidence of reduced brain serotonin has also been reported in antisocial individuals who engage in impulsive suicidal acts (Linnoila & Virkkunen, 1992), which have been conceptualized as an alternative, self-directed expression of impulsive aggressive tendencies (Verona & Patrick, 2000).
The evidence for distinctive correlates of aggressive deviancy suggests that it may be fruitful to examine the structure of adult indicators of antisocial personality, as has been done with indicators of child conduct disorder. To achieve this goal, it would be necessary to partition symptoms of adult antisocial deviance more clearly into aggressive and nonaggressive types and to have multiple indicators of each, which would necessitate some departure from the current DSM-IV-TR criterion set (e.g., failure to abide by laws could be parsed into offenses of a violent versus nonviolent nature; different expressions of irritability/aggressiveness could be scored as separate indicators). If adult antisocial deviance were found to comprise distinctive aggressive and nonaggressive facets, as appears to be the case for child conduct disorder, it would be interesting to explore (particularly in a
  
120
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

longitudinal sample) the stability of each of these facets from childhood to adulthood. For example, it might be hypothesized that the aggressive facet would evidence greater stability across time (cf. Moffit, 1993).
Prevalence
The prevalence of APD in the general community is estimated to be about 2%, with rates for men (3%) substantially exceeding those for women (1%; APA, 2000). The gender difference in the prevalence of APD is an intriguing but unresolved issue. One perspective is that gender role stereotypes and socialization pressures account for the difference (i.e., men, by sociohistoric tradition, are expected and encouraged by society to be more aggressive and adventurous, leading to a higher rate of deviance in the form of APD). Another is that the difference in APD prevalence reflects basic biologically rooted differences between women and men in trait dispositions that pro- mote the occurrence of antisocial deviance in people. The ecumenical stance, of course, would be that both factors play a role. Further research, includ- ing genetics studies of gender-related differences in traits and affiliated behaviors, may allow us to determine which of these positions, if either, is more substantially correct. Interestingly, despite well-documented differ- ences in arrest rates for individuals from different ethnic groups, robust dif- ferences in rates of APD have not been found across ethnic groups in epidemiological studies. For example, the Epidemiological Catchment Area (ECA) study (Robins & Regier, 1991) found no significant differences in the prevalence of APD (diagnosed according to DSM-III criteria) across White, African-American, and Hispanic groups.
The prevalence of APD in clinical settings tends to be substantially higher than in the community at large, particularly within correctional and foren- sic settings, where estimated base rates run as high as 50% to 80% (Hare, 2003). The very high rate base rate in forensic settings highlights one of the major criticisms that has been leveled against the APD diagnostic criteria, namely, that they are biased so heavily toward criminally deviant behaviors that most individuals who run into serious trouble with the law will be diag- nosed with APD, even though such individuals vary widely in the expression of their deviance and in the underlying bases for it. From this perspective, APD is a “wastebasket” category into which various sorts of deviant indi- viduals are placed. However, recent evidence suggests that this position is too extreme. An alternative perspective is that APD reflects in part the influ- ence of a broad trait factor that determines general risk for a variety of impulse control problems, along with other specific etiologic influences that determine the precise expression of risk in particular individuals (Krueger et al., 2002; Krueger, Markon, Patrick, & Iacono, 2005). From this per- spective, some individuals will develop APD largely because they have a high general vulnerability to impulse control problems, whereas others will
  
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 121

develop APD primarily as a function of unique environmental experiences (e.g., early abuse, deviant peers) that promote the occurrence of antisocial behavior. Individuals of the former sort will tend to show an earlier age of onset of behavioral deviance, greater impulsive aggressive tendencies, and a higher prevalence of comorbid impulse control problems of other types (e.g., alcohol and drug problems).
Comorbidity With Other DSM Disorders
APD shows well-documented patterns of comorbidity with other dis- orders in the DSM, most notably substance use disorders. In the ECA study (Robins & Regier, 1991), the base rate of substance use disorders (of any type) among individuals diagnosed with APD exceeded 80%. Earlier studies employing DSM-III criteria reported similar high rates of alcohol and drug problems among individuals diagnosed with APD (e.g., Koenigsberg, Kaplan, Gilmore, & Cooper, 1985; Lewis, Rice, & Helzer, 1983). APD has also been shown to be associated with greatly enhanced risk for alcohol and drug use disorders in a number of more recent comor- bidity studies (Grant et al., 2004; Kessler & Walters, 2002; Skodol, Oldham, & Gallaher, 1999).
The fact that substance use disorders co-occur with APD to a much greater extent than would be expected if each occurred by chance, given their respective population prevalence rates, implies that something system- atic underlies the association between the two. This hypothesis has been con- firmed by recent factor-analytic investigations of the diagnostic overlap among common disorders within the DSM. For example, employing diag- nostic data from the National Comorbidity Survey (Kessler et al., 1994), Krueger (1999a) reported evidence for two broad factors underlying the most common Axis I disorders: an “externalizing” factor encompassing APD, alcohol dependence, and drug dependence, and an “internalizing” dimension encapsulating the mood and anxiety disorders (see also Krueger, Caspi, Moffitt, & Silva, 1998; Vollebergh, Iedema, Bijl, de Graaf, Smit, & Ormel, 2001). One possible explanation for the systematic association between APD and substance use disorders is that it reflects overlap between the criteria for the two types of disorders. For example, it could be argued that some of the behaviors that define APD (e.g., irresponsibility, reckless- ness, aggressiveness) are common sequelae of alcohol or drug abuse, so that increased rates of such behaviors would be expected among individuals with substance use problems. However, this seems unlikely to explain the sys- tematic association between APD and substance use disorders. For one thing, the onset of APD typically precedes that of substance use problems in cases where the two are comorbid. In addition, the relationship between APD and substance abuse problems is asymmetric, due to the higher popu- lation prevalence of the latter—that is, whereas most individuals diagnosed
  
122
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

with APD also show evidence of substance use disorders, the majority of individuals from the general community diagnosed with substance abuse or dependence do not meet criteria for APD.
Another possibility is that APD and substance use problems arise from a common diathesis—i.e., a common underlying trait factor that predisposes individuals toward the development of both types of disorders. Consistent with this possibility, behavior genetics (twin) studies have revealed evidence of shared genetic factors underlying APD and substance use disorders (e.g., Grove et al., 1990; Pickens, Svikis, McGue, & LaBuda, 1995; Slutske et al., 1998). More recent quantitative analyses of etiologic factors contributing to the broad externalizing factor representing the systematic covariance among these disor- ders have revealed that this factor is substantially heritable (Kendler, Prescott, Myers, & Neale, 2003; Krueger et al., 2002; Young, Stallings, Corley, Krauter, & Hewitt, 2000). This work is described in more detail below.
Personality Correlates
Two personality trait variables in particular, represented in various models of personality, have been shown to be related to APD. One is impulsiveness, represented in the five-factor model (FFM; Digman, 1990) by the conscien- tiousness factor (reversed) and in Tellegen’s (in press) Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire (MPQ) model by the higher-order factor of con- straint (reversed). The other is aggressiveness, represented in the FFM by the agreeableness factor (reversed) and in the MPQ by the lower-order trait of aggression. For example, within a sample of adult psychiatric patients (N = 54), Trull (1992) reported significant correlations of −.32 and −.36, respectively, between symptoms of APD assessed by means of clinical inter- view and scores on the conscientiousness and agreeableness factors of the FFM assessed concurrently by the NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI; Costa & McCrae, 1985). Krueger (1999b) reported that, in a community epidemiological sample consisting of young adults (N = 961), scores on the MPQ higher-order factors of constraint and negative emotionality (the aggression facet of the latter, in particular) assessed at age 18 predicted diag- noses and symptoms of APD assessed via interview at age 21, even after con- trolling for the presence of APD at age 18. Notably, these same personality variables (impulsivity and aggression) show reliable associations with sub- stance use disorders (Acton, 2003; Casillas & Clark, 2002; Krueger, 1999b; Lynam, Leukefeld, & Clayton, 2003; Sher, Bartholow, & Wood, 2000; Slutske et al., 2002; Trull & Sher, 1994).
These relations between personality trait variables and psychopathological syndromes have been interpreted in various ways. One perspective is that traits indexed by personality scales reflect basic individual-difference processes from which mental disorder symptoms arise; another is that deviations in person- ality emerge as a consequence of psychopathology (for a discussion of these perspectives, see Widiger, Verheul, & van den Brink, 1999). A third per- spective is that psychopathological symptoms and personality trait variables
  
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 123

correlate with one another because they are indicators of a shared underly- ing (latent) individual differences factor. With regard to APD and substance dependence, this perspective would suggest that these disorders are related to one another and in turn to personality traits of impulsivity and aggression because all of these variables are manifest indicators of a shared underlying externalizing factor. Krueger et al. (2002) evaluated this hypothesis for the broad MPQ factor of constraint by including this personality variable along with child and adult symptoms of APD and alcohol and drug dependence symptoms in a joint-factor analysis. Consistent with the aforementioned hypothesis, the analysis revealed the presence of a single latent factor on which constraint loaded significantly together with all four symptom variables.
Krueger, Markon, Patrick, Benning, and Kramer (in press) extended this work by undertaking a fine-grained analysis of traits and problem behaviors within the domain of externalizing psychopathology to elucidate the scope and structure of this spectrum more fully. They began by identifying various constructs embodied in the DSM definitions of the disorders included in the Krueger et al. (2002) analysis, then developed self-report items to tap these constructs. They also surveyed the literature to identify other behavioral and trait constructs related empirically or conceptually to externalizing psy- chopathology and developed additional items to index these constructs. Across multiple rounds of data collection and analysis, item response model- ing and factor analysis were used to refine the overall item set and thereby clarify the nature of constructs associated with the broad externalizing factor.
Employing this strategy, Krueger et al. (in press) arrived at a final set of 23 constructs, each operationalized by a unique subscale. These constructs included alcohol, drug, and marijuana use and problems; aggression of vari- ous sorts; impulsiveness; irresponsibility; rebelliousness; excitement seeking; and blame externalization. Structural analyses of these 23 subscales yielded evidence of one broad superordinate factor (“externalizing”) on which all sub- scales loaded (the strongest indicators being “irresponsibility” and “problem- atic impulsivity”) and two subordinate factors accounting for residual variance in specific subscales—one factor marked by subscales indexing aggression (all forms), callousness, and excitement seeking, and the other marked by sub- scales indexing substance-related problems. These findings provide support for the idea that problem behaviors and affiliated personality traits within this domain are indicators of a shared underlying factor (externalizing). In addi- tion, consistent with results emerging from structural analyses of the child (conduct disorder) criteria for APD, this more comprehensive analysis of con- structs within the externalizing domain revealed evidence of distinctive aggres- sive and nonaggressive expressions of this general factor.
Neurobiological Correlates
A variety of neurobiological correlates of APD have been identified. For example, as noted earlier, antisocial individuals—in particular, those displaying
  
124
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

impulsive aggressive behavior—show evidence of reduced levels of the neuro- transmitter serotonin in the brain. Other research has consistently demonstrated that low resting heart rate is a correlate of antisocial deviance (Ortiz & Raine, 2004; Raine, 1993, 2002). Indeed, prospective studies have reliably found that low resting heart rate in childhood and adolescence predicts antisocial behavior in adulthood (e.g., Maliphant, Hume & Furnham, 1990; Raine & Venables, 1984; Wadsworth, 1976; for a review, see Ortiz & Raine, 2004). This robust association with low resting heart rate has been interpreted as indicating that general physiological hypoarousal represents an underlying risk factor for anti- social behavior (Raine, 1993, 2002)—because it promotes sensation seeking behavior as a means to enhancing arousal (Eysenck, 1967; Zuckerman, 1979).
Neuropsychological studies provide evidence of deficits in frontal brain function in individuals diagnosed as antisocial. Morgan and Lilienfeld (2000) reported meta-analytic evidence for deficits on frontal lobe tasks in individuals exhibiting conduct disorder and adult antisocial behavior. Notably, individuals at risk for alcoholism by virtue of a positive parental history also show evidence of impairment on neuropsychological tests of frontal lobe function (Peterson & Pihl, 1990; Tarter, Alterman, & Edwards, 1985). Iacono, Carlson, and Malone (2000) suggested that impairments in frontal brain function may be generally characteristic of individuals with externalizing problems.
There is also evidence that reduced amplitude of the P300 brain potential response, long known to be an indicator of risk for alcohol problems (Polich, Pollock, & Bloom, 1994), may be a marker of externalizing problems more generally, including APD. A number of studies have reported evidence of reduced P300 brain response amplitude in individuals with APD (Bauer, Hesselbrock, O’Connor, & Roberts, 1994; Bauer, O’Connor, & Hesselbrock, 1994; Costa et al., 2000; Iacono, Carlson, Malone, & McGue, 2002). Reduced P300 response amplitude has also been found in individuals with other impulse control problems, including nicotine dependence (Anokhin et al., 2000; Iacono et al., 2002), child conduct disorder (Bauer & Hesselbrock, 1999a, 1999b, 2002; Kim, Kim, & Kwon, 2001), and atten- tion deficit hyperactivity disorder (Johnstone & Barry, 1996; Klorman, 1991). The implication is that reduced P300 amplitude could be an indica- tor of the general externalizing factor that these disorders share.
Patrick, Bernat, Malone, and their colleagues (2006) evaluated this possibil- ity in a sample of 969 males recruited from the community by examining the association between reduced P300 amplitude and scores on the externalizing factor, defined as the primary component derived from a principal- components analysis of symptoms of various DSM-III-R impulse control disor- ders (i.e., conduct disorder, adult antisocial behavior, and alcohol, drug, and nicotine dependence). These investigators found a highly significant negative association between scores on the externalizing factor and P300 brain response amplitude (i.e., higher externalizing scores, reflecting more severe symptoms of a greater number of impulse problems, were associated with smaller P300
  
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 125

amplitude). Moreover, significant associations between each individual diag- nostic variable and P300 amplitude were accounted for entirely by the exter- nalizing factor—that is, after controlling for scores on this common factor, all associations for individual disorders dropped to nonsignificance. These results suggest that the relationship that has been demonstrated between APD and P300 response may reflect the influence of this broad externalizing factor, which accounts for a sizable portion of the variance in APD.
Etiologic Perspectives on Antisocial Personality Disorder
A variety of etiologic models of APD have been proposed, some of them based on the aforementioned neurobiological findings (for reviews, see Raine, 1993; Zuckerman, 1999). Most of these models focus on the under- pinnings of APD as a distinctive syndrome, without considering its relations to other forms of psychopathology (e.g., substance use disorders). However, recent efforts have been made to develop integrative etiologic models that accommodate APD’s associations with other disorders and distinctive per- sonality traits by conceptualizing APD as one facet of a broader spectrum of traits and problem behaviors. An example of this is the hierarchical spectrum model proposed by Krueger and his colleagues (2002).
The essence of the hierarchical spectrum model is that there is a broad dispositional factor that disorders within a spectrum share, along with unique etiologic influences that determine the unique symptomatic expres- sion of each disorder. The database on which the model was based consisted of symptom scores on four diagnostic variables (child conduct disorder, adult antisocial behavior, alcohol dependence, and drug dependence), along with a trait measure of impulsiveness (the constraint factor of the MPQ) for a sample of male and female twins recruited from the community (N = 1,048). A biometric structural analysis revealed a large common factor (“externalizing”) on which all of these diagnostic variables loaded substan- tially (.58–.78); more than 80% of the variance in this common factor was attributable to additive genetic influence (see also Kendler et al., 2003; Young et al., 2000). The remaining variance in each disorder not accounted for by the broad externalizing factor was attributable primarily to nonshared environmental influence—although for conduct disorder there was also a significant contribution of shared environment.
Based on these findings, Krueger et al. (2002) proposed that a general constitutional factor contributes to the development of various disorders in this spectrum, but that the precise expression of this underlying vulnerabil- ity (i.e., as antisocial deviance of different kinds, or as alcohol or drug prob- lems) is determined by disorder-specific etiologic influences. Although the analysis pointed to unique environmental experience as the main determi- nant of diagnostic specificity (with some contribution of family environment for conduct disorder), owing to the somewhat modest sample size and large
  
126
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

confidence intervals around parameters in the model reflecting unique etio- logic contributions to specific syndromes, the authors allowed for the possi- bility that specific genetic factors also contribute to the uniqueness of these disorders. Indeed, Kendler et al. (2003) presented evidence for this possibil- ity in a subsequent study. As noted above, Krueger et al. (in press) extended the work of Krueger et al. (2002) by providing a more comprehensive analy- sis of traits and problem behaviors within the externalizing spectrum. However, an etiologic analysis of this newer, more comprehensive model remains to be undertaken. In particular, it will be important to evaluate dif- ferences in etiologic contributions to the two subordinate factors identified by Krueger et al. (in press) in comparison with those for the broad external- izing factor. (For a discussion of possible neurobiological mechanisms, see Patrick & Bernat, 2006.)
Psychopathy: Current
Conceptualizations and Empirical Findings _____________
Hare’s Psychopathy Checklist—Revised (PCL-R)
Description. The conceptualization that has dominated contemporary experimental research on psychopathy is the construct embodied in Hare’s (1991, 2003) Psychopathy Checklist—Revised (PCL-R). The PCL-R was devised to identify incarcerated offenders who exemplify Cleckley’s (1976) description of the psychopathic personality. It consists of 20 items (see Table 6.3), each rated on a 0–2 scale (absent, equivocal, or present) on the basis of infor- mation obtained from a semistructured interview and prison files.3 The stan- dard PCL-R interview covers a range of topics, including education/schooling, employment, family background, relationships and children, criminal history, and drug and alcohol use. A number of questions are included to tap features such as grandiosity, lack of remorse, lack of empathy, shallow affectivity, and failure to accept responsibility for actions. The file review is performed to gather additional information as well as to corroborate information collected in the interview. The PCL-R manual (Hare, 2003) provides a narrative descrip- tion of the sources and types of information to be used in the scoring of each item. Scores on the 20 individual items are summed to yield an overall psy- chopathy score, and total score cutoffs (Hare, 2003) are applied to designate individuals as psychopathic (total PCL-R score of 30 or higher), nonpsycho- pathic (total score of 20 or lower), or intermediate (total PCL-R score between 20 and 30). Overall scores on the PCL-R are highly reliable: for example, Hare (2003) reported pooled intraclass correlation coefficients (single rater) of .83 and .86 across seven male prisoner samples and across four male forensic psy- chiatric samples, respectively.
The original version of this instrument, the PCL (Hare, 1980), evolved out of a global rating system that drew directly on Cleckley’s diagnostic criteria.
 
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 127

Table 6.3 Items of the Psychopathy Checklist—Revised (PCL-R; Hare, 2003)
Item Number Item Label
1. Glibness/superficial charm1,a
2. Grandiose sense of self-worth1,a
3. Need for stimulation/proneness to boredom2,c
4. Pathological lying1,a
5. Conning/manipulative1,a
6. Lack of remorse or guilt1,b
7. Shallow affect1,b
8. Callous/lack of empathy1,b
9. Parasitic lifestyle2,c
10. Poor behavioral controls2,d
11. Promiscuous sexual behavior
12. Early behavior problems2,d
13. Lack of realistic, long-term goals2,c
14. Impulsivity2,c
15. Irresponsibility2,c
16. Failure to accept responsibility for own actions1,b
17. Many short-term marital relationships
18. Juvenile delinquency2,d
19. Revocation of conditional release2,d
20. Criminal versatilityd
1Items comprising Factor 1 in the original two-factor PCL-R model (Harpur, Hakstian, & Hare, 1988). 2Items comprising Factor 2 in the original two-factor PCL-R model (Harpur, Hakstian, & Hare, 1988).
aItems comprising the interpersonal facet in the revised four-factor model (Hare, 2003); these same items comprise the arrogant and deceitful interpersonal style factor in the revised three-factor model (Cooke & Michie, 2001). bItems comprising the affective facet in the revised four-factor model (Hare, 2003); these same items comprise the deficient affective experience factor in the three-factor model (Cooke & Michie, 2001). cItems comprising the lifestyle facet in the revised four-factor model (Hare, 2003); these same items comprise the impulsive and irresponsible behavioral style factor in the revised three-factor model (Cooke & Michie, 2001). dItems comprising the antisocial facet in the revised four-factor model.
This global rating approach, a variant of one employed initially by Lykken (1957), was used for several years in experimental studies of incarcerated male offenders by Hare and his colleagues, beginning in the mid 1960s. Ratings from 1 to 7 were assigned by diagnosticians familiar with the history and behavior of the subject—with 1 signifying that the individual was definitely not a psy- chopath and 7 signifying that the individual clearly matched the Cleckley pro- totype description. Scores of 1–2 and 6–7, respectively, were used to assign individuals to nonpsychopathic and psychopathic groups for testing purposes. This global rating system proved to be quite reliable, but the aura of “subjec- tivity” surrounding it led to calls for a more systematic assessment procedure based on objective behavioral criteria. The strategy Hare took in developing the
128
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

PCL was to select items from a large list of candidate indicators that differenti- ated empirically between individuals assigned low and high scores on the initial global rating system. The original PCL item set comprised 22 items. In the sub- sequent revised version (Hare, 1991), two of those items (“previous diagnosis as a psychopath or similar” and “antisocial behavior not due to alcohol intox- ication”) were dropped, and the descriptions of the scoring criteria for the remaining 20 items were revised somewhat. The items and scoring criteria in the most recent (second) edition of the PCL-R (Hare, 2003) remain the same as they were in the first (1991) edition.
It is notable that the behavioral deviance features and emotional-interpersonal features described by Cleckley (Table 6.1, second and third sections) are well represented in the PCL-R. In contrast, the positive adjustment features (Table 6.1, first section) are not. Although it could be argued that the first part of Cleckley’s “superficial charm and good ‘intelligence’” criterion is captured by item 1 of the PCL-R (“glibness and superficial charm”), a comparison of the wording of Cleckley’s criterion with that of PCL-R item 1 reveals a key dif- ference. PCL-R item 1 includes reference to excessive talkativeness, insincerity, slickness, and a lack of believability in the target individual’s social presenta- tion. Related to this, the instructions for scoring call for an intermediate rating of 1 in cases where the individual presents with a “macho” or “tough guy” image, whereas a rating of 0 is called for if the individual presents as sincere and straightforward. Thus, the emphasis is on a somewhat deviant (“too good to be true”) or hypermasculine self-presentation. The wording of Cleckley’s (1976) criterion has a notably different flavor:
There is nothing at all odd or queer about him, and in every respect he tends to embody the concept of a well-adjusted, happy person. Nor does he, on the other hand, seem to be artificially exerting himself like one who is covering up or who wants to sell you a bill of goods . . . Signs of affectation or excessive affability are not characteristic. He looks like the real thing. (p. 339)
The inclusion of “good ‘intelligence’” (reflecting good sense, intact rea- soning, and above average or superior intellect) as part of this diagnostic criterion reinforces the impression that Cleckley intended this to be an indi- cator of positive psychological adjustment.
What might account for the absence of pure indicators of adjustment among the items of the PCL-R? Although information in the PCL-R manual (Hare, 1991, 2003) and in the initial report on the development of the PCL (e.g., Hare, 1980) is somewhat unclear as to what criteria were used to select items, these sources do indicate that indicators were retained if they discrim- inated low- and high-psychopathy groups defined on the basis of “global rat- ings” (i.e., degree of match with Cleckley’s case examples) and if they showed “good psychometric properties.” The latter implies that indicators were cho- sen that contributed to the reliability (internal consistency) of the overall scale
  
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 129

as well as helped to discriminate extreme groups. This item selection strategy, which fits with the aim of indexing a unitary construct, would operate to homogenize the item set: indicators similar to most other indicators in the candidate pool would be retained, while those differing most from the others would be dropped. Because the greater majority of Cleckley’s diagnostic cri- teria (12 of 16) reflect tendencies toward deviance as opposed to adjustment, the initial candidate pool would almost certainly have included more indica- tors of deviance. Indicators of positive adjustment presumably dropped out because they failed to coalesce with the larger proportion of (pathologic) indi- cators. This would yield a final item set more uniformly indicative of deviance and maladjustment than Cleckley’s original criterion set.
Some of the data contained in the initial report on the development of the PCL (Hare, 1980) appear consistent with this account. Of particular interest are data for a sample of 143 prison inmates who were rated on Cleckley’s 16 cri- teria for psychopathy as well on the 22 items of the original PCL. A principal- components analysis of the Cleckley items in this sample yielded five components, accounting for 64% of the total variance. The largest of these were an emotional-interpersonal component (marked by pathological ego- centricity, poverty of affect, unresponsiveness in interpersonal relations, untruthfulness, and lack of remorse), accounting for 29.3% of the variance, and a behavioral deviance component (marked by absence of life plans, unre- liability, and failure to learn by experience), accounting for 12.0% of the vari- ance. In addition, a psychological adjustment component clearly emerged (marked by charm and good intelligence, absence of delusions/irrationality, and absence of nervousness), but this accounted for a smaller proportion of the variance (7.1%). A principal-components analysis of the PCL items also yielded five components, accounting for approximately 61% of the overall variance. However, in this case the dominant component, accounting for 27.3% of the total variance, was one reflecting behavioral deviance (prone- ness to boredom, lack of realistic plans, parasitic lifestyle, and impulsivity). The second component, reflecting the emotional-interpersonal features of psychopathy (lack of remorse, failure to accept responsibility, conning, grandiosity, glibness, and callousness), accounted for only 13% of the vari- ance. These findings are consistent with the idea that the effort to opera- tionalize Cleckley’s criteria as a unitary construct in the PCL resulted in an item set generally more reflective of deviance and maladjustment.
The relations that overall scores on the PCL-R show with criterion mea- sures further reinforce the notion that, compared with Cleckley’s original conceptualization, the construct underlying the PCL-R as a whole is more purely pathological in nature. For example, overall scores on the PCL-R are highly correlated with overall symptoms of APD (which, as noted earlier, primarily reflect behavioral deviance): the mean correlation across 10 stud- ies reported by Hare (2003) was .67. The personality traits that are corre- lated most strongly with overall scores on the PCL-R are traits reflecting aggression and impulsivity—that is, agreeableness and conscientiousness
  
130
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

from the FFM (significant negative correlations in each case; Lynam & Derefinko, 2006), and lower-order aggression and higher-order constraint from the MPQ (significant positive and negative correlations, respectively; Verona, Patrick, & Joiner, 2001). In addition, overall scores on the PCL-R show robust positive associations with various behavioral indices of aggres- sion (Hare, 2003) as well as with alcohol and drug problems (Reardon, Lang, & Patrick, 2002), and weak positive associations with suicidal behav- ior (Verona et al., 2001). Also, in contrast with Cleckley’s portrayal of psy- chopaths as low in anxiousness, overall scores on the PCL-R show negligible associations with measures of trait anxiety (Hare, 2003) and weak positive associations with FFM neuroticism (Lynam & Derefinko, 2006). Thus the focus on psychopathy as a unitary construct leads to a picture of the psy- chopath as more aggressive and psychologically maladjusted than the major- ity of Cleckley’s case examples.
Distinctive Factors of the PCL-R. It is important to note that the PCL/PCL-R, while developed to assess a putatively unitary construct, does include distinctive factors that exhibit discriminant validity in their relations with external criterion variables. Until fairly recently, on the strength of ini- tial factor-analytic work (Hare et al., 1990; Harpur, Hakstian, & Hare, 1988), the PCL-R has been viewed as comprising two correlated dimensions. Within this two-factor model, Factor 1 is marked by items reflecting the emo- tional and interpersonal features of psychopathy (charm, grandiosity, and deceitfulness/conning; absence of remorse, empathy, and emotional depth; and blame externalization). In contrast, Factor 2 is marked by items describ- ing a chronic antisocial lifestyle, including child behavior problems, impul- siveness, irresponsibility, and absence of long-term goals. Scores on the two PCL-R factors are typically correlated at about .5 (Hare, 1991, 2003). Recently, Cooke and Michie (2001) proposed an alternative three-factor model. In this model, the items of Factor 1 are parsed into two separate (albeit correlated) factors: “arrogant and deceitful personality style,” marked by charm, grandiosity, deceitfulness, and manipulation; and “deficient affec- tive experience,” encompassing absence of remorse or empathy, shallow affect, and failure to accept responsibility. The third factor in this model (“impulsive-irresponsible behavioral style”) consists of a pared-down version of Factor 2, comprising the five indicators deemed to be most traitlike. Even more recently, Hare (2003; see also Hare & Neumann, 2006) proposed a nested four-factor model in which Factor 1 of the original two-factor model was partitioned into “interpersonal” and “affective” facets (mirroring Cooke and Michie’s first two factors) and Factor 2 was divided into a “lifestyle” facet (mirroring Cooke and Michie’s third factor) and an “antisocial” facet (comprising the remaining antisocial behavior indicators from Factor 2).
Most of the data regarding the distinctive correlates of PCL-R item sub- sets pertain to the two factors of the original model. These two factors show divergent relations with independent criterion measures of personality and behavior, particularly when their overlap (covariance) is controlled for using
  
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 131

partial correlation or hierarchical regression techniques. For example, find- ings have generally indicated that the unique variance in the emotional- interpersonal component of the PCL-R (Factor 1) is negatively correlated with measures of trait anxiety, whereas the behavioral deviance component (Factor 2) is positively related to trait anxiety (Hicks & Patrick, 2006; Patrick, 1994; Verona et al., 2001).4 The unique variance in Factor 1 also appears to be positively associated with measures of social dominance (Verona et al., 2001; see also Hare, 1991; Harpur, Hare, & Hakstian, 1989), and in some work, with measures of trait-positive affectivity (Patrick, 1994) and achievement (Verona et al., 2001). These findings indicate that the positive adjustment component of psychopathy contained in Cleckley’s conceptualization may be tapped to some extent by the unique variance in Factor 1 (i.e., the part that is unrelated to behavioral deviance). Moreover, recent work by Hall, Benning, and Patrick (2004) examining associations sep- arately for the interpersonal and affective components of Factor 1 (Cooke & Michie, 2001; Hare, 2003) indicates that the interpersonal component in particular accounts for these relations with measures of adjustment/ resiliency. In addition, scores on PCL-R Factor 1 show negative associations with measures of empathy (Hare, 2003) and positive associations with con- structs reflecting a self-centered interpersonal style, including narcissistic personality and Machiavellianism (Hare, 1991; Harpur et al., 1989; Verona et al., 2001). Factor 1 also shows selective positive associations with indices of instrumental aggression (Patrick, Zempolich, & Levenston, 1997; Woodworth & Porter, 2002).
In contrast, Factor 2 of the PCL-R shows selective positive associations with child symptoms of DSM APD as well as markedly stronger associations than Factor 1 with adult APD symptoms (Hare, 2003; Verona et al., 2001). Factor 2 is associated much more strongly than PCL-R Factor 1 with crimi- nal history variables, such as overall frequency of offending (Hare, 2003). In addition, Smith and Newman (1990) reported that this component of PCL- R psychopathy was positively associated with alcohol and drug dependence, whereas Factor 1 of the PCL-R was unrelated to substance abuse (see also Reardon et al., 2002). Research has demonstrated selective positive associa- tions between PCL-R Factor 2 and various indices of reactive aggression (including child and adult fights, assault charges, and partner abuse; Patrick & Zempolich, 1998; Patrick et al., 1997; see also Woodworth & Porter, 2002). PCL-R Factor 2 is also related positively to suicidal behavior, whereas the unique variance in PCL-R Factor 1 tends to be negatively asso- ciated with suicidality (Verona, Hicks, & Patrick, 2005; Verona et al., 2001). In the domain of personality traits, ratings on PCL-R Factor 2 show robust positive correlations with aggression, impulsivity, and overall sensa- tion seeking (Hare, 1991; Harpur et al, 1989).
These divergent associations for PCL-R Factors 1 and 2 are notable for two variables considered to be facets of a single higher-order construct (e.g., Hare, 1991, 2003). Particularly striking are instances in which opposing
  
132
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

associations of the two PCL-R factors with criterion measures become stronger once their covariance (overlap) is removed. This phenomenon is known as cooperative suppression (Cohen & Cohen, 1975; Frick, Lilienfeld, Ellis, Loney, & Silverthorn, 1999; Paulhus, Robins, Trzesniewski, & Tracy, 2004). In such cases, true associations between the unique part of each pre- dictor variable and external criterion measures are muted or concealed by the variance that the two predictors share. The occurrence of suppressor effects, particularly cooperative suppressor effects, is conceptually important because it signifies the presence of distinctive underlying constructs embed- ded within a common measurement instrument (Paulhus et al., 2004).
Recently, Hicks and Patrick (2006) applied this formulation to an analysis of relations between the two PCL-R factors and facets of negative emotionality— including distress (or general anxiousness), fearfulness, and anger—as well as depression. For three of these four criterion variables (emotional distress, fear, depression), cooperative suppressor effects were found (i.e., associations for both PCL-R factors increased, in opposing directions, when the two were included concurrently in a prediction model). For the anger variable, a crossover suppression effect was evident (i.e., simultaneous inclusion of the two PCL-R factors in a prediction model resulted in a change in the direction of association for Factor 1—that is, from significantly positive to nonsignifi- cantly negative) and a nonsignificant increase in the positive association for Factor 2). Moreover, for all four criterion variables, prediction using the two PCL-R factors together was superior to that based on PCL-R total scores alone. In particular, for the distress, fear, and depression variables, PCL-R total scores provided negligible predictive power, whereas concurrent use of the two PCL-R factors yielded significant prediction. Hicks and Patrick noted that suppression effects for the two PCL-R factors have previously been reported for other criterion variables, including suicidal behavior (cooperative suppression; Verona et al., 2005) and alcohol/drug problems (crossover sup- pression; Smith & Newman, 1990).
As noted, the presence of suppressor effects, particularly instances of cooperative suppression, implies that the items of a measurement instrument presumed to index a single broad construct are actually tapping separate, distinctive underlying constructs. In the case of the PCL-R, the occurrence of suppressor effects for its two factors is actually consistent with Cleckley’s original idea that the syndrome of psychopathy entails the convergence of paradoxical dispositions toward psychological resiliency and behavioral maladjustment. Although the strategy used to select items for the PCL-R favored the emergence of a unidimensional criterion set, distinctive underly- ing constructs are nevertheless evident in terms of both internal structure and relations with external criterion measures. As discussed in the final section of this chapter, there is also evidence that these separable components of psy- chopathy may have distinctive etiologic underpinnings.
Prevalence of PCL-R–Defined Psychopathy. Because the PCL-R was developed for use in male correctional and forensic settings, firm prevalence
  
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 133

estimates are available primarily for samples of this kind. Using a total score cutoff of 30, the estimated base rate of PCL-R–defined psychopathy in male correctional and forensic populations is 15% to 25%, versus a base rate of 50% to 80% for DSM-defined APD (Hare, 2003, p. 92). For example, for a sample of 100 male prisoners from a medium-security institution in Canada, the DSM-IV APD field trial report listed base rates of 28% and 70%, respec- tively, for psychopathy as defined by an abbreviated version of the PCL-R and APD as defined by DSM-III-R criteria. Figure 6.1 depicts this asymme- try in prevalence rates for the two disorders. This difference in prevalence has been attributed to the fact that a PCL-R diagnosis of psychopathy requires the presence of prominent emotional-interpersonal features as well as prominent antisocial deviance (Hare, 2003; Widiger et al., 1996).
As noted earlier, the prevalence of APD among men in the general com- munity is estimated to be about 3% (APA, 2000). Because the PCL-R crite- ria are tailored to offender samples and limited efforts have been made to assess PCL-R psychopathy in community samples (for a notable exception, see Ishikawa, Raine, Lencz, Bihrle, & Lacasse, 2001), the prevalence of PCL- R–defined psychopathy in the community at large is essentially unknown. Using the median prevalence figures for prison settings (65% for APD and 20% for psychopathy, respectively), and assuming a comparable base rate of criminal psychopathy among APD individuals in the community, the esti- mated base rate for criminal psychopathy among community males would be approximately 1%. Using an abbreviated, screening version of the PCL- R (the PCL:SV; Hart et al., 1995), Farrington (2006) reported a base rate for psychopathy of approximately 2% (i.e., 8 cases out of 411) in a large sam- ple of community boys followed up to age 48.
However, such estimates do not include individuals who manifest core affective-interpersonal symptoms of psychopathy without meeting criteria for APD. Because the field lacks an agreed-upon set of criteria for diagnosing psychopathy in nonprisoners, population prevalence estimates for noncrim- inal psychopathy are unknown. Cleckley referred to such cases as “incom- plete manifestations” of the syndrome, but he did not speculate about prevalence. Hare (1993) suggested, with dramatic emphasis, that clinically documented cases of psychopathy
represent only the tip of a very large iceberg . . . The rest of the iceberg is to be found nearly everywhere—in business, the home, the profes- sions, the military, the arts, the entertainment industry, the news media, academe, and the blue-collar world. (p. 115)
However, he did not speculate about prevalence rates.
Women are incarcerated at much lower rates than men, and as noted earlier, the base rate of APD among women is estimated to be only a third of that among men (APA, 2000). In addition, reported base rates of PCL- R–defined psychopathy in female prisoner samples, employing a standard
  
134
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

total score cutoff of 30, tend to be lower than for males. Although some studies of incarcerated women have reported base rates within the range that is typical for incarcerated men, others have reported lower base rates (for a review, see Verona & Vitale, 2006). Studies of psychopathy and related con- structs in nonincarcerated samples have also yielded reliably lower preva- lence figures for women compared to men (Verona & Vitale, 2006). With regard to race/ethnicity, an initial study by Kosson, Smith, and Newman (1990) yielded evidence that overall PCL-R scores were higher among African-American offenders than they were among European-American offenders. A recent meta-analysis of studies of this kind (Skeem, Edens, Camp, & Colwell, 2004) revealed a small but significant effect size for this comparison. With regard to culture, there is evidence that American inmate samples generally score higher on the PCL-R than European inmate samples (Sullivan & Kosson, 2006).
Comorbidity With DSM Disorders. The DSM disorder most frequently associated with PCL-R–defined psychopathy is APD. As noted in the pre- ceding section, the relationship between PCL-R psychopathy and APD is asymmetric. Within offender samples, most individuals who meet criteria for a diagnosis of psychopathy (i.e., PCL-R total score > 30) also meet criteria for a DSM diagnosis of APD; in contrast, the majority of individuals who meet criteria for a diagnosis of APD do not meet PCL-R criteria for psy- chopathy (see Figure 6.1). Also, as noted earlier, the two PCL-R factors show an asymmetric association with APD: the social deviance (Factor 2) component of the PCL-R is related selectively to the child component of APD, and is also related more substantially to the adult component. This is because the behavioral features embodied in the child and adult criteria for APD overlap substantially with the behavioral features of psychopathy embodied in PCL-R Factor 2. By contrast, only one of the 15 child criteria for APD (lying) and only two of the seven adult criteria (deceitfulness and lack of remorse) intersect with the emotional-interpersonal features of psy- chopathy embodied in PCL-R Factor 1.
Factor 2 of the PCL-R is also associated selectively with substance use prob- lems (Reardon et al., 2002; Smith & Newman, 1990) and with features of bor- derline personality disorder (Shine & Hobson, 1997; Warren et al., 2003). In contrast, scores on PCL-R Factor 1 tend to be associated more strongly with diagnostic ratings of narcissistic personality disorder (Harpur et al., 1989; Hart & Hare, 1989, 1998) and histrionic personality disorder (Hart & Hare, 1989; Hildebrand & de Ruiter, 2004). In contrast with Cleckley’s description of psychopaths as individuals lacking in anxiousness and “psychoneurotic” features, overall scores on the PCL-R appear to be generally unrelated to depression and symptoms of anxiety disorders (Hare, 2003). However, as noted earlier, the two factors of the PCL-R show opposing, mutually suppres- sive relations with measures of anxiety and depression—that is, after control- ling for the overlapping variance between the two factors, Factor 1 shows negative associations with such measures, whereas Factor 2 shows positive
  
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 135

?% of nonprisoner (community) men are psychopaths
15% – 25% of prisoners
50% – 80% of prisoners
Psychopathy
          
Male Prisoners
∼3% of nonprisoner (community) men meet APD criteria
APD

Figure 6.1
Depiction of prevalence rates and diagnostic overlap for antisocial personality disorder, as defined by DSM-IV criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), and psychopathy, as defined by items of the Psychopathy Checklist—Revised (Hare, 2003), in male prisoner samples (shaded circle) and in the community at large (unshaded portions of circle and ellipse).
associations (Hicks & Patrick, 2006). The fact that the unique variance in Factor 1 is negatively associated with anxiety and depression suggests that this component of the PCL-R (particularly its interpersonal facet [Hall et al., 2004]) captures something of the positive adjustment and resiliency Cleckley described as characteristic of psychopaths. The positive associations for Factor 2, in contrast, are consistent with data indicating that the DSM diagnosis of APD is associated with an increased prevalence of anxiety and mood disorders (APA, 2000, p. 702; see also Krueger, 1999a).
Neurobiological Correlates. Neurobiological correlates of psychopathy have been studied mainly in relation to overall scores on the PCL-R, and in relation to the Cleckley global ratings of psychopathy that served as the ref- erent for the PCL-R. Historically, one of the most consistent findings— beginning with Lykken’s (1957) seminal study and continuing with Hare’s classic investigations of autonomic reactivity in psychopaths through the 1960s and 1970s—has been that individuals high in overall psychopathy show reduced electrodermal (skin conductance) reactivity to stressors of var- ious kinds, particularly cues signaling an impending noxious event (for reviews, see Arnett, 1997; Hare, 1978; Lorber, 2004; Siddle & Trasler,
136
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

1981). This finding has been interpreted as a reflection of a basic deficiency in anxiety or fear (Fowles, 1980; Hare, 1978; Lykken, 1957).
Another reliable finding in the literature, also consistent with the idea of a negative emotional reactivity deficit, is that high PCL-R psychopaths fail to show normal augmentation of the startle blink reflex during viewing of aversive visual stimuli (Herpertz et al., 2001; Levenston, Patrick, Bradley, & Lang, 2000; Patrick, 1994; Patrick, Bradley, & Lang, 1993; Sutton, Vitale, & Newman, 2002; Vanman, Mejia, Dawson, Schell, & Raine, 2003). In this case, reactivity differences have been tied specifically to elevations on the emotional-interpersonal factor of psychopathy. A lack of fear-potentiated startle is evident not just among individuals with high scores on both factors of the PCL-R but also among individuals who score high on Factor 1 only, whereas individuals who score high on Factor 2 alone show normal aug- mentation of the startle reflex during aversive cuing (Patrick, 1994; Patrick et al., 1993; Vanman et al., 2003). In contrast with electrodermal reactivity, which is a general index of sympathetic arousal, the startle blink reflex is a protective reaction that has been shown to increase with activation of the amygdala, a key component of the brain’s defensive (fear) system (see Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1990). Thus an absence of fear-potentiated startle implies a weakness in reactivity at this basic subcortical level. Consistent with this hypothesis, Blair and his colleagues have reported deficits among psychopathic individuals on behavioral tasks believed to be sensitive to amygdala function (for a review, see Blair, 2006), and recent neuroimaging research has demonstrated reduced amygdala activity during aversive condi- tioning in high PCL-R scorers (Veit et al., 2002).
A variety of studies employing other methodologies have yielded evidence of differences in brain reactivity or function among individuals high in over- all PCL-R psychopathy, including investigations of cerebral asymmetry (for a review, see Hare, 2003, pp. 124–126), brain event-related potential (ERP) studies (e.g., Kiehl, Hare, MacDonald, & Brink, 1999; Williamson, Harpur, & Hare, 1991), and structural and functional neuroimaging studies (for a review, see Raine & Yang, 2006). Nevertheless, studies examining the per- formance of high-psychopathy individuals on neuropsychological tests of frontal lobe function have not yielded reliable evidence of impairment (e.g., Hare, 1984; Hart, Forth, & Hare, 1990; for a review, see Rogers, 2006), nor have studies examining the P300 component of the event-related potential (for a review of conflicting findings, see Raine, 1993, but see also more recent work by Kiehl, Hare, Liddle, & MacDonald, 1999). This stands in contrast to evidence, cited earlier, that APD is reliably associated with deficits on tests of frontal lobe dysfunction and with reduced amplitude of the P300 brain potential response. Given that Factor 2 of the PCL-R is asso- ciated more strongly with APD and that the unique variance in PCL-R Factor 1 (i.e., that unrelated to Factor 2) shows positive associations with some measures of psychological adjustment, it is possible that the two fac- tors of the PCL-R are differentially related to performance on tests of frontal
  
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 137

lobe function and to P300 amplitude (i.e., deficits may be evident for Factor 2 only). However, this possibility remains to be examined (Rogers, 2006).
One type of impairment that has been related selectively to PCL-R Factor 2 is reduced autonomic reactivity during mental imagery of emotional situa- tions. Specifically, Patrick, Cuthbert, and Lang (1994) found reduced skin conductance and heart rate reactivity during imagery of fearful text scripts relative to neutral scripts in prisoners who were high on Factor 2 of the PCL-R, regardless of whether they were low or high on Factor 1, compared with prisoners low on Factor 2. The authors’ interpretation was that the anti- social deviance component of psychopathy is associated with reduced auto- matic-elaborative processing of symbolic (in this case, linguistic) affective information (see also Patrick & Lang, 1999). Another neurobiological indi- cator that appears to be associated preferentially with PCL-R Factor 2 is reduced functioning of the serotonergic neurotransmitter system, which as noted previously is also associated with impulsive aggressive behavior in anti- social individuals (Dolan & Anderson, 2003; Minzenberg & Siever, 2006).
Alternative Self-Report–Based Conceptualizations of Psychopathy
Aside from issues regarding its coverage of the positive adjustment fea- tures of psychopathy emphasized by Cleckley, other limitations of the PCL-R include the fact that it is time consuming to administer and that sev- eral of its items (those scored on the basis of criminal offense behaviors) are not applicable to individuals outside of forensic and correctional settings. For these reasons, other approaches to the assessment of psychopathy have been sought, many of them self-report based. Because they consist of more general queries about attitudes and behavioral tendencies, standard ques- tionnaire inventories can be used across a range of different participant sam- ples. Self-report questionnaires are also efficient to administer and score and thus are amenable to large-scale screening studies.
Lilienfeld and Fowler (2006) provided an authoritative review of self- report instruments developed to assess psychopathy. As these authors point out, most existing inventories for the assessment of psychopathy correlate much more strongly with the antisocial deviance component of psychopathy embodied in PCL-R Factor 2 than they do with the emotional-interpersonal (Factor 1) component (see also Hare, 1991, 2003). These inventories include the Psychopathic Deviate (Pd) scale of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI; Hathaway & McKinley, 1943), the Socialization (So) scale of the California Psychological Inventory (Gough, 1960), the Self- Report Psychopathy (SRP) scale (Hare, 1985), and the Primary and Secondary Psychopathy scales developed by Levenson, Kiehl, and Fitzpatrick (1995). However, one self-report inventory that appears to tap the two components of psychopathy more equally is the Psychopathic Personality
  
138
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Inventory (PPI; Lilienfeld, 1990; Lilienfeld & Andrews, 1996). For example, Poythress, Edens, and Lilienfeld (1998) reported significant correlations of .56 and .44, respectively, between overall scores on the PPI and scores on Factors 1 and 2 of the PCL-R for a sample of 50 youthful offenders. Also, recent factor analyses of the subscales of the PPI (Benning, Patrick, Hicks, Blonigen, & Krueger, 2003; Benning, Patrick, Salekin, & Leistico, 2005) have revealed evidence of two underlying factors with distinctive external correlates. Other research examining the validity of these two PPI factors has yielded findings with interesting conceptual and etiologic implications. For these reasons, we focus the remainder of this section on recent studies that have explored the constructs underlying the two distinctive factors of the PPI.
The Psychopathic Personality Inventory and Its Factors. The PPI was developed using a personality-oriented approach in which the goal was to capture dispositional tendencies or traits considered central to psychopathy (Lilienfeld & Andrews, 1996). This contrasts with inventories such as the So and MMPI-Pd scales, which were developed using an empirical, contrasted- groups strategy. In the development of the PPI, a comprehensive survey of the literature was performed to identify all relevant constructs related to psy- chopathy, and items were developed to index these constructs. Iterative rounds of data collection and analysis were undertaken to refine the initial item set as well as to clarify the target constructs. The PPI comprises 187 questions, each answered using a four-point scale (false, mostly false, mostly true, true). The inventory yields a total score index of psychopathy, as well as scores on eight subscales reflecting specific elements of the psychopathy con- struct (see Table 6.4). The eight subscales of the PPI demonstrate good inter- nal consistencies (.70 to .91; Blonigen, Carlson, Krueger, & Patrick, 2003; Lilienfeld & Andrews, 1996), as well as high test-retest reliabilities (.82 to .94 across a mean retest interval of 26 days; Lilienfeld & Andrews, 1996).
As noted, the PPI was developed to comprehensively index personality trait constructs relevant to the domain of psychopathy (Lilienfeld, 1990). Distinctive subscales were developed to index these trait constructs without a priori assumptions about their underlying structure. Recently, Benning et al. (2003) examined the structure of the PPI subscales in a male community sam- ple (N = 353) using principal axis factor analysis and found evidence of two dominant factors accounting for a substantial proportion of the covariance (50.9%) among seven of the eight scales. The Social Potency, Stress Immunity, and Fearlessness subscales loaded more predominantly on one factor (PPI-I), and the Impulsive Nonconformity, Blame Externalization, Machiavellian Egocentricity, and Carefree Nonplanfulness subscales loaded more predomi- nantly on the other (PPI-II). In contrast with the two factors of the PCL-R, which are moderately correlated, these two PPI factors were uncorrelated (r = −.07). The eighth subscale of the PPI, Coldheartedness, did not load appreciably on either of these factors. This factor structure was replicated by Benning, Patrick, Salekin, et al. (2005) in a sample of male and female college
  
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 139

Table 6.4 Subscales of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory (PPI; Lilienfeld, 1990)
Subscale Label # of Items Description of a High Scorer
Social Potency1 24 Stress Immunity1 11 Fearlessness1 19 Impulsive Nonconformity2 17 Blame Externalization2 18 Machiavellian Egocentricity2 30 Carefree Nonplanfulness2 20 Coldheartedness 21
Able to influence and dominate others
Minimal experience of anxiety
Takes risks; seeks thrills in danger
Reckless; rebellious; unconventional
Blames others; sees self as victim
Aggressive; selfish; exploitative
Present oriented; lacks forethought and planning
Unsentimental; insensitive; lacks imaginative capacity
1Subscales comprising first factor of the PPI (PPI-I; Benning, Patrick, Hicks, Blonigen, & Krueger, 2003).
2Subscales comprising the second factor of the PPI (PPI-II; Benning, Patrick, Hicks, Blonigen, & Krueger, 2003). Note. The Coldheartedness subscale does not load appreciably on either of the two PPI factors.

students (N = 326), and by Ross, Benning, Patrick, Thompson, and Thurston (2005) in a mixed sample comprising male and female college students (n = 134) and male and female prisoners (n = 159).
Personality and Behavioral Correlates of the PPI Factors. The two dis- tinctive factors of the PPI show conceptually meaningful, and in many cases diverging, patterns of relations with a wide range of criterion measures. Benning et al. (2003) reported that PPI-I and PPI-II showed opposing associ- ations with indices of adjustment, including verbal intelligence, educational attainment, and occupational status and income. Whereas the direction of association in each case was negative for PPI-II, it was positive for PPI-I (i.e., higher PPI-I was associated with better adjustment). In support of the idea that the two PPI factors reflect different facets of an overarching psychopathy construct, PPI-I and PPI-II both showed significant positive correlations with adult symptoms of antisocial personality disorder (r = .15 and .27, respec- tively; p < .01). However, child symptoms of APD were significantly associ- ated only with PPI-II. PPI-II was uniquely associated with indices of alcohol and drug problems. Likewise, Patrick, Edens, Poythress, Lilienfeld, and Benning (2006) reported positive associations with the Alcohol Problems and Drug Problems scales of the Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI; Morey, 1991) only for PPI-II. PPI-II (but not PPI-I) showed robust correlations (in all cases positive) with the PAI Antisocial Features, Aggression, Borderline Features, and Suicidal Ideation scales. Furthermore, PPI-II showed significant positive correlations with the Anxiety Disorders and Somatization scales of the PAI, whereas correlations for PPI-I with these scales were significantly

140
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

negative. In contrast, PPI-I was associated uniquely (positively) with the PAI Dominance scale. Taken together, these findings indicate that PPI-I taps aspects of social efficacy and adjustment as well as deviancy, whereas PPI-II is associated more uniformly with deviancy and maladjustment (i.e., child and adult antisociality, substance abuse problems, heightened anxiety and soma- tization, and suicidal ideation).
Ross et al. (2005) examined relations between the two PPI factors and self- report measures of executive functioning along with alternative self-report indices of psychopathy. The measure of executive functioning was the Frontal Systems Behavior Scale (FrSBe; Grace & Malloy, 2001), a 46-item inventory that assesses personality and behavioral characteristics associated with frontal lobe damage. PPI-I showed a modest positive correlation with the Disinhibition subscale of the FrSBe, a negative association of comparable magnitude with the Executive Function subscale (reflecting better function), and no correla- tion with the Apathy subscale. In contrast, PPI-II showed robust positive asso- ciations with all three subscales of the FrSBe. Ross et al. also examined relations between the two PPI factors and FFM psychopathy prototype scores derived from a revised version of the NEO-PI inventory (NEO-PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992). The computation of psychopathy prototype scores was based on the work of Miller, Lynam, Widiger, and Leukefeld (2001), who com- posed the prototypical personality profile of the psychopath in FFM terms on the basis of input from experts in the field, then devised a methodology for quantifying the resemblance of individual profiles to this expert-generated prototype. Ross et al. found that PPI-I and PPI-II, though uncorrelated, each showed a robust positive association with FFM prototype scores (r = .61 and .47, respectively); taken together, the two PPI factors predicted psychopathy prototype scores at a reliability of .76. This pattern of results provides further support for the idea that the two factors of the PPI tap distinctive components of an overarching psychopathy construct.
Some other recent work has examined the correlates of PPI factor scores estimated from scores on a general inventory of personality, the MPQ (Tellegen, in press). Data for both measures were available in the study carried out by Benning et al. (2003), and these authors noted that scores on PPI-I and PPI-II could be predicted with a high degree of accuracy (r = .70 and .67, respectively) from the lower-order trait scales of the MPQ. The MPQ trait scales that contributed to the prediction of PPI-I scores were Social Potency (positive), Stress Reaction (negative), and Harm Avoidance (negative); the MPQ trait scales that contributed to the prediction of PPI-II scores were Alienation (positive), Aggression (positive), Control (Negative), Traditionalism (negative), Social Closeness (negative), and Absorption (positive). Benning, Patrick, Blonigen, Hicks, and Iacono (2005) used the regression models from Benning et al. (2003) to estimate PPI factor scores in three study samples for which MPQ data were available as well as scores on a variety of psychopa- thy-relevant criterion measures.5 The study samples included a sample of male and female undergraduates recruited from psychology classes (N = 346),
  
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 141

a large sample of male and female twins recruited from the community (N = 1,122), and a sample of male prisoners recruited from a federal correc- tional facility (N = 218).
A variety of interesting findings emerged from analyses of relations between MPQ-estimated PPI scores and available criterion measures in these samples. Within both the undergraduate and prisoner samples, higher scores on PPI-I (estimated by means of the MPQ) were selectively associated with lower temperamental fearfulness and distress and higher activity and socia- bility; higher scores on the thrill–adventure seeking component of sensation seeking (Zuckerman, 1979); and high narcissism. In contrast, within these samples, higher PPI-II scores were selectively associated with higher tem- peramental anger and impulsivity; higher scores on the boredom susceptibil- ity component of sensation seeking; and lower socialization as indexed by scores on the So scale. Within both the community and prisoner samples, higher PPI-I scores were associated with lesser symptoms of phobia (partic- ularly social phobia) and lesser symptoms of depression, whereas higher PPI- II scores were associated with greater symptoms of depression and also greater symptoms of alcohol and drug dependence. Interestingly, within the prisoner sample, scores on both PPI factors showed significant negative cor- relations with a measure of emotional empathy.
Scores on the PCL-R were also available for the prisoner sample in this study, permitting an evaluation of the association between the two factors of the PPI (estimated by means of the MPQ) and those of the PCL-R. Simple cor- relations revealed a significant correlation between PPI-I and PCL-R Factor 1 only, whereas PPI-II showed significant zero-order associations with both PCL-R factors. When the overlap between the two PCL-R factors was con- trolled for (using partial correlations), PPI-I showed a significant positive association with PCL-R Factor 1 only (r = .28), and PPI-II showed a signifi- cant positive association with PCL-R Factor 2 only (r = .31). Supplementary analyses of PCL-R scores based on Cooke and Michie’s (2001) three-factor model revealed that PPI-I was related most strongly to the interpersonal com- ponent of Factor 1 (i.e., compared with the affective component). This find- ing is interesting because, as noted earlier, the interpersonal component of the PCL-R appears to account largely for associations between Factor 1 and indices of psychological adjustment and resiliency.
The findings from these studies provide strong support for the validity of the two PPI factors as indices of distinctive components of the psychopathy construct. Although parallels are evident between the PPI factors and the PCL-R factors in terms of their relations with external criterion measures (e.g., the first factor of both instruments is selectively related to dominance, narcissism, and low anxiousness; the second factor of both is selectively related to impulsivity, aggression, low socialization, and alcohol and drug problems), the parallels become evident for some measures (e.g., anxiety) only when the overlap between the PCL-R factors is removed using partial corre- lations. Moreover, correlations between corresponding factors for the two
  
142
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

instruments are only modest in magnitude (~.3, based on the findings of Benning, Patrick, Blonigen, et al., 2005), even after controlling for overlap between the PCL-R factors. One variable contributing to this modest corre- spondence may be the fact that scores on the PCL-R and PPI are derived using different assessment methods (i.e., interview and examination of file records versus self-report). This “method variance” (Campbell & Fiske, 1959) would operate to attenuate relations between the two even if they were indexing the same construct. However, a key divergence between the two that is not eas- ily explained by method variance is the correlated versus uncorrelated nature of their factor structures. What might account for this difference?
One possible explanation concerns the item selection strategies that were used in developing the two instruments. As discussed earlier, candidate items for the PCL-R were retained if they contributed to prediction of global rat- ings of psychopathy based on Cleckley’s criteria and if they showed good psy- chometric properties (meaning, presumably, positive correlations with other candidate items such that they contributed to internal consistency of the over- all inventory). One apparent consequence of this selection strategy is that indicators of positive psychological adjustment included among Cleckley’s criteria were omitted from the PCL-R. In contrast, candidate items for the PPI were selected to serve as indicators of target constructs within the broad domain of psychopathy-related personality traits. Although the trait con- structs themselves were refined across separate rounds of data collection and items were selected to form internally consistent subscales, there was no a pri- ori requirement that the items as a whole or the subscales be indicators of a single, unitary construct. Thus, the items of the PPI were not constrained to tap one broad, unitary construct. As a result, the two factors of the PPI appear to be indexing components of the psychopathy construct in a more clearly differentiated way. One component (PPI-II) taps impulsive, aggressive (externalizing) tendencies. The other (PPI-I) taps a construct that encompasses aspects of positive adjustment (higher agency, lower anxiety and depression, higher educational and occupational attainment) as well as aspects of deviancy (narcissism, thrill–adventure seeking, low empathy).
Neurobiological Correlates. Although the two distinctive factors of the PPI have become a topic of investigation only recently, findings regarding the neurobiological correlates of these factors have begun to emerge. Benning, Patrick, and Iacono (2005) examined physiological reactivity of groups selected due to their low or high scores on one or the other factor of the PPI as estimated by scores on the MPQ (cf. Benning, Patrick, Blonigen, et al., 2005) in an affective picture viewing paradigm. Participants who scored very high on PPI-I showed a deviant pattern of startle reactivity resembling that of offenders with high scores on PCL-R Factor 1 (i.e., an absence of fear- potentiated startle), whereas participants who scored very high on PPI-II showed no such deviation. This finding suggests that the PPI-I construct taps the diminished emotional reactivity (in particular, reduced defensive reactiv- ity to aversive cues) associated with PCL-R Factor 1. In contrast, participants
  
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 143

who scored very high on PPI-II showed generally smaller electrodermal responses to picture stimuli (i.e., whether affective or neutral) compared with participants who scored low on PPI-II, suggesting reduced overall arousabil- ity for individuals high in the impulsive-antisocial tendencies associated with this PPI factor (cf. Raine, 1997). This finding of reduced electrodermal reac- tivity in individuals who scored high on PPI-II was also reported in a recent study by Verschuere, Crombez, de Clercq, and Koster (2005).
In another recent investigation, Gordon, Baird, and End (2004) used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine the brain responses of high and low PPI scorers during performance of a recognition task in which the focus of attention was directed on some trials to the iden- tity of a target face and on other trials to the type of affective expression appearing on the target face. Individuals scoring high on PPI-I showed lesser activation in the right amygdala and affiliated regions of the frontal cortex (i.e., right inferior frontal cortex, medial prefrontal cortex) during emotion processing trials, as well as greater activation in the visual cortex and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, compared with individuals scoring low on PPI-I. These groups did not differ in brain activation during identity pro- cessing trials. The authors’ interpretation was that high PPI-I scorers relied on brain regions associated with cognition and perception to perform the emotion recognition task, whereas low PPI-I scorers relied more on regions known to be involved in affective evaluation and reactivity. Analyses of acti- vation patterns for individuals grouped in terms of PPI-II scores revealed, if anything, evidence of enhanced amygdala activation in high PPI-II scorers compared with low scorers. This finding is consistent with the idea that anti- social individuals lacking the core affective-interpersonal features of psy- chopathy show normal or enhanced negative emotional reactivity (Patrick, 2007; Patrick & Lang, 1999).
Etiologic Perspectives on Psychopathy
A variety of etiologic models have been advanced to account for the syn- drome of psychopathy (for recent reviews, see Blackburn, 2006; Fowles & Dindo, 2006; Hare, 2003; Hiatt & Newman, 2006). These can be grouped into two broad categories. One category consists of theories that propose some underlying emotional deficit or deviation. For example, Cleckley (1976) postulated that psychopaths are generally deficient in their capacity for affec- tive experience:
Behind the exquisitely deceptive mask of the psychopath the emotional alteration we feel appears to be primarily one of degree, a consistent leveling of response to petty ranges and an incapacity to react with suf- ficient seriousness to achieve much more than pseudoexperience or quasi-experience. (p. 383)
  
144
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

According to Cleckley, this underlying deficiency (referenced directly in his “poverty in major affective reactions” criterion) accounts for the super- ficial, manipulative quality of the psychopath’s interactions with others as well as the whimsical nature of his or her behavior in other domains. According to this view, the psychopath’s “mask of sanity” arises from an adaptive inclination to mimic the affective displays of others so as to “blend in” and achieve basic goals, with no true recognition that these reactions are actually simulated:
Let us say that, despite his otherwise perfect functioning, the major emotional accompaniments are absent or so attenuated as to count for little. Of course, he is unaware of this, just as everyone is bound, except theoretically, to be unaware of that which is out of his scale or order or mode of experience. (p. 371)
Most other affective models of psychopathy have postulated a more spe- cific impairment in negative emotional reactivity. For example, Lykken (1957) presented experimental evidence that individuals diagnosed as psy- chopathic according to Cleckley’s criteria were deficient in anxiety responses. In his earlier work, Hare (1965, 1978) proposed that psychopathy is marked by an abnormally steep gradient of fear arousal (i.e., punishment cues that are remote in time fail to inhibit the behavior of psychopaths because such cues do not elicit normal anticipatory fear). Fowles (1980), referencing Gray’s (1971) neurobiological theory of motivation, postulated that true (“pri- mary”) psychopaths have a weak behavioral inhibition (anxiety) system but a normal behavioral activation (appetitive) system. Patrick and his colleagues (Patrick, 1994; Levenston et al., 2000) proposed that psychopathy entails a heightened threshold for activation of the defensive motivational system. Blair (2006) proposed that the core features of psychopathy arise from dys- function in the subcortical amygdala. In his model, Blair acknowledged the possibility that psychopathy might entail some impairment in positive as well as negative emotional reactivity (cf. Cleckley, 1976). Indeed, some studies have reported evidence of reduced autonomic and electrocortical reactivity to pleasurable stimuli in PCL-R–defined psychopaths (e.g., Verona, Patrick, Curtin, Bradley, & Lang, 2004; Williamson, Harpur, et al., 1991).
The other category of theories consists of those that have postulated some sort of higher cognitive processing deficit or deviation in psychopathy. For example, Newman and his colleagues (Newman, 1998; Patterson & Newman, 1993) have proposed that psychopathy is characterized by a core deficit in response modulation, defined as the ability to switch from an ongo- ing (dominant) action set to an alternative mode of responding when envi- ronmental cues signal the need for a shift. An alternative perspective is that psychopaths are deficient in the ability to process peripheral cues when their attention is prioritized toward specific task-relevant cues in the environment (Jutai & Hare, 1993; Kosson & Newman, 1986). Kosson (1996, 1998), in
  
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 145

a refinement of this hypothesis, suggested that psychopaths show impaired processing of secondary stimulus features primarily under task conditions that promote activation of the left hemisphere. Other work has focused on the idea that the dissociation between what psychopaths say and what they actually do reflects some underlying abnormality in language processing. Studies of this kind have focused on deviations in lateralized verbal process- ing tasks (e.g., Day & Wong, 1996; Hare & McPherson, 1984) and abnor- malities in the use of contextual or associative elements of language such as abstraction, connotation, and metaphor (e.g., Brinkley, Bernstein, and Newman, 1999; Brinkley, Newman, Harpur, & Johnson, 1999; Williamson, 1991). It should be noted that work in this area intersects with affective models of psychopathy in that a number of language processing studies have yielded evidence of deviations in the processing of emotional words in psy- chopaths (e.g., Lorenz & Newman, 2002; Williamson, Harpur, et al., 1991).
In addition to these affective and cognitive processing models, one other theoretical perspective warrants mention. In contrast with many of the afore- mentioned models, which emphasize underlying deficits, this perspective views psychopathy as an evolutionary adaptive life strategy that enhances reproductive success (Harris, Skilling, & Rice, 2001; Mealey, 1995). From this perspective, the behavior of psychopaths reflects a cheating strategy that, although socially disruptive, is advantageous from an evolutionary stand- point because it promotes sexual contacts with multiple partners and yields large numbers of offspring. It should be noted that this theoretical position is not necessarily at odds with models that view the affective deviation in psychopathy as a genotypic extreme of normal temperament that favors a goal-oriented approach over avoidance of risk (e.g., Lykken, 1995; Patrick, 2001, 2007).
Integration: A Dual-Process Perspective on the Relation Between Antisocial ___________________ Personality Disorder and Psychopathy
A theoretical model that provides an integrative perspective on differing facets of the psychopathy construct and on relations between psychopathy and impulse control disorders, including APD and substance abuse and dependence, is the dual-process model of psychopathy. This model was first proposed by Patrick and Lang (1999) to account for diverging relations between components of PCL-R psychopathy and indices of physiological reactivity within different task paradigms. Subsequent elaborations of the model have appeared in publications by Patrick and his colleagues (Patrick, 2001, 2007; Patrick, Hicks, Krueger, & Lang, 2005). A related model that draws on this research base as well as on findings from the developmental literature was proposed recently by Fowles and Dindo (2006).
  
146
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

The dual-process model posits that different etiologic mechanisms under- lie distinctive facets of psychopathy. Cleckley characterized psychopathy as a severe behavioral pathology masked by a veneer of normalcy. The “mask” component of the disorder includes aspects of positive psychological func- tioning and a superficial but engaging affective-interpersonal style. From the dual-process perspective, the mask that Cleckley described reflects an extreme temperament disposition that entails an underlying weakness in emotional reactivity, particularly defensive (fear) reactivity. Neurobiologically, this tem- perament style is presumed to reflect differences in the functioning of core affect systems (e.g., the amygdala). In terms of personality constructs, this temperament disposition is marked by a blend of traits, including social dom- inance, low trait anxiousness, and affective fearlessness (i.e., diminished reac- tivity to threat or danger as opposed to general sensation seeking). In contrast, the behavioral deviance component of psychopathy reflects exter- nalizing vulnerability—that is, the broad factor that accounts for the covari- ance among various impulse control problems, including child and adolescent antisocial behavior and substance use disorders. From a neurobiological standpoint, this vulnerability reflects deviations in the functioning of higher brain systems that operate to regulate emotion and guide decision making and action in situations involving competing cues. In terms of personality traits, high externalizing is marked by traits of impulsiveness (low constraint) and high negative emotionality (particularly aggression and alienation).
Relations between the two factors of the PCL-R and the latent external- izing factor of general psychopathology were examined in a study conducted by Patrick, Hicks, et al. (2005). Scores on the externalizing factor were esti- mated from child and adult symptoms of APD, measures of alcohol and drug abuse and dependence, and scores on the constraint factor of the MPQ (cf. Krueger et al., 2002). Zero-order correlations between the latent exter- nalizing factor and the two psychopathy factors modeled as latent variables (i.e., using item parcels; cf. Hare & Neumann, 2006) were .44 for Factor 1 and .84 for Factor 2. In addition, a regression-based structural model was used to examine associations between the unique variance in each psy- chopathy factor and the externalizing factor (i.e., the association of each psy- chopathy factor with externalizing after controlling for the other psychopathy factor). Within this model, the partial association between Factor 2 and the externalizing factor approached unity, whereas the associ- ation of the unique variance in Factor 1 with externalizing was nonsignifi- cant. When the model was rerun with Factor 1 parsed into its interpersonal and affective facets, neither facet showed a significant association with exter- nalizing after controlling for Factor 2. The results of this study confirm that the antisocial deviance component of the PCL-R taps the broad externaliz- ing factor, with which APD has also been associated.
As noted in the section on APD, behavior genetics research points to a coherent, genetic basis for the externalizing factor. In addition, neuropsycho- logical and psychophysiological studies indicate that externalizing disorders
  
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 147

are associated with alterations in higher brain function, as evidenced by impaired performance on frontal lobe tasks and reduced brain potential response in cognitive processing tasks. The best-established brain response indicator of externalizing psychopathology is the P300 component of the event-related potential (Iacono et al., 2002; Patrick, Bernat, et al., 2006). The close association between PCL-R Factor 2 and externalizing suggests that it would be fruitful to test for an association between P300 response amplitude and this component of the PCL-R specifically, with some consideration of potential moderating effects of age (Hill & Steinhauer, 2001). Research along these lines could help resolve inconsistencies in the literature on psychopathy and brain response (cf. Raine, 1993).
Etiologic models that focus more broadly on cognitive processing deficits may prove to be especially relevant to the antisocial deviance (externalizing) component of psychopathy. For example, Newman’s conceptualization of response modulation is reminiscent of processes that have been posited to underlie a brain potential effect known as the error-related negativity (ERN). The ERN is a negative-polarity scalp potential that peaks within approxi- mately 100 milliseconds following an incorrect response in a speeded reac- tion time task. It is theorized to reflect an error detection (Scheffers, Coles, Bernstein, Gehring, & Donchin, 1996) or conflict monitoring (Carter et al., 1998) process that serves a self-corrective function in the context of perfor- mance of an ongoing task. Brain source localization studies have converged on the anterior cingulate cortex as its probable generator (Holroyd, Dien, & Coles, 1998; Luu, Flaisch, & Tucker, 2000; Miltner, Braun, & Coles, 1997). Recent research indicates that individuals high in externalizing tendencies show reduced ERN compared with individuals low in externalizing tenden- cies (Hall, Bernat, & Patrick, in press). Based on the close association between PCL-R Factor 2 and externalizing, one might predict that this com- ponent of the PCL-R would show a selective association with ERN response. Consistent with this hypothesis, Dikman and Allen (2000) reported that individuals who scored low on the So scales, which as noted earlier relate exclusively to Factor 2 of the PCL-R, showed reduced ERN response in a speeded reaction time paradigm.
Based on the external correlates of the second factor of Lilienfeld’s (1990) PPI—which include impulsivity and aggression in the domain of personality, and alcohol and drug abuse as well as child and adult antisocial deviance in the realm of problem behaviors—one would also predict a selective associa- tion between this component of self-report psychopathy and the broad exter- nalizing factor of DSM psychopathology. Blonigen, Hicks, et al. (2005) recently examined this issue in a large sample of male and female partici- pants recruited from the community. Participants consisted of monozygotic and dizygotic twins, permitting an analysis of both phenotypic and genetic associations between scores on the two PPI factors (estimated using the trait scales of the MPQ) and scores on the externalizing factor, indexed as a composite of symptoms of child and adult antisocial behavior and alcohol,
  
148
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

nicotine, and drug dependence (assessed via diagnostic interview). A robust positive phenotypic association between PPI-II and externalizing scores was found for both men and women (r = .36 and .40, respectively; p < .001),6 whereas corresponding associations between PPI-I and externalizing were low (r = .15 and .04) and significant for men only (p < .05). Significant genetic correlations were also found between PPI-II and externalizing in both gender groups (r = .45 and .52 for men and women, respectively), indicating significant overlap in genetic contributions to the two variables; corre- sponding associations for PPI-I were again smaller and significant for men only. These results indicate that PPI-II is selectively associated with exter- nalizing psychopathology in both women and men, and that this association is mediated to a significant extent by common genetic influences.
From the standpoint of the dual-process model, individuals high in exter- nalizing vulnerability only, while impulsive, aggressive, and antisocially deviant, would not exhibit the full psychopathic syndrome described by Cleckley. The other key ingredient in the disorder is an underlying tempera- mental disposition marked by agency, social dominance, emotional resiliency, and a heightened threshold for defensive (fear) activation. This temperamental style moderates the expression of externalizing to yield the clinical picture of an apparently well-adjusted, likable individual who is nevertheless untrust- worthy and self-serving in his or her relations with others and generally capri- cious in his or her behavior. For ease of reference, and in recognition of prominent theories of psychopathy that have emphasized deficits in defensive (fear) reactivity, I will refer to this temperamental disposition as low trait fear. However, it should be borne in mind that this affective disposition might entail some weaknesses in reactivity to positive appetitive stimuli as well as aversive stimuli (cf. Blair, 2006; Verona, Patrick, Curtin, Lang, & Bradley, 2004).
The conceptualization of this underlying temperament disposition emerges from studies of the psychometric and neurobiological correlates of PCL-R Factor 1 (its unique variance, in particular) and of the corresponding factor of Lilienfeld’s (1990) PPI. Although these factors are not isomorphic (i.e., PPI-I correlates mainly with the interpersonal component of Factor 1, and only modestly so; Benning, Patrick, Blonigen, et al., 2005), a variety of par- allels are evident in their relations with external criterion measures. As described earlier, both are associated positively with personality measures reflecting dominance, agency more broadly (including traits of well-being and achievement in addition to dominance; cf. Patrick, Curtin, & Tellegen, 2002), and narcissism, and negatively with measures of trait anxiousness (stress reac- tivity) and empathy. Mirroring Cleckley’s positive adjustment criteria, both are also associated negatively with anxiety disorder symptoms, depression, and indices of suicidality. Indeed, Blonigen et al. (2005) reported a negative genetic association between scores on PPI-I and symptoms of anxiety and depression. With regard to neurobiological correlates, elevations on both PCL-R Factor 1 and PPI-I have been associated with a lack of fear-potentiated startle during exposure to aversive cues and with reduced amygdala reactiv- ity to emotional stimuli in brain imaging studies.
  
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 149

One key question that arises from this dual-process perspective is how to think about individuals who achieve high overall scores on the PCL-R. One possibility is that this group comprises a mixture of individuals who typify one or the other etiologic process (i.e., high externalizing, low trait fear). This possibility was evaluated in a recent study by Hicks, Markon, Patrick, Krueger, and Newman (2004), who tested for the presence of psychopathy subgroups by using a sophisticated (model-based) cluster-analytic technique to categorize MPQ personality profiles of 96 male offenders with high over- all PCL-R scores. The analysis yielded evidence of two subgroups: an “aggressive” subgroup characterized by high overall negative emotionality (especially aggression) and low overall constraint, and a “stable” subgroup characterized by high agency (i.e., elevated well-being, social potency, and achievement) and low stress reactivity. The profiles of these two subgroups closely resembled those associated with the externalizing and low trait fear constructs, respectively. Notably, the proportion of aggressive psychopaths (68.8%) was over twice that in the stable group (31.2%), indicating that the PCL-R as a whole is weighted toward the detection of individuals who exhibit extreme externalizing tendencies (particularly high-aggressive exter- nalizers; cf. Krueger et al., in press). Consistent with this finding, in the aforementioned study of relations between PCL-R scores and externalizing (Patrick, Hicks, et al., 2005), the correlation between the PCL-R as a whole and scores on the latent externalizing factor was .7. However, the findings of Hicks et al. (2004) indicate that individuals resembling Cleckley’s psy- chologically well-adjusted prototype are also represented among high PCL-R scorers. It seems reasonable to suppose that individuals who more purely exemplify the low trait fear diathesis (i.e., without accompanying externalizing vulnerability) would be more strongly represented among “successful” psychopathic individuals who achieve positions of stature within the community (e.g., corporate executives, political leaders; see Hall & Benning, 2006; Hare, 1993; Lykken, 1995).
Another key issue is how the affective facet of PCL-R Factor 1 (encompass- ing absence of remorse, lack of empathy, shallow affect, and failure to accept responsibility for actions) maps onto this dual-process model. The variance in this facet overlaps statistically both with the interpersonal facet and with the behavioral deviance embodied in Factor 2, so one possibility is that deficient affectivity is associated with externalizing vulnerability as well as with low trait fear. In the case of externalizing vulnerability, affective deficits may arise sec- ondarily to impairments in higher brain systems (cf. Davidson, Putnam, & Larson, 2000; Patrick & Lang, 1999). However, the unique variance in the affective facet of the PCL-R must also be considered. This variance relates min- imally to personality variables and also shows some association with low social closeness (Hall et al., 2004). The unique variance in the affective facet also shows some relationship to criminal offense behavior, particularly violent types of offenses—suggesting that it taps something of the detached, coldhearted nature of the criminal psychopath. Fowles and Dindo (2006) theorized that this affective component of PCL-R psychopathy arises in part from negative
  
150
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

relationship experiences early in life (with parental figures, in particular) that operate separately from the diatheses underlying impulsive-externalizing ten- dencies and fearless temperament. Further research is needed to establish whether a separate etiologic mechanism underlies this affective facet of PCL-R psychopathy, and the extent to which the unique variance in this facet (which appears to reflect aggressiveness and interpersonal detachment) is part of Cleckley’s original conceptualization of the disorder.
Approaches to Treatment of Antisocial
Personality Disorder and Psychopathy _________________
Reviews of the literature on the treatment of antisocial and psychopathic individuals have generally emphasized the difficulties inherent in treating such individuals and the ineffectiveness of psychological interventions with these disorders (e.g., Barley, 1986; Blackburn, 1993, chaps. 13–14; Harris & Rice, 2006). For example, Harris and Rice (2006) concluded that standard psychological interventions for offenders, such as individual cognitive- behavioral therapy, group psychotherapy, and therapeutic community pro- grams, are completely ineffective with psychopaths. In fact, these authors cited findings from some outcome studies indicating that psychopathic indi- viduals who participated in psychotherapeutic treatment actually reoffended at higher rates after release than psychopathic individuals who did not receive such treatment. As an explanation for this counterintuitive finding, Harris and Rice suggested that participation in psychotherapy led to improve- ments in the ability of psychopaths to exploit the weaknesses of other people as a function of their increased understanding of others’ motives without accompanying increases in emotional sensitivity. However, Rice and Harris did allow that some highly structured forms of behavioral therapy (e.g., behavior modification aimed at specific skills development, multisystemic therapy targeting influences in the family and local community) might prove more effective with this population—while acknowledging that the outcome studies required to evaluate this possibility remain to be done. However, other authors have identified cross-situational generalization and treatment maintenance as problems in highly structured behavioral interventions, par- ticularly when it comes to the treatment of chronic offenders (Barley, 1986; Blackburn, 1993, chap. 13).
The kinds of cognitive and emotional processing impairments that have been posited to underlie psychopathy may pose a basic obstacle to the effec- tiveness of purely psychological interventions. As has been suggested with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, induction of changes in the function- ing of underlying neural systems—for example, through pharmacological means—may be required first before cognitive and behavioral interventions can have a significant impact on maladaptive behaviors associated with APD and psychopathy. In this regard, evidence has accumulated for the effective- ness of drug treatments for certain impulse control (externalizing) problems,
   
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 151

including impulsive aggression (e.g., lithium, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors; Minzenberg & Siever, 2006), alcoholism (e.g., acamprosate, nal- trexone; O’Malley, Croop, Wroblewski, Labriola, & Volpicelli, 1995; Paille et al., 1995), sexual deviancy (e.g., antiandrogens; Briken, Hill, & Berner, 2003), and pathological gambling (e.g., naltrexone; Kim & Grant, 2001). The fact that certain drug treatments (e.g., naltrexone) have proven to be effective with impulse control problems of different kinds fits with the idea that disorders within the externalizing spectrum have shared etiologic under- pinnings (Krueger et al., 2002). From the perspective of the hierarchical model of externalizing (Krueger et al., 2002, in press), particular pharmaco- logical treatments may be needed to target neurocognitive impairments asso- ciated with general externalizing vulnerability, whereas others may be needed to target distinctive cognitive and affective processing deviations that under- lie aggressive and addictive problems within this spectrum.
From the perspective of the dual-process model of psychopathy, an effective intervention strategy would also need to contend separately with the interper- sonal and affective elements of psychopathy that are distinguishable from gen- eral externalizing vulnerability. As noted in the last part of the preceding section, the affective features of psychopathy (which reflect aggressiveness and social detachment) may intersect with the aggressive subfactor of externaliz- ing. In contrast, the variance in the interpersonal component of PCL-R psy- chopathy that is distinct from externalizing (and that is tapped by the first factor of the PPI) appears to reflect aspects of positive psychological adjust- ment (dominance, nonanxiousness, fearlessness) in addition to aspects of deviancy. Precisely because it is associated with resiliency and an absence of distress, this component of psychopathy may pose an important obstacle to therapy that may need to be dealt with in a fundamentally different way from that by which externalizing features of the disorder are dealt with.
In summary, to be maximally effective, therapeutic intervention strategies for APD and psychopathy will need to recognize and contend with the variegated nature of these syndromes. Specifically, multifaceted treatment programs that employ pharmacological as well as psychological-behavioral techniques to target specific processing impairments associated with distinc- tive symptomatic features—including general impulsiveness, callous aggres- sion, addictive urges, and insouciant narcissism—are likely to offer the best hope for dealing with these challenging and costly disorders (cf. Seto & Quinsey, 2006).
___________________________________________ Conclusion
APD and psychopathy are related but distinctive phenomena. APD as defined in the DSM can be seen as one behavioral expression (facet) of a broader under- lying vulnerability to problems of impulse control. Among disorders within the externalizing spectrum, APD is characterized particularly by irritability and aggressiveness along with impulsiveness and irresponsibility. Psychopathy as
 
152
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

defined by Hare’s PCL-R intersects with APD through its social deviance (Factor 2) component, which taps the broad externalizing factor of which APD is an indicator. In addition to externalizing, however, the PCL-R includes items that tap, to some degree, the nexus of positive adjustment and interpersonal- affective deviancy that Cleckley described as the “mask” component of psy- chopathy. Recent research findings indicate that the first factor of Lilienfeld’s (1990) PPI, which comprises elements of dominance, stress immunity, and fear- lessness, may tap this mask component in a purer fashion. Other evidence sug- gests that this component of psychopathy may reflect a different underlying neurobiological mechanism (i.e., individual differences in trait fear) from the externalizing component (i.e., individual differences in anterior brain function). Further systematic research on these two distinctive components of the psy- chopathy construct, one of which intersects fundamentally with APD, should help to elucidate the essential nature of these high-impact disorders and con- tribute to improved strategies for dealing with them.
Notes _____________________________________________
1. Lykken (1995) proposed that the distinctive use of these two terms be revived. Specifically, he proposed that the term psychopath be used for individuals whose pathology is primarily constitutional in origin (i.e., the product of extreme tempera- ment) and that the term sociopath be applied to individuals whose pathology is deter- mined more by environmental factors (in particular, by deviant or inadequate parenting).
2. Cleckley’s idea that the essence of true psychopathy lies in deficient affectivity and impaired social relations was adopted by other influential writers in the field. Cleckley’s contemporary Karpman (1941, 1948) introduced the distinction between primary and secondary psychopathy. Karpman’s view, paralleling Cleckley’s, was that true (“primary”) psychopathy reflects a constitutional deficit in emotional respon- siveness, whereas pseudo (“secondary”) psychopathy arises from negative socializa- tion experiences that instill hostility, alienation, and rebellious behavior. Johns and Quay (1962) characterized psychopathy as involving a disconnection between affect and cognition (i.e., psychopaths know the “words” of emotion, but not the “music”). McCord and McCord (1964) described “lovelessness” and “guiltlessness” as the essence of the syndrome. This impact of Cleckley on conceptualizations by others in the field helps to explain why experts in psychopathy have been so critical of the DSM’s APD construct for its overemphasis on symptoms of behavioral deviance.
3. It should be noted that alternative versions of the PCL-R have been developed for various purposes. A shortened, screening version (PCL:SV; Hart, Cox, & Hare, 1995) was developed for more efficient clinical assessment in forensic populations as well as for use in high-risk community studies. A youth version (PCL:YV; Forth, Kosson, & Hare, 2003) has been developed for use with adolescent offenders. The Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD; Frick & Hare, 2001), a psychopathy inventory for younger children, was modeled after the PCL-R. In addition, efforts are under way to develop a version of the PCL-R called the Business Scan (B-Scan; Babiak & Hare, 2005) for identifying individuals with psychopathic ten- dencies in corporate environments.
  
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 153

4. One report, by Schmitt and Newman (1999), failed to detect significant nega- tive relations between PCL-R factor 1 and measures of trait anxiousness. However, the level of interrater reliability for overall PCL-R scores in this study was quite low (only .70) in relation to other published studies, and reliabilities for the two PCL-R factors were not reported. Because factor scores are based on fewer items, reliabili- ties for these scores would likely have been even lower. The unreliability of PCL-R scores (which would operate to attenuate correlations with criterion measures, including anxiety scales) could account for the discrepancy between the findings of this study and those of other studies that have examined associations between the PCL-R factors and anxiety measures.
5. Other work has shown that the two factors of the PPI can also be effectively estimated using scores from other omnibus inventories of personality. For example, Sellbom, Ben-Porath, Graham, Lilienfeld, and Patrick (2005) reported cross- validated multiple correlations of .62 and .60 for the prediction of PPI-I and PPI-II, respectively, from scores on the restructured clinical scales of the MMPI-2 (Tellegen et al., 2003). Ross et al. (2005) reported average split-half cross-validation reliabil- ities of .75 and .78 for the prediction of PPI-I and PPI-II, respectively, from scores on the NEO-PI-R. These findings, together with those for the MPQ (Benning et al., 2003), open the door to further investigation of the criterion-related and predictive validity of these distinctive psychopathy constructs in large existing data sets in which scores on one or more of these omnibus inventories of personality are available.
6. The moderate level of association between PPI-II and externalizing in this study probably reflects the fact that the two variables were assessed using different methods (i.e., self-report versus clinical interview). In more recent work (Patrick, 2005), we found a much higher phenotypic association between PPI-II and external- izing (r = .84) when both variables were assessed using the same method (i.e., self-report).
_________________________________________ References
Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 and 1991 profile. Burlington: University of Vermont.
Acton, G. S. (2003). Measurement of impulsivity in a hierarchical model of per- sonality traits: Implications for substance use. Substance Use & Misuse, 38, 67–83.
American Psychiatric Association. (1952). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1968). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., revised). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.
   
154
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Anokhin, A. P., Vedeniapin, A. B., Sirevaag, E. J., Bauer, L. O., O’Connor, S. J., Kuperman, S., et al. (2000). The P300 brain potential is reduced in smokers. Psychopharmacology, 149, 409–413.
Arnett, P. A. (1997). Autonomic responsivity in psychopaths: A critical review and theoretical proposal. Clinical Psychology Review, 17, 903–936.
Babiak, P., & Hare, R. D. (2005). The B-Scan 360: Research version. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.
Barley, W. D. (1986). Behavioral and cognitive treatment of criminal and delinquent behavior. In W. H. Reid, D. Dorr, J. I. Walker, & J. W. Bonner III (Eds.), Unmasking the psychopath: Antisocial personality and related syndromes. New York: W. W. Norton.
Bauer, L. O., & Hesselbrock, V. M. (1999a). P300 decrements in teenagers with conduct problems: Implications for substance abuse risk and brain development. Biological Psychiatry, 46, 263–272.
Bauer, L. O., & Hesselbrock, V. M. (1999b). Subtypes of family history and conduct disorder: Effects on P300 during the Stroop test. Neuropsychopharmacology, 21, 51–62.
Bauer, L. O., & Hesselbrock, V. M. (2002). Brain maturation and subtypes of con- duct disorder: Interactive effects on P300 amplitude and topography in male adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 42,106–115.
Bauer, L. O., Hesselbrock, V. M., O’Connor, S., & Roberts, L. (1994). P300 differ- ences between non-alcoholic young men at average and above-average risk for alcoholism: Effects of distraction and task modality. Progress in Neuro- Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 18, 263–277.
Bauer, L. O., O’Connor, S., & Hesselbrock, V. M. (1994). Frontal P300 decrements in antisocial personality disorder. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 18, 1300–1305.
Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., Blonigen, D. M., Hicks, B. M., & Iacono, W. G. (2005). Estimating facets of psychopathy from normal personality traits: A step toward community epidemiological investigations. Assessment, 12, 3–18.
Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., Hicks, B. M., Blonigen, D. M., & Krueger, R. F. (2003). Factor structure of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory: Validity and implications for clinical assessment. Psychological Assessment, 15, 340–350.
Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., & Iacono, W. G. (2005). Fearlessness and underarousal in psychopathy: Startle blink modulation and electrodermal reactivity in a young adult male community sample. Psychophysiology, 42, 753–762.
Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., Salekin, R. T., & Leistico, A. R. (2005). Convergent and discriminant validity of psychopathy factors assessed via self-report: A comparison of three instruments. Assessment, 12, 270–289.
Birnbaum, K. (1909). Die psychopathischen Verbrecher [The psychopathic criminal]. Leipzig, Germany: Thieme.
Blackburn, R. (1993). The psychology of criminal conduct. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Blackburn, R. (2006). Other theoretical models of psychopathy. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 35–57). New York: Guilford Press.
Blair, R. J. R. (2006). Subcortical brain systems in psychopathy: The amygdala and asso- ciated structures. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 296–312). New York: Guilford Press.
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 155

Blonigen, D. M., Carlson, S. R., Krueger, R. F., & Patrick, C. J. (2003). A twin study of self-reported psychopathic personality traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 179–197.
Blonigen, D. M., Hicks, B. M., Patrick, C. J., Krueger, R. F., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. K. (2005). Psychopathic personality traits: Heritability and genetic overlap with internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. Psychological Medicine, 35, 1–12.
Briken, P., Hill, A., & Berner, W. (2003). Pharmacotherapy of paraphilias with long- acting agonists of luteinizing hormone-releasing hormone: A systematic review. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 64, 890–897.
Brinkley, C. A., Bernstein, A., & Newman, J. P. (1999). Coherence in the narratives of psychopathic and nonpsychopathic criminal offenders. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 519–530.
Brinkley, C. A., Newman, J. P., Harpur, T. J., & Johnson, M. M. (1999). Cohesion in texts produced by psychopathic and nonpsychopathic criminal inmates. Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 873–885.
Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81–105.
Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Botvinick, M. M., Noll, D. N., & Cohen, J. D. (1998). Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitor- ing of performance. Science, 280, 747–749.
Casillas, A., & Clark, L. A. (2002). Dependency, impulsivity, and self-harm: Traits hypothesized to underlie the association between Cluster B personality and sub- stance use disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 16, 424–436.
Cleckley, H. (1941). The mask of sanity. St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
Cleckley, H. (1976). The mask of sanity (5th ed.). St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1975). Applied multiple correlation/regression analysis for
the social sciences. New York: Wiley.
Cooke, D. J., & Michie, C. (2001). Refining the construct of psychopathy: Towards
a hierarchical model. Psychological Assessment, 13, 171–188.
Costa, L., Bauer, L., Kuperman, S., Porjesz, B., O’Connor, S., Hesselbrock, V., et al. (2000). Frontal P300 decrements, alcohol dependence, and antisocial personal-
ity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 47, 1064–1071.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory manual.
Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO PI-R professional manual. Lutz, FL:
Psychological Assessment Resources.
Davidson, R. J., Putnam, K. M., & Larson, C. L. (2000). Dysfunction in the neural cir-
cuitry of emotion regulation: A possible prelude to violence. Science, 289, 591–594. Day, R., & Wong, S. (1996). Anomalous perceptual asymmetries for negative emo- tional stimuli in the psychopath. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
105, 648–652.
Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model.
Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 417–440.
Dikman, Z. V., & Allen, J. J. (2000). Error monitoring during reward and avoidance
learning in high- and low-socialized individuals. Psychophysiology, 37, 43–54. Dolan, M. C., & Anderson, I. M. (2003). The relationship between serotonergic function and the Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version. Journal of
Psychopharmacology, 17, 216–222.
156
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.
Farrington, D. (2006). Family background and psychopathy. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 229–250). New York: Guilford Press.
Feighner, J. P., Robins, E., Guze, S. B., Woodruff, R. A., Winokur, G., & Munoz, R. (1972). Diagnostic criteria for use in psychiatric research. Archives of General Psychiatry, 26, 57–63.
Forth, A. E., Kosson, D. S., & Hare, R. D. (2003). The Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version manual. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.
Fowles, D.C. (1980). The three arousal model: Implications of Gray’s two-factor learning theory for heart rate, electrodermal activity, and psychopathy. Psychophysiology, 17, 87–104.
Fowles, D. C., & Dindo, L. (2006). A dual deficit model of psychopathy. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 14–34). New York: Guilford Press. Frances, A. J. (1980). The DSM-III personality disorders section: A commentary.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 137, 1050–1054.
Frick, P. J., & Hare, R. D. (2001). Antisocial process screening device. Toronto,
Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.
Frick, P. J., Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., Green, S., Hart, E. L.,
et al. (1991). Oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder in boys: Patterns
of behavioral covariation. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 20, 202–208. Frick, P. J., Lilienfeld, S. O., Ellis, M., Loney, B., & Silverthorn P. (1999). The asso- ciation between anxiety and psychopathy dimensions in children. Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology, 27, 383–392.
Gordon, H. L., Baird, A. A., & End, A. (2004). Functional differences among those high
and low on a trait measure of psychopathy. Biological Psychiatry, 56, 516–521. Gough, H. G. (1960). Theory and method of socialization. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 24, 23–30.
Grace, J., & Malloy, P. F. (2001). Frontal Systems Behavior Scale: Professional man-
ual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Grant, B. F., Stinson, F. S., Dawson, D. A., Chou, S. P., Ruan, W. J., & Pickering,
R. P. (2004). Co-occurrence of 12-month alcohol and drug use disorders and personality disorders in the United States: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 361–368.
Gray, J. A. (1971). The psychology of fear and stress. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Grove, W. M., Eckert, E. D., Heston, L., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Segal, N., & Lykken, D. T. (1990). Heritability of substance abuse and antisocial behavior: A study of monozygotic twins reared apart. Biological Psychiatry, 27, 1293–1304.
Hall, J. R., & Benning, S. D. (2006). The “successful” psychopath: Adaptive and sub- clinical manifestations of psychopathy in the general population. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 459–478). New York: Guilford Press.
Hall, J. R., Benning, S. D., & Patrick, C. J. (2004). Criterion-related validity of the three-factor model of psychopathy: Personality, behavior, and adaptive func- tioning. Assessment, 11, 4–16.
Hall, J. R., Bernat, E. M., & Patrick, C. J. (in press). Externalizing psychopathology and the error-related negativity. Psychological Science.
Hare, R. D. (1965) Temporal gradient of fear arousal in psychopaths. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 70, 442–445.
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 157

Hare, R. D. (1978). Electrodermal and cardiovascular correlates of psychopathy. In R. D. Hare & D. Schalling (Eds.), Psychopathic behavior: Approaches to research (pp. 107–143). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Hare, R. D. (1980). A research scale for the assessment of psychopathy in criminal populations. Personality and Individual Differences, 1, 111–119.
Hare, R. D. (1983). Diagnosis of antisocial personality disorder in two prison popu- lations. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 887–890.
Hare, R. D. (1984). Performance of psychopaths on cognitive tasks related to frontal lobe function. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 133–140.
Hare, R. D. (1985). A comparison of procedures for the assessment of psychopathy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 7–16.
Hare, R. D. (1991). The Hare psychopathy checklist—revised. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.
Hare, R. D. (1993). Without conscience: The disturbing world of the psychopaths among us. New York: Pocket Books.
Hare, R. D. (2003). The Hare psychopathy checklist—revised (2nd ed.). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.
Hare, R. D., Harpur, T. J., Hakstian, A. R., Forth, A. E., Hart, S. D., & Newman, J. P. (1990). The Revised Psychopathy Checklist: Descriptive statistics, reliabil- ity, and factor structure. Psychological Assessment, 2, 338–341.
Hare, R. D., & Hart, S. D. (1995). A comment on the DSM-IV antisocial personal- ity disorder field trial. In W. J. Livesley (Ed.), The DSM-IV personality disor- ders. New York: Guilford Press.
Hare, R. D., & McPherson, L. M. (1984). Psychopathy and perceptual asymmetry during verbal dichotic listening. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 141–149. Hare, R. D., & Neumann, C. S. (2006). The PCL-R assessment of psychopathy: Development, structural properties, and new directions. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.),
Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 58–88). New York: Guilford Press.
Harpur, T. J., Hakstian, A. R., & Hare, R. D. (1988). Factor structure of the psy- chopathy checklist. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 741–747. Harpur, T. J., Hare, R. D., & Hakstian, A.R. (1989). Two-factor conceptualization of psychopathy: Construct validity and assessment implications. Psychological
Assessment, 1, 6–17.
Harris, G. T., & Rice, M. E. (2006). Treatment of psychopathy: A review of empir-
ical findings. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 555–572).
New York: Guilford Press.
Harris, G. T., Skilling, T. A., & Rice, M. E. (2001). The construct of psychopathy. In
M. Tonry (Ed.), Crime and justice: An annual review of research (pp. 197–264).
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Hathaway, S. R., & McKinley, J. C. (1943). The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality
Inventory manual. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Hart, S. D., Cox, D., & Hare, R. D. (1995). Manual for the Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version (PCL:SV). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health Systems. Hart, S. D., Forth, A. E., & Hare, R. D. (1990). Performance of criminal psychopaths on
selected neuropsychological tests. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 374–379. Hart, S. D., & Hare, R. D. (1989). Discriminant validity of the Psychopathy Checklist in a forensic psychiatric population. Psychological Assessment, 1,
211–218.
Hart, S. D., & Hare, R. D. (1998). Association between psychopathy and narcissism:
Theoretical reviews and empirical evidence. In E. F. Ronningstam (Ed.),
158
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Disorders of narcissism: Diagnostic, clinical, and empirical implications
(pp. 415–436). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Herpertz, S. C., Werth, U., Lukas, G., Qunaibi, M., Schuerkens, A., Kunert, H., et al. (2001). Emotion in criminal offenders with psychopathy and borderline
personality disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 737–744.
Hiatt, K. D., & Newman, J. P. (2006). Understanding psychopathy: The cognitive side. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 334–352). New
York: Guilford Press.
Hicks, B. M., Markon, K. E., Patrick, C. J., Krueger, R. F., & Newman, J. P. (2004).
Identifying psychopathy subtypes on the basis of personality structure.
Psychological Assessment, 16, 276–288.
Hicks, B. M., & Patrick, C. J. (2006). Psychopathy and negative affectivity: Analyses
of suppressor effects reveal distinct relations with trait anxiety, depression, fear-
fulness, and anger-hostility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 276–287. Hildebrand, M., & de Ruiter, C. (2004). PCL-R psychopathy and its relation to DSM- IV Axis I and Axis II disorders in a sample of male forensic psychiatric patients
in the Netherlands. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 24, 233–248. Hill, S. Y., & Steinhauer, S. R. (2001). Familial/genetic studies of alcohol depen- dence: Developmental trajectory in P300 amplitude in childhood/adolescence as
an endophenotypic marker of risk. Psychophysiology, 38, S12.
Holroyd, C. B., Dien, J., & Coles, M. G. H. (1998). Error-related scalp potentials elicited by hand and foot movements: Evidence for an output-independent error-
processing system in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 242, 65–68.
Iacono, W. G., Carlson, S. R., & Malone, S. M. (2000). Identifying a multivariate endophenotype for substance use disorders using psychophysiological measures.
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 38, 81–96.
Iacono, W. G., Carlson, S. R., Malone, S. M., & McGue, M. (2002). P3 event-related
potential amplitude and risk for disinhibitory disorders in adolescent boys.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 750–757.
Ishikawa, S. S., Raine, A., Lencz, T., Bihrle, S., & Lacasse, L. (2001). Autonomic stress
reactivity and executive functions in successful and unsuccessful criminal psy-
chopaths from the community. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 423–432. Johns, J. H., & Quay, H. C. (1962). The effect of social reward on verbal condi- tioning in psychopathic and neurotic military offenders. Journal of Consulting
Psychology, 26, 217–220.
Johnstone, S. J., & Barry, R. J. (1996). Auditory event-related potentials to a two-
tone auditory discrimination paradigm in attention deficit hyperactivity disor-
der. Psychiatry Research, 64, 179–192.
Jutai, J. W., & Hare, R. D. (1983). Psychopathy and selective attention during per-
formance of a complex perceptual motor task. Psychophysiology, 20, 146–151. Kahn, E. (1931). Psychopathic personalities. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Karpman, B. (1941). On the need for separating psychopathy into two distinct clinical
types: Symptomatic and idiopathic. Journal of Criminology and Psychopathology,
3, 112–137.
Karpman, B. (1948). Conscience in the psychopath: Another version. American
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 18, 455–491.
Kendler, K. S., Prescott, C. A., Myers, J., & Neale, M. C. (2003). The structure of
genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance use disorders in men and women. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 929–937.
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 159

Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Zhao, S., Nelson, C. B., Hughes, M., Eshleman, S., et al. (1994). Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 8–19.
Kessler, R. C., & Walters, E. E. (2002). The National Comorbidity Survey. In M. T. Tsuang & M. Tohen (Eds.), Textbook in psychiatric epidemiology (2nd ed., pp. 343–362). New York: Wiley.
Kiehl, K. A., Hare, R. D., Liddle, P. F., & McDonald, J. J. (1999). Reduced P300 responses in criminal psychopaths during a visual oddball task. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 1498–1507.
Kiehl, K. A., Hare, R. D., McDonald, J. J., & Brink, J. (1999). Semantic and affec- tive processing in psychopaths: An event related potential (ERP) study. Psycho- physiology, 36, 765–774.
Kim, S. W., & Grant, J. E. (2001). An open naltrexone treatment study in pathologi- cal gambling disorder. International Clinical Psychopharmacology, 16, 285–289. Kim, M. S., Kim, J. J., & Kwon, J. S. (2001). Frontal P300 decrement and executive dysfunction in adolescents with conduct problems. Child Psychiatry and Human
Development, 32, 93–106.
Klorman, R. (1991). Cognitive event-related potentials and in attention deficit disor-
der. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24, 130–140.
Koch, J. L. (1891). Die psychopathischen Minderwertigkeiten [The psychopathic
inferiority]. Ravensburg, Germany: Maier.
Koenigsberg, H. W., Kaplan, R. D., Gilmore, M. M., & Cooper, A. M. (1985). The
relation between syndrome and personality disorder in DSM III: Experience
with 2,412 patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 207–212.
Kosson, D. S. (1996). Psychopathy and dual task performance under focusing con-
ditions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 391–400.
Kosson, D. S. (1998). Divided visual attention in psychopathic and nonpsychopathic
offenders. Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 373–391.
Kosson, D. S., & Newman, J. P. (1986). Psychopathy and the allocation of attentional capacity in a divided-attention situation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95,
257–263.
Kosson, D. S., Smith, S. S. & Newman, J. P. (1990). Evaluating the construct valid-
ity of psychopathy in African American and European American male inmates:
Three preliminary studies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 250–259. Kraepelin, E. (1904). Psychiatrie: Ein Lehrbuch [Psychiatry: A textbook] (7th ed.).
Leipzig, Germany: Barth.
Kraepelin, E. (1915). Psychiatrie: Ein Lehrbuch [Psychiatry: A textbook] (8th ed.).
Leipzig, Germany: Barth.
Krueger, R. F. (1999a). The structure of common mental disorders. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 56, 921–926.
Krueger, R. F. (1999b). Personality traits in late adolescence predict mental disorders
in early adulthood: A prospective-epidemiological study. Journal of Personality,
67, 39–65.
Krueger, R. F., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1998). The structure and sta-
bility of common mental disorders (DSM-III-R): A longitudinal-epidemiological
study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 216–227.
Krueger, R. F., Hicks, B., Patrick, C. J., Carlson, S., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M.
(2002). Etiologic connections among substance dependence, antisocial behavior,
160
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

and personality: Modeling the externalizing spectrum. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 111, 411–424.
Krueger, R. F., Markon, K. E., Patrick, C. J., Benning, S. D., & Kramer, M. (in press).
Linking antisocial behavior, substance use, and personality: An integrative quan- titative model of the adult externalizing spectrum. Journal of Abnormal Psychology.
Krueger, R. F., Markon, K. E., Patrick, C. J., & Iacono, W. G. (2005). Externalizing psychopathology in adulthood: A dimensional-spectrum conceptualization and its implications for DSM-V. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 537–550.
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1990). Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex. Psychological Review, 97, 377–398.
Levenson, M. R., Kiehl, K. A., & Fitzpatrick, C. M. (1995). Assessing psychopathic attributes in a noninstitutionalized population. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 151–158.
Levenston, G. K., Patrick, C. J., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2000). The psy- chopath as observer: Emotion and attention in picture processing. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 373–385.
Lewis, C. E., Rice, J., & Helzer, J. E. (1983). Diagnostic interactions: Alcoholism and antisocial personality. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 171, 105–113.
Lilienfeld, S. O. (1990). Development and preliminary validation of a self-report mea- sure of psychopathic personality. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis.
Lilienfeld, S. O., & Andrews, B. P. (1996). Development and preliminary validation of a self-report measure of psychopathic personality traits in noncriminal popu- lations. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66, 488–524.
Lilienfeld, S. O., & Fowler, K. A. (2006). The self-report assessment of psychopathy: Problems, pitfalls, and promises. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopa- thy (pp. 107–132). New York: Guilford Press.
Linnoila, M., & Virkkunen, M. (1992). Aggression, suicidality, and serotonin. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 53, 46–51.
Linnoila, M., Virkkunen, M., Schwannian, M., Nuutila, A., Rimon, R., & Goodwin, F. K. (1983). Low cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid concentration differentiates impulsive from non-impulsive violent behavior. Life Sciences, 33, 2609–2614.
Lorber, M. F. (2004). Psychophysiology of aggression, psychopathy, and conduct problems: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 531–552.
Lorenz, A. R., & Newman, J. P. (2002). Do emotion and information processing deficiencies found in Caucasian psychopaths generalize to African-American psychopaths? Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 1077–1086.
Luu, P., Flaisch, T., & Tucker, D. M. (2000). Medial frontal cortex in action moni- toring. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 464–469.
Lykken, D. T. (1957). A study of anxiety in the sociopathic personality. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 55, 6–10.
Lykken, D. T. (1995). The antisocial personalities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lynam, D. R. (1997). Pursuing the psychopath: Capturing the fledgling psychopath
in a nomological net. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 425–438.
Lynam, D. R., & Derefinko, K. J. (2006). Psychopathy and personality. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 133–155). New York: Guilford
Press.
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 161

Lynam, D. R., Leukefeld, C., & Clayton, R. R. (2003). The contribution of person- ality to the overlap between antisocial behavior and substance use/misuse. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 316–331.
Maliphant, R., Hume, F., & Furnham, A. (1990). Autonomic nervous system (ANS) activity, personality characteristics, and disruptive behaviour in girls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 31, 619–628.
McCord, W., & McCord, J. (1964). The psychopath: An essay on the criminal mind. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.
Mealey, L. (1995). The sociobiology of sociopathy: An integrated evolutionary model. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 523–599.
Meyer, A. (1904). A review of recent problems of psychiatry. In A. Church & F. Peterson (Eds.), Nervous and mental diseases (4th ed.). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
Miller, J. D., Lynam, D. R., Widiger, T. A., & Leukefeld, C. (2001). Personality dis- orders as extreme variants of common personality dimensions: Can the five- factor model adequately represent psychopathy? Journal of Personality, 69, 253–276.
Millon, T. (1981). Disorders of personality: DSM-III: Axis II. New York: Wiley. Miltner, W. H. R., Braun, C. H., & Coles, M. G. H. (1997). Event-related brain poten- tials following incorrect feedback in a time-estimation task: Evidence for a “generic”
neural system for error detection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 788–798. Minzenberg, M. J., & Siever, L. J. (2006). Neurochemistry and pharmacology of psy- chopathy and related disorders. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopa-
thy (pp. 251–277). New York: Guilford Press.
Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behav-
ior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review, 100, 674–701.
Morey, L. (1991). The Personality Assessment Inventory professional manual.
Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Morgan, A. B., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2000). A meta-analytic review of the relation
between antisocial behavior and neuropsychological measures of executive func-
tion. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 113–136.
Newman, J. P. (1998). Psychopathic behavior: An information processing perspec-
tive. In D. J. Cooke, R. D. Hare, & A. Forth (Eds.), Psychopathy: Theory, research and implications for society (pp. 81–104). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
O’Malley, S. S., Croop, R. S., Wroblewski, J. M., Labriola, D. F., Volpicelli, J. R. (1995). Naltrexone in the treatment of alcohol dependence: A combined analy- sis of two trials. Psychiatric Annals, 25, 681–688.
Ortiz, J., & Raine, A. (2004). Heart rate level and antisocial behavior in children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 154–162.
Paille, F. M., Guelfi, J. D., Persons, A. C., Royer, R. J., Steru, L., & Parot, P. (1995). Double blind randomized multicentre trial of acamprosate in maintaining absti- nence from alcohol. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 30, 239–247.
Patrick, C. J. (1994). Emotion and psychopathy: Startling new insights. Psychophys- iology, 31, 319–330.
Patrick, C. J. (2001). Emotions and psychopathy. In A. Raine & J. Sanmartin (Eds.), Violence and psychopathy (pp. 57–77). New York: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
162
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Patrick, C. J. (2005). What constructs underlie the two factors of the psychopathic personality inventory? Paper presented at the 1st Annual Meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Psychopathy, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada.
Patrick, C. J. (2006). Getting to the heart of psychopathy. In H. Herve & J. C. Yuille (Eds.), Psychopathy: Theory, research, and social implications (pp. 207–252). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Patrick, C. J., & Bernat, E. (2006). The construct of emotion as a bridge between per- sonality and psychopathology. In R. F. Krueger & J. Tackett (Eds.), Personality and psychopathology (pp. 174–209). New York: Guilford Press.
Patrick, C. J., Bernat, E., Malone, S. M., Iacono, W. G., Krueger, R. F., & McGue, M. K. (2006). P300 amplitude as an indicator of externalizing in adolescent males. Psychophysiology, 43, 84–92.
Patrick, C. J., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1993). Emotion in the criminal psy- chopath: Startle reflex modulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 82–92. Patrick, C. J., Curtin, J. J., & Tellegen, A. (2002). Development and validation of a brief form of the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Psychological
Assessment, 14, 150–163.
Patrick, C. J., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Emotion in the criminal psy-
chopath: Fear image processing. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103,
523–534.
Patrick, C. J., Edens, J. F., Poythress, N. G., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Benning, S. D. (2006).
Construct validity of the PPI two-factor model with offenders. Psychological
Assessment, 18, 204–208.
Patrick, C. J., Hicks, B. M., Krueger, R. F., & Lang, A. R. (2005). Relations between
psychopathy facets and externalizing in a criminal offender sample. Journal of
Personality Disorders, 19, 339–356.
Patrick, C. J., & Lang, A. R. (1999). Psychopathic traits and intoxicated states:
Affective concomitants and conceptual links. In M. E. Dawson, A. M. Schell, & A. H. Boehmelt (Eds.), Startle modification: Implications for clinical science, cognitive science, and neuroscience (pp. 209–230). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Patrick, C. J., & Zempolich, K. A. (1998). Emotion and aggression in the psycho- pathic personality. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 3, 303–338.
Patrick, C. J., Zempolich, K. A., & Levenston, G. K. (1997). Emotionality and violent behavior in psychopaths: A biosocial analysis. In A. Raine, P. A. Brennan, D. P. Farrington, & S. A. Mednick (Eds.), Biosocial bases of violence (pp. 145–161). New York: Plenum.
Patterson, C. M., & Newman, J. P. (1993). Reflectivity and learning from aversive events: Toward a psychological mechanism for the syndromes of disinhibition. Psychological Review, 100, 716–736.
Paulhus, D. L., Robins, R. W., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Tracy, J. L. (2004). Two replicable suppressor situations in personality research. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39, 303–328.
Peterson, J. B., & Pihl, R. O. (1990). Information processing, neuropsychological function, and the inherited predisposition to alcoholism. Neuropsychology Review, 1, 343–369.
Pickens, R. W., Svikis, D. S., McGue, M., & LaBuda, M. C. (1995). Common genetic mechanisms in alcohol, drug, and mental disorder comorbidity. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 39, 129–138.
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 163

Pinel, P. (1801/1962). A treatise on insanity (D. Davis, Trans.). New York: Hafner. Polich, J., Pollock, V. E., & Bloom, F. E. (1994). Meta-analysis of P300 amplitude
from males at risk for alcoholism. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 55–73. Poythress, N. G., Edens, J. F., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (1998). Criterion-related validity of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory in a prison sample. Psychological
Assessment, 10, 426–430.
Pritchard, J. C. (1835). A treatise on insanity and other disorders affecting the mind.
London: Sherwood, Gilbert & Piper.
Raine, A. (1993). The psychopathology of crime. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Raine, A. (1997). Antisocial behavior and psychophysiology: A biosocial perspective
and a prefrontal dysfunction hypothesis. In D. M. Stoff, J. Breiling, & J. D. Maser (Eds.), Handbook of antisocial behavior (pp. 289–303). New York: Wiley.
Raine, A. (2002). Biosocial studies of antisocial and violent behavior in children and adults: A review. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 311–326.
Raine, A., & Venables, P. H. (1984). Tonic heart rate level, social class, and antiso- cial behaviour in adolescents. Biological Psychology, 18, 123–132.
Raine, A., & Yang, Y. (2006). The neuroanatomical bases of psychopathy: A review of brain imaging findings. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 278–295). New York: Guilford Press.
Reardon, M. L., Lang, A. R., & Patrick, C. J. (2002). Antisociality and alcohol problems: An evaluation of subtypes, drinking motives, and family history in incarcerated men. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 26, 1188–1197.
Robins, L. N. (1966). Deviant children grown up. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. Robins, L. N., & Regier, D. A. (1991). Psychiatric disorders in America: The epi-
demiological catchment area study. New York: Free Press.
Rogers, R. D. (2006). The functional architecture of the frontal lobes: Implications for research with psychopathic offenders. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of
psychopathy (pp. 313–333). New York: Guilford Press.
Ross, S. R., Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., Thompson, A., & Thurston, A. (2005).
Factors of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory: Criterion-related validity and relationships with the five-factor model of personality. Manuscript sub- mitted for publication.
Rush, B. (1812). Medical inquiries and observations upon the diseases of the mind. Philadelphia: Kimber & Richardson.
Scheffers, M., Coles, M. G. H., Bernstein, P., Gehring, W. J., & Donchin, E. (1996). Event-related potentials and error-related processing: An analysis of incorrect responses to go and no-go stimuli. Psychophysiology, 33, 42–53.
Schmitt, W. A., & Newman, J. P. (1999). Are all psychopathic individuals low- anxious? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 353–358.
Schneider, K. (1934). Die psychopathischen Persönlichkeiten [The psychopathic per- sonalities] (3rd ed.). Vienna: Deuticke.
Sellbom, M., Ben-Porath, Y., Graham, J. K., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Patrick, C. J. (2005). Assessing psychopathic personality traits with the MMPI-2. Journal of Personality Assessment, 85, 334–343.
Seto, M. C., & Quinsey, V. L. (2006). Toward the future: Translating basic research into prevention and treatment strategies. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 589–601). New York: Guilford Press.
164
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Sher, K. J., Bartholow, B. D., & Wood, M. D. (2000). Personality and substance use disorders: A prospective study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 818–829.
Shine, J., & Hobson, J. (1997) Construct validity of the Hare Psychopathy Checklist—Revised. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry, 8, 546–561.
Siddle, D. A. T., & Trasler, G. B. (1981). The psychophysiology of psychopathic behavior. In M. J. Christie & P. G. Mellett (Eds.), Foundations of psychoso- matics (pp. 283–303). New York: Wiley.
Skeem, J. L., Edens, J. F., Camp, J., & Colwell, L. H. (2004). Are there racial differ- ences in levels of psychopathy? A meta-analysis. Law and Human Behavior, 28, 505–527.
Skodol, A. E., Oldham, J. M., & Gallaher, P. E. (1999). Axis II comorbidity of sub- stance use disorders among patients referred for treatment of personality disor- ders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 733–738.
Slutske, W. S., Heath, A. C., Dinwiddie, S. H., Madden, P. A. F., Bucholz, K. K., Dunne, M. P., et al. (1998). Common genetic risk factors for conduct disorder and alcohol dependence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 363–374.
Slutske, W. S., Heath, A. C., Madden, P. A. F., Bucholz, K. K., Statham, D. J., & Martin, N. G. (2002). Personality and the genetic risk for alcohol dependence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 124–133.
Smith, S. S., & Newman, J. P. (1990). Alcohol and drug abuse-dependence disorders in psychopathic and nonpsychopathic criminal offenders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 430–439.
Spitzer, R. L., Endicott, J., & Robins, E. (1978). Research diagnostic criteria: Rationale and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 773–782.
Sullivan, E. A., & Kosson, D. S. (2006). Ethnic and cultural variations in psychopa- thy. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 437–458). New York: Guilford Press.
Sutton, S. K., Vitale, J. E., & Newman, J. P. (2002). Emotion among females with psy- chopathy during picture perception. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 610–619. Tackett, J. L., Krueger, R. F., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2005). Symptom-based subfactors of DSM-defined conduct disorder: Evidence for etiologic distinctions.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 483–487.
Tackett, J. L., Krueger, R. F., Sawyer, M. G., & Graetz, B. W. (2003). Subfactors
of DSM-IV conduct disorder: Evidence and connections with syndromes from the child behavior checklist. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 647–654.
Tarter, R. E., Alterman, A. I., & Edwards, K. L., (1985). Vulnerability to alcoholism in men: A behavior-genetic perspective. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 46, 329–356.
Tellegen, A. (in press). Manual for the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Tellegen, A., Ben-Porath, Y. S., McNulty, J. L., Arbisi, P. A., Graham, J. R., & Kaemmer, B. (2003). MMPI-2 Restructured Clinical (RC) scales: Development, validation, and interpretation. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Trull, T. J. (1992). DSM-III-R personality disorders and the five-factor model of person- ality: An empirical comparison. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 553–560.
Trull, T. J., & Sher, K. J. (1994). Relationship between the five-factor model of per- sonality and Axis I disorders in a nonclinical sample. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 350–360.
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy 165

Vanman, E. J., Mejia, V. Y., Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., & Raine, A. (2003). Modification of the startle reflex in a community sample: Do one or two dimen- sions of psychopathy underlie emotional processing? Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 2007–2021.
Veit, R., Flor, H., Erb, M., Lotze, M., Grodd, W., & Birbaumer, N. (2002). Brain circuits involved in emotional learning in antisocial behavior and social phobia in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 328, 233–236.
Verona, E., Hicks, B. M., & Patrick, C. J. (2005). Psychopathy and suicidal behav- ior in female offenders: Effects of personality and abuse history. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 1065–1073.
Verona, E., & Patrick, C. J. (2000). Suicide risk in externalizing syndromes: Temperamental and neurobiological underpinnings. In T. E. Joiner (Ed.), Suicide science: Expanding the boundaries (pp. 137–173). Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Verona, E., Patrick, C. J., Curtin, J. J., Lang, P. J., & Bradley, M. M. (2004). Psychopathy and physiological response to emotionally evocative sounds. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 99–108.
Verona, E., Patrick, C. J., & Joiner, T. E. (2001). Psychopathy, antisocial personal- ity, and suicide risk. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 462–470.
Verona, E., & Vitale, J. (2006). Psychopathy in women: Assessment, manifestations, and etiology. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 415–436). New York: Guilford Press.
Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., de Clercq, A., & Koster, E. H. W. (2005). Psychopathic traits and autonomic responding to concealed information in a prison sample. Psychophysiology, 42, 239–245.
Virkkunen, M., Rawlings, R., Tokola, R., Poland, R. E., Cuidotti, A., Nemweroff, C., et al. (1994). CSF biochemistries, glucose metabolism, and diurnal activity rhythms in alcoholic, violent offenders, fire setters, and healthy volunteers. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 20–27.
Vollebergh, W. A. M., Iedema, J., Bijl, R. V., de Graaf, R., Smit, F., & Ormel., J. (2001). The structure and stability of common mental disorders: The NEMESIS study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 597–603.
Wadsworth, M. E. J. (1976). Delinquency, pulse rate and early emotional depriva- tion. British Journal of Criminology, 16, 245–256.
Warren, J. I., Burnette, M. L., South, S. C., Preeti, C., Bale, R., Friend, R., et al. (2003). Psychopathy in women: Structural modeling and comorbidity. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 26, 223–242.
Widiger, T. A., Cadoret, R., Hare, R., Robins, L., Rutherford, M., Zanarini, M., et al. (1996). DSM-IV antisocial personality disorder field trial. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 3–16.
Widiger, T. A., Verheul, R., & van den Brink, W. (1999). Personality and psy- chopathology. In L. A. Pervin & O. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 347–366). New York: Guilford Press.
Williamson, S. E. (1991). Cohesion and coherence in the speech of psychopathic criminals. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada.
Williamson, S. E., Harpur, T. J., & Hare, R. D. (1991). Abnormal processing of affective words by psychopaths. Psychophysiology, 28, 260–273.
Woodworth, M., & Porter, S. (2002). In cold blood: Characteristics of criminal homi- cides as a function of psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 436–445.
166
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

World Health Organization. (1948). International statistical classification of dis- eases, injuries, and causes of death (6th revision). Geneva, Switzerland: Author. Young, S. E., Stallings, M. C., Corley, R. P., Krauter, K. S., & Hewitt, J. K. (2000). Genetic and environmental influences on behavioral disinhibition. American
Journal of Medical Genetics, 96, 684–695.
Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking: Beyond the optimal level of arousal.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Zuckerman, M. (1999). Vulnerability to psychopathology: A biosocial model.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Borderline 7 Personality Disorder
Rebekah Bradley Emory University
Carolyn Zittel Conklin Cambridge Health Alliance and Harvard Medical School
Drew Westen Emory University

Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is one of the most prevalent, most widely studied, and yet most controversial of the personality disorders (PDs) described in the fourth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994). Its public health significance arguably rivals that of any other diagnostic syndrome. Patients with BPD constitute 20% of psychiatric
Authors’ notes: Preparation of this article was supported in part by National Institute of Mental Health grants MH62377 and MH62378 to the third author. Some of the information presented in this chapter is drawn from an article by the second and third authors: “Conceptual Issues and Research Findings on Borderline Personality Disorder: What Every Clinician Should Know” (Zittel & Westen, 1998). Correspondence concerning this chapter should be addressed to Rebekah Bradley, Ph.D., Director, Trauma Recovery Program, Atlanta VAMC, 1670 Clairmont Road, Atlanta, Georgia 30033. E-mail: rebekah.bradley@emory.edu
167
168
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

inpatients and 10% of patients seen in outpatient mental health clinics (APA, 1994) and are high consumers of emergency room services, crisis lines, and psychiatric consultations requested by other medical services (Ellison, Barsky, & Blum, 1989; Forman, Berk, Henriques, Brown, & Beck, 2004; Gross et al., 2002; Reich, Boerstler, Yates, & Nduaguba, 1989; Zanarini, Frankenburg, Hennen, & Silk, 2004). Between 70% and 75% of BPD patients have a history of at least one self-injurious act (Clarkin, Widiger, Frances, Hurt, & Gilmore, 1983; Cowdry, 1992), and quick calculations with available statistics (APA, 1994; McGlashan, 1986; Samuels et al., 2002; Stone, 1993; Torgersen, Kringlen, & Cramer, 2001) indicate that of the 6 million individuals currently estimated to have BPD in the United States alone, between 180,000 and 540,000 will die by suicide.
In this chapter, we provide a broad overview of the state of knowledge of BPD. We begin by briefly describing the evolution of the diagnosis and con- temporary controversies regarding the construct itself and the way it should be defined. We then discuss the assessment of BPD. Next we examine what is known about the development and developmental course of BPD, includ- ing its etiology, longitudinal stability, and prognosis. The final section con- siders treatment approaches, including a number of relatively recent empirical developments in the psychotherapy of BPD.
The Borderline Diagnosis:
Evolution and Diagnostic Controversies ________________
The concept of “borderline” has undergone a substantial evolution since its early identification by psychoanalytic clinical theorists, who first identified the construct as “pseudoneurotic schizophrenia,” “as-if personality,” and eventually “borderline state” (Knight, 1953, 1954). In this section, we briefly describe the evolution of the construct. We then examine contempo- rary controversies and diagnostic dilemmas in the understanding and diag- nosis of the borderline construct.
Evolution of the Borderline Construct
Initially the term borderline referred to individuals who seemed neither neurotic nor psychotic but were somewhere in between. This was the conceptualization that Kernberg (1967) later elaborated in his concept of borderline personality organization (BPO). By “personality organization,” Kernberg meant enduring ways of feeling, thinking, behaving, experiencing the self and others, and dealing with unpleasant realities. In Kernberg’s view, patients with borderline personality organization tend to use drastic, imma- ture ways of dealing with impulses and emotions (e.g., behaviors such as cutting and defensive maneuvers such as denial of obvious realities). They
  
Borderline Personality Disorder 169

are not psychotic but can become cognitively more disorganized than most people, particularly under stress, and have difficulty maintaining balanced views of the self and significant others (“splitting” their representations into all good and all bad).
Over time, the concept of borderline as a level of disturbance (originally between neurotic and psychotic) shifted from this broader construct to the more specific diagnostic category first defined in the third edition of the DSM (DSM-III; APA, 1980). Kernberg’s concept of borderline influenced the description of the disorder in DSM-III, which has remained intact, with small modifications, for the last 20 years. However, his concept of border- line as a form of personality organization is a broader construct that describes a level of personality sickness that encompasses many of the DSM- IV PDs, including all the Cluster A (odd, eccentric) PDs; the Cluster B (erratic, dramatic) PDs, with the exception of some higher functioning nar- cissistic patients; and the more disturbed subset of patients within each of the Cluster C (anxious, fearful) PDs.
Like most diagnoses, the construct of BPD first emerged from the work of prescient clinical observers who attempted to identify patterns of covari- ation among symptoms not previously understood, followed by research aimed at refining the construct. The initial efforts to establish a more empir- ically grounded concept of BPD actually began prior to DSM-III with the work of Grinker, Werble, and Drye (1968), who suggested the first empiri- cally derived diagnostic criterion set for the borderline syndrome. This was followed by development of the Diagnostic Interview for Borderline Personality Disorder (DIB; Gunderson & Kolb, 1978; Gunderson, Kolb, & Austin, 1981; Gunderson & Singer, 1975). As editor of DSM-III, Spitzer developed potential diagnostic criteria for BPD by reviewing clinical and research literature and consulting with clinicians expert in treating border- line patients. He then collected data in a national survey of psychiatrists who evaluated the selected criteria. The resulting set of distinguishing borderline characteristics (Spitzer, Endicott, & Gibbon, 1979) became the basis for the BPD criteria in the DSM-III (APA, 1980). This resulted in BPD’s becoming an official psychiatric disorder rather than a level of personality structure or disturbance. DSM-IV defines the essential features of BPD as a “pervasive pattern of instability of interpersonal relationships, self-image, and affects, and marked impulsivity that begins by early adulthood and is present in a variety of contexts” (APA, 2000, p. 706).
Current Controversies and Diagnostic Dilemmas
Like the other PDs, the BPD diagnosis in DSM-IV emerged through over half a century of clinical observation, which largely generated the criteria for the disorder in DSM-III (and instruments for assessing it), followed by 25 years of research aimed at refining the diagnosis. The criteria for the disor- der clearly capture a group of severely impaired patients frequently seen in
  
170
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

mental health settings. However, a number of problems limit the clinical util- ity and validity of the diagnostic criteria for BPD. We focus here on three: heterogeneity of symptom presentation, categorical diagnosis, and excessive comorbidity with other Axis II disorders as well as Axis I disorders.
With respect to heterogeneity, a patient can receive the BPD diagnosis in over 150 different ways based on varying combinations of the nine criteria for the disorder (Skodol, Gunderson, Pfohl, et al., 2002). Put another way, two patients may both be diagnosed with BPD while sharing only one symp- tom in common. This fact has important clinical implications because sub- types of BPD seem to exist that do not reflect random variation among criteria but rather meaningful, patterned heterogeneity, such as internalizing and externalizing subtypes of the disorder (Bradley, Zittel, et al., 2005; Conklin & Westen, 2005; Conklin, Bradley, & Westen, 2006; Westen & Shedler, 1999b; Zittel & Westen, 2002).
With respect to categorical diagnosis, the DSM approach to classification assumes that PDs represent categorically distinct classes of psychopathology. However, most research on classification of PDs favors a dimensional rather than a categorical understanding of PD (e.g., Clark, Livesley, & Morey, 1997; Trull, 2001; Widiger, 1995). Consistent with this overall trend in per- sonality research, research on BPD, including research applying taxometric analysis (Meehl, 1995), suggests that the disorder is likely best represented dimensionally and does not represent a distinct taxon (e.g., Rothschild, Cleland, Haslam, & Zimmerman, 2003).
With respect to comorbidity, research using both DSM-III and DSM-IV criteria indicates high levels of comorbidity with other PDs, particularly anti- social PD, avoidant PD, dependent PD, and paranoid PD (Becker, Grilo, Edell, & McGlashan, 2000; Gunderson, Zanarini, & Kisiel, 1991, 1995; Oldham et al., 1992; Stuart et al., 1998). This finding suggests that the diag- nostic criteria do not adequately capture a disorder distinct from other dis- orders or from a general personality pathology dimension. Indeed, many of the DSM-IV PDs—including paranoid, schizoid, schizotypal, antisocial, histrionic, and sometimes dependent—are consistent with borderline per- sonality organization as defined by Kernberg. With the exception of schizoid, all of these PDs show high comorbidity with DSM-defined BPD, tending to cluster together in studies of adaptive functioning, and disorders such as avoidant, narcissistic, and obsessive-compulsive generally showing better adaptive functioning (e.g., Skodol, Gunderson, McGlashan, et al., 2002; Skodol, Gunderson, Pfohl, et al., 2002; Tyrer, 1996). In any case, the comorbidity of BPD with other Cluster B PDs (histrionic, antisocial, and nar- cissistic; Fyer, Frances, Sullivan, Hurt, & Clarkin, 1988) as well as with dis- orders such as avoidant and schizotypal PDs (Barasch, Kroll, Carey, & Sines, 1983; Pfohl, Coryell, Zimmerman, & Stangl, 1986), is highly prob- lematic, particularly given that schizotypal and avoidant individuals tend to be socially withdrawn, whereas BPD is associated with fear of aloneness and the trait of extraversion (e.g., Lynam & Widiger, 2001). Borderline PD also shows high comorbidity with most nonpsychotic Axis I disorders, notably
  
Borderline Personality Disorder 171

mood, anxiety, substance use, and eating disorders (e.g., Zanarini et al., 1998; Zimmerman & Mattia, 1999).
In response to these problems, DSM task forces and PD work groups since DSM-III have attempted to adjust diagnostic criteria with the goal of making BPD less redundant with other diagnoses. For example, the Axis II Work Group for DSM-IV rewrote the DSM-III-R criterion “affective insta- bility: marked shifts from baseline mood to depression, irritability, or anxi- ety, usually lasting a few hours and only rarely more than a few days.” In hopes of better discriminating between major depression and BPD, the word “depression” was replaced with “dysphoria”; in hopes of better differentiat- ing between the mood lability seen in cyclothymic disorder and the unstable affect seen in BPD, the phrase “marked shifts . . . [of] mood” was replaced by “marked reactivity of mood.” Such efforts do not appear, however, to have substantially reduced the comorbidity of BPD with other disorders, raising questions about whether the diagnosis remains, in Akiskal’s (1996; 2004) words, “an adjective in search of a noun.”
In summary, the development of diagnostic criteria for BPD in DSM-III laid the groundwork for a surge of research on the disorder. BPD is now the most highly researched PD and has the strongest empirical evidence regard- ing its phenomenology, etiology, and treatment. Nevertheless, the research that was in large measure fostered by the presence of DSM criteria since 1980 has resulted in the identification of a number of problems with the diagnosis that remain to be resolved.
___________________________________________ Assessment
The Diagnostic Interview for Borderline Personality Disorder (DIB; Gunderson & Kolb, 1978; Gunderson et al., 1981; Gunderson & Singer, 1975) was the gold standard procedure for assessing BPD in the decade fol- lowing the definition of operational criteria for the disorder in DSM-III. However, what quickly became apparent was that any sample of BPD patients could differ in unknown ways from any other sample, depending on the pres- ence of comorbid PDs. Researchers addressed this problem with the develop- ment of structured interviews designed to assess all of the DSM PDs. The advantage of these instruments was that they assessed the range of personality pathology defined by the DSM. The disadvantage was that, in less time than it typically takes to administer the DIB (a semistructured interview for a single disorder), they attempted to assess the roughly 10 PDs defined by the various versions of the DSM since DSM-III. To economize the assessment of these dis- orders, interviews came to emphasize more the behavioral manifestations of the disorder (e.g., cutting) over the functional or “structural” aspects of per- sonality that originally defined the disorder in the clinical literature. In turn, the diagnostic criteria for the disorder shifted toward readily observable behaviors that could be assessed by structured interview, leading to the possi- bility of the procedural tail wagging the conceptual dog (Westen, 1997).
  
172
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Methods for assessing BPD generally rely on patients’ self-reported symp- toms using either structured interviews or questionnaires. A full review of such measures is beyond the scope of this chapter (see Clark & Harrison, 2001, for a review). However, we briefly present the relative strengths and weaknesses of these approaches.
The currently accepted diagnostic gold standard for the assessment of BPD is a standardized structured interview yoked to DSM criteria (e.g., the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders [SCID-II; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1997]; the Structured Interview for DSM- III-R Personality Disorders [SIDP; Pfohl, Blum, Zimmerman, & Stangl, 1989]). A primary advantage of this approach is that it asks questions about each criterion directly, ensuring adequate coverage for a DSM-IV diagnosis. A second advantage is reliability, particularly when this approach is compared with the method more common in clinical practice of conducting unstructured interviews with patients before referring to DSM or International Classification of Diseases (ICD) diagnostic criteria, which yields low interrater reliability (Mellsop, Varghese, Joshua, & Hicks, 1982; Satorius et al., 1993).
However, this approach to PD diagnosis has limitations. First, rates of comorbidity are extremely high, with the average patient receiving any PD diagnosis receiving 4 to 6 of the 10 DSM-IV PDs by structured interview and often even more by questionnaire (see Westen & Shedler, 1999a). Although this problem stems at least in part from the overlap among the DSM-IV dis- orders themselves, other approaches to diagnosis, such as assessing the patient’s match to a prototype of the disorder, show similar external corre- lates indicative of diagnostic validity while substantially decreasing estimates of comorbidity (Westen, Shedler, & Bradley, 2006). Second, neither struc- tured interviews nor questionnaires correlate strongly with consensus diag- noses made using all available data collected over time by teams of clinicians who not only have access to data from other informants but also know the patients well (e.g., Pilkonis et al., 1995; Pilkonis, Heape, Ruddy, & Serrao, 1991; Skodol, Oldham, Rosnick, Kellman, & Hyler, 1991). The third and most central problem of this approach is reliance on the self-awareness among a group of patients (PD patients) who, almost by definition, are likely to have distorted views of themselves and others. For example, Oltmanns, Turkheimer, and their colleagues have demonstrated across multiple samples that although lay informants converge remarkably well in assessing their peers’ personality pathology, aggregated peer assessments tend to correlate only on the order of r = .20 to .30 with self-reports (Clifton, Turkheimer, & Oltmanns, 2003; Klein, 2003; Oltmanns, Melley, & Turkheimer, 2002; Thomas, Turkheimer, & Oltmanns, 2003). This relatively modest level of self-informant agreement is only slightly lower than meta-analytic estimates, which are in the mid .30s (Klonsky, Oltmanns, & Turkheimer, 2002). For the more overt symptoms of BPD, such as self-mutilation and suicidal ideation, self-report biases are less likely to be problematic. For more subtle
  
Borderline Personality Disorder 173

personality symptoms, and particularly for externalizing symptoms (see Fiedler, Oltmanns, & Turkheimer, 2004), these biases may be more prob- lematic. Unfortunately, the more subtle personality symptoms appear to be the most stable indicators of the disorder (Grilo et al., 2004; Zanarini, Frankenburg, Vujanovic, et al., 2004).
Another approach to the assessment of personality, including BPD, relies on the use of a systematic clinical interview paired with psychometrically valid instruments for rating data gathered in the interview. Westen and Shedler (1999a; 1999b) developed a Q-sort instrument designed to quantify the judgments of experienced clinical interviewers, combining clinical description with statistical prediction. Clinically experienced observers sort the 200 items of the SWAP-II Q-sort (or its progenitor, the SWAP-200) based either on their observation of a patient over time in treatment or on data ascertained using a systematic clinical interview, the Clinical Diagnostic Interview (CDI; Westen & Muderrisoglu, 2003, 2006; Westen, Muderrisoglu, Fowler, Shedler, & Koren, 1997). The CDI differs from structured PD inter- views in that it does not primarily ask patients to describe themselves (although it does not avoid face-valid questions about behaviors, intentions, or phenomenology, such as whether the patient has self-mutilated or thought about suicide). Instead, it asks patients to provide detailed narratives about their symptoms, their school and work history, and their relationship history, focusing on specific examples of emotionally salient experiences. From these data (or from all available clinical data, if the clinician is describ- ing a patient in ongoing treatment), the clinician-informant makes judg- ments about the ways the patient characteristically thinks, feels, regulates impulses and emotions, views the self and others, and behaves in significant relationships, and these are reflected in the clinician’s placement (ranking) of the items.
Several recent studies using the SWAP-200 or the newly developed SWAP-II have focused on BPD (Bradley, Zittel, et al., 2005; Westen, Bradley, & Shedler, 2005; Westen & Shedler, 1999a, 1999b; Zittel & Westen, 2005). These studies indicate that SWAP-based assessment of BPD predicts external correlates, such as adaptive functioning and developmental history, in ways predicted by prior research (Zittel & Westen, 2005). These data also highlight the importance of understanding not only stress- dependent behaviors that are hallmarks of BPD (e.g., self-harming behavior) but also those characteristics (e.g., depressed mood, anxiety, hopelessness) that are characteristic of the everyday experience of BPD patients but not necessarily distinctive to them because they are common in psychiatric sam- ples (Bradley, Zittel, et al., 2005).
In addition, these studies identify aspects of BPD not captured fully by the nine DSM BPD criteria, which are probably better understood as indicators of a latent construct than as the signs and symptoms that exhaustively define the disorder. For example, SWAP-based data provide a more thorough description of affect dysregulation among BPD patients. Specifically, data
  
174
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

obtained using the SWAP-200 and SWAP-II reveal that emotion dysregula- tion in BPD comprises a tendency for emotions to spiral out of control, a ten- dency to become irrational under stress, and a dependence on others to regulate emotions.
All approaches to assessment have their limitations, and the SWAP is no exception. The most central limitation of most data obtained on BPD so far using the SWAP is that these data rely, like most studies using structured interviews and questionnaires, on the perspective of one informant (in this case, the clinician; in the modal study of BPD, the informant is the patient). Future research using all assessment procedures needs to triangulate data gathered from multiple sources, including self-reports, quantified clinical judgments, informant ratings (e.g., friends and family), and laboratory tasks.
Etiology of BPD ____________________________________
Research on BPD implicates a broad array of factors in the etiology of BPD, including biological/genetic factors, separation and loss, childhood abuse, global family environment, and disrupted attachments. Research on the eti- ology of BPD has largely addressed each of these domains separately and hence has not yet established models for their combination and interaction, although such work is under way. We will first review research for each of these etiologic factors and then summarize the current status of the field with respect to understanding their interplay.
Biological and Genetic Factors
Clearly, personality traits are heritable (see Plomin, Chipuer, & Loehlin, 1990), although the extent to which genetic transmission contributes to the development of BPD has yet to be fully understood. Nevertheless, a growing number of studies, including two preliminary twin studies (Nigg & Goldsmith, 1994; Torgersen, 1980; Torgersen et al., 2000), suggest the importance of familial aggregation. In a recent review of family studies of BPD, White, Gunderson, Zanarini, and Hudson (2003) found little support for familial links between schizophrenia or bipolar disorders and BPD, some support for familial links with major depression, and stronger support for familial aggre- gation of impulse spectrum disorders, including BPD itself. As we describe below, research that addresses both main effects and interactive effects in combination with environmental traumas is likely to prove more fruitful (see Nigg & Goldsmith, 1994; Torgersen, 1980; Torgersen et al., 2000; White et al., 2003).
An alternate approach to understanding the heritability of BPD is to look at subsyndromal markers, or endophenotypes, of the BPD construct (e.g., affect dysregulation and relationship instability). A recent study (Zanarini,
  
Borderline Personality Disorder 175

Frankenburg, Yong, et al., 2004) found that although the diagnosis of BPD showed familial aggregation, both specific BPD criteria and the broader BPD symptom categories of affect, cognition, impulsivity, and interpersonal rela- tionship disturbance showed even stronger familial aggregation and discrim- inated better between the relatives of BPD probands and those of comparison subjects. This idea is consistent with theory and research conceptualizing BPD as the extreme presentation of aspects of heritable temperament or traits (e.g., impulsivity, neuroticism, and affective lability; see Paris, 2003; Skodol, Gunderson, Pfohl, et al., 2002, for reviews). Impulsive aggression is a central characteristic of Cluster B Axis II disorders, particularly BPD and APD (Coccaro, Bergeman, & McClearn, 1993; Goodman & Yehuda, 2002; Skodol, Gunderson, Pfohl, et al., 2002), and shows substantial heritability. However, the data on familial aggregation are difficult to interpret because of the complexity in disaggregating heritable temperamental and family environ- ment effects (i.e., having a parent with borderline or related psychopathology increases the likelihood of adverse childhood events).
A burgeoning literature on the neurobiology of BPD focuses primarily on two trait aspects considered central to BPD: affect instability/dysregulation and impulsivity/impulsive aggression (e.g., Siever & Davis, 1991). Most functional neuroimaging research rests on the premise that BPD is associated with hyperreactivity to emotional stimuli, which should be manifest in such neural responses as heightened activation of the amygdala (Donegan et al., 2003; Herpertz et al., 2001). Several studies using functional magnetic reso- nance imaging (fMRI) do indeed find increased amygdala reactivity when these individuals are exposed to emotion-related stimuli, particularly faces (Donegan et al., 2003; Herpertz et al., 2001). Interestingly, BPD patients appear to show greater amygdala reactivity to neutral faces as well, perhaps supporting prior research linking BPD to a malevolence attribution style (Nigg, Lohr, Westen, Gold, & Silk, 1992; Westen, 1991b). Another study (Schmahl, Vermetten, Elzinga, & Bremner, 2003) identified decreased amyg- dala volume in BPD subjects (Driessen et al., 2000). Currently, the field is witnessing an explosion of fMRI research with BPD patients that is likely to elucidate the nature of the disorder (e.g., by examining links between amyg- dala reactivity and hypoactivity of cortical circuits that would normally reg- ulate it, such as the ventromedial prefrontal cortex). At the same time, such research should be treated cautiously from an etiologic standpoint. Finding that BPD patients show greater amygdala reactivity, for example, is impor- tant, but it does not go far beyond a neural translation of the definition of a disorder characterized by emotional reactivity. Complicating matters, the pattern of data across studies may be complex because of the complex ways BPD patients try to regulate their affects. For example, the amygdala of BPD patients who dissociate may be relatively less reactive than that of non-BPD patients (Schmahl et al., 2004).
The majority of the research looking at the biological basis of impulsivity/ impulsive aggression focuses on the role of reduced serotonergic responsivity
  
176
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

(for reviews, see Skodol, Gunderson, Pfohl, et al., 2002; Soloff, Lynch, Kelly, Malone, & Mann, 2000). For example, some research associates lower levels of 5-hydroxytryptophan (e.g., Mann, 1998) with increased self-harming and suicidal behaviors. Using positron-emission tomography (PET), Leyton et al. (2001) identified an inverse relationship between alpha-methyl-L-tryptophan (converted to alpha-methyl-serotonin) and impulsivity as measured by errors on a go/no-go task. Other studies (De La Fuente et al., 1997; Soloff, Meltzer, Greer, Constantine, & Kelly, 2000) suggest that orbital prefrontal dysfunction may be associated with increased aggression via inhibition of limbic regions. Other neuropsychological studies implicate impaired functioning on laboratory tasks designed to evaluate planning and decision making abilities (Bazanis et al., 2002; Lenzenweger, Clarkin, Fertuck, & Kernberg, 2004), which could impli- cate prefrontal circuits more broadly.
Separation and Loss
A considerable body of research points to separation from or loss of parental figures during childhood as etiologically relevant to BPD. For example, a meta-analytic review found that 20% to 40% of BPD patients had experienced traumatic separations from one or both parents (Gunderson & Sabo, 1993). Childhood histories involving lengthy separations from, or the permanent loss of, one or both parents have been found to discriminate BPD patients from patients with schizophrenia, depression, and other PDs (Akiskal et al., 1985; Bradley, 1979; Frank & Paris, 1981; Goldberg, Mann, Wise, & Segall, 1985; Gunderson, Kerr, & Englund, 1980; Links, Steiner, Offord, & Eppel, 1988; Paris, Nowlis, & Brown, 1988; Soloff & Millward, 1983; Zanarini, Gunderson, Marino, Schwartz, & Frankenburg, 1989).
In evaluating the relationship of separation and loss to BPD, however, factors such as the child’s age, nature and duration of the separation or loss, and availability of nurturant, enduring surrogate caregivers in the absence of the primary caregiver need to be taken into account. For example, a classic study of depression (see Brown & Harris, 1989, for details) found a con- stellation of symptoms resembling BPD to be highly prevalent among patients who had a peculiar kind of separation history (which they labeled “aberrant”), in which the mother appeared to have left the children for no “socially acceptable” reason (e.g., she abandoned her children for months because of her own instability). It is important to note when interpreting these findings that causal direction is not clear due to possible genetic con- founds or gene-environment interactions.
Childhood Abuse
Early writing on BPD (Stern, 1938) focused on the etiologic role of child- hood abuse, noting that “actual cruelty, neglect, and brutality by the parents
  
Borderline Personality Disorder 177

of many years’ duration are factors found in these patients. These factors operate more or less constantly over many years from earliest childhood. They are not single experiences” (p. 470). This early observation has been corroborated in the empirical literature, with numerous studies identifying a link between abuse, particularly childhood sexual abuse, and BPD (e.g., Ogata et al., 1990; Silk, Lohr, Ogata, & Westen, 1990; Westen, Ludolph, Misle, Ruffins, & Block, 1990; Zanarini, 1997). In Herman, Perry, and van der Kolk’s (1989) sample of BPD patients, 81% had childhood histories that included abuse, both physical abuse (71%) and sexual abuse (67%). In a community-based longitudinal study of PDs, Johnson, Cohen, Brown, Smailes, and Bernstein (1999) found that experiences of childhood physical, sexual, and emotional abuse increased risk for development of virtually all of the DSM-IV PDs. However, when they adjusted for the effects of co- occurring PDs, only the Cluster B PDs remained significantly related to expe- riences of childhood maltreatment. Zelkowitz, Paris, Guzder, and Feldman (2001) found that people who had experienced childhood sexual abuse were four times more likely to develop BPD than those who had not. Not sur- prisingly, some studies did not find such a significant link between childhood abuse and BPD (see Fossati, Madeddu, & Maffei, 1999). The association between child abuse and BPD exists in the context of multiple, interactive genetic, environmental, and social factors, and the idea of one-to-one corre- spondence is an unfounded oversimplification (Bradley, Jenei, & Westen, 2005; Paris, 1997).
In addition to the presence/absence of abuse, several studies suggest that characteristics of abuse, including severity, age of onset, and number of types of abuse experienced, contribute to degree of impairment related to border- line pathology (McLean & Gallop, 2003; Silk, Lee, Hill, & Lohr, 1995; Yen et al., 2002; Zanarini et al., 2002). Studying BPD in adolescents, Ludolph et al. (1990) suggested that cumulative trauma, rather than a single trau- matic event, appears to be more relevant to the development of BPD (see also Weaver & Clum, 1993). Thus, although not all patients with BPD have experienced childhood trauma, the current research indicates that traumatic experiences are a salient component of the developmental history of many individuals who develop BPD.
Family Environment
More generally, an unstable, nonnurturing family environment appears to contribute to the development of BPD. In adolescent patients, for example, the tendency to misunderstand people’s actions and intentions (poor understand- ing of social causality) characteristic of BPD shows a strong association (r = ~.50) with a simple metric of family instability, namely, the number of times the family moved (Westen, Ludolph, Block, Wixom, & Wiss, 1990). Much of the literature on traumatic precursors to PDs (and other psychiatric symptoms, such as depression) has not taken into account the impact of family
  
178
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

environment, making it difficult to disentangle the impact of sexual or physi- cal abuse from the overall family context within which abuse typically occurs, such as family chaos, disrupted attachments, multiple caregivers, parental neglect, alcoholism, and/or evidence of affective instability among family members (Dahl, 1995; Gunderson & Phillips, 1991; Ogata et al., 1990).
Studies of adverse childhood events have linked the number of such events to multiple adverse medical and psychiatric outcomes (Dong et al., 2004; Edwards, Holden, Anda, & Felitti, 2003). Research that has considered sev- eral of these variables together with regard to the etiology of PDs has often found that the context within which abuse occurs (e.g., problematic attach- ment relationships, emotional abuse, and neglect) is as strongly associated with BPD as the presence or absence of physical or sexual abuse (Johnson et al., 2001; Ludolph et al., 1990; Zanarini et al., 1989). For example, a recent study of the relationship between childhood abuse, family environ- ment, and BPD found that family environment partially mediated the rela- tionship between abuse and level of BPD symptoms (Bradley, Jenei, et al., 2005), although abuse showed a substantial unmediated relation to BPD. In other words, sexual trauma predicted BPD, but part of its impact reflected the effects of an unstable, nonnurturing family environment. The dearth of research on the relation between abuse and family environment is particu- larly problematic in the case of BPD, which is associated not only with the attribution of malevolence on others but also with fears of abandonment and aloneness that may be related to neglectful, absent, or unstable parent- ing (see Gunderson, 2001; Zanarini & Frankenburg, 1997).
One attempt to clarify this literature suggests that insecure attachment to parental figures, coupled with emotionally unstable or neglectful family environments, may account for the development of BPD, whereas physical or sexual abuse may account for symptom severity (Salzman, Salzman, & Wolfson, 1997). Other models suggest that sexual abuse may account for some of the severity of impulsive symptoms in BPD, such as self-mutilation, suicide attempts, substance abuse, promiscuity, running away, and assaultiveness (e.g., Westen, Ludolph, Misle, et al., 1990). Zanarini and Frankenburg (1997) reviewed research on the etiology of BPD and distin- guished three types of trauma that they felt better explained the pathogenesis of BPD. Type I trauma includes “unfortunate but not entirely unavoidable or unexpectable experiences,” including prolonged early separations, chronic insensitivity to the pre-borderline child’s feelings and needs, and serious emotional discord in the family, perhaps leading to separation or divorce. Type II trauma includes experiences of verbal and emotional abuse, neglect of age-appropriate physical needs, and circumscribed episodes of parental psychiatric illness. Type III trauma includes experiences of clear physical and sexual abuse, chronic psychiatric illness in caretaker or caretakers (particu- larly Axis II psychopathology and substance abuse), and a generally chaotic and dysfunctional home environment (e.g., parents repeatedly engaging in shouting matches, children physically assaulting one another, constant
  
Borderline Personality Disorder 179

disregard of family rules and invasions of other family members’ bound- aries). Zanarini and Frankenburg (1997) estimated that approximately half of borderline patients report a childhood characterized by type I and/or type II trauma, and the remaining half of borderline patients report a childhood characterized by all three types of trauma.
Attachment
Attachment theory (Bowlby, 1969, 1973) provides a framework for some of the most important recent theory and research on the etiology of BPD (see Agrawal, Gunderson, Holmes, & Lyons-Ruth, 2004, for a meta-analytic review). Research on attachment in BPD focuses on an infant’s or young child’s experience of unpredictable, frightening, and/or abusive caregiving that interrupts the formation of coherent internal working models of relationships. This presumably results in an inability to predict, understand, and flexibly respond to the actions of significant others (Lyons-Ruth & Jacobvitz, 1999; Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985). Attachment theorists describe a child’s attachment status as “disorganized-disoriented” when the child is faced with an unsolvable dilemma: separation from a caregiver causes distress; this activates proximity seeking; the caregiver is unavailable, unpredictable, or frightening; and the infant or child is left without coherent strategies for making sense of or obtaining security from the caregiver. This disorganized-disoriented attachment pattern in infancy (similar to the char- acterization of “unresolved with respect to loss and trauma” in adult attach- ment) ismarkedinlaboratorystudiesbyincoherentandineffectiveattempts to self-regulate following a separation from a caregiver. Instead, disorga- nized-disoriented infants demonstrate seemingly undirected or contradictory behavior, such as freezing, rocking, or head banging.
Research on disorganized attachment in children (beyond infancy) high- lights perceptions of parental figures as unpredictable, unavailable, and frightening. In one series of studies, children with disorganized attachment were more likely to respond to pictures of distressed children separated from their parents with stories depicting violent harm to the child or others (Kaplan, 1987; Main et al., 1985). In other studies, parents are described as unavailable, frightening, or frightened (Solomon, George, & De Jong, 1995), and dolls representing the child engage in angry/violent and idiosyncratic/ odd behavior (Cassidy, 1988). Interestingly, these findings parallel research on adolescent and adult patients with BPD using storytelling procedures such as the Thematic Apperception Test (TAT), which finds that BPD is associated with negative emotional tone of relationship descriptions and par- ticularly by malevolence attributions (Westen, 1991b; Westen, Lohr, Silk, Gold, & Kerber, 1990; Westen, Ludolph, Block, et al., 1990).
Because “unresolved” is a qualifier rather than one of the three primary attachment patterns coded categorically from the Adult Attachment Interview
  
180
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

(AAI), the most common attachment pattern associated with BPD is preoc- cupied (analogous to anxious/ambivalent in infancy and childhood). A com- bination of unresolved and preoccupied attachment has been associated with BPD in adolescents as well as adults (Nakash-Eisikovits, Dutra, & Westen, 2002; Westen, Thomas, Nakash, & Bradley, 2006). In general, preoccupied attachment in combination with the unresolved qualifier resembles the inter- personal style of BPD patients, marked by rejection sensitivity, alternation between anxious preoccupation and anger with attachment figures, and inco- herent strategies for attempting to make intimate contact with others.
Recent work integrating object relations and attachment theories con- nects insecure or disorganized attachment to BPD symptoms using the con- cept of mentalization (Fonagy, Target, Gergely, Allen, & Bateman, 2003). Mentalization refers to the ability to make sense of one’s own and others’ actions by reflecting on and understanding their mental states (including feelings, beliefs, wishes, and ideas). In healthy development, this capacity is developed in the context of attachment relationships with primary care- givers, during which infants and children develop internal working models of self, others, and relationships (Bowlby, 1988). According to Fonagy, an inability to make sense of one’s own and others’ mental (and particular emo- tional) states not only results from interactions with inexplicable caregivers but also renders anticipation of attachment figures’ actions impossible, lead- ing to difficulty in self-regulating emotion.
Difficulties with mentalization can also be seen in an implicit or explicit belief in a one-to-one correspondence between one’s perceptions (of situa- tions, others’ feelings and motives, etc.) and reality, and hence in an inability to consider possible alternate interpretations. This often leads to instability of interpersonal relationships, as whatever emotion one feels in reaction to others (e.g., anger, happiness) is perceived as directly and unquestionably reflective of the other’s feelings or intentions (e.g., intent to harm), whether correctly or incorrectly perceived. According to this model, deficits in men- talization also contribute to an unstable sense of self and a sense of emptiness.
Interaction of Biological and Psychosocial Risk Factors
Despite the relatively neat categorization of putative risk factors presented above, research dating back to Harlow’s monkeys should lead to circum- spection in making distinct attributions to nature or nurture in the etiology of BPD or any other psychiatric disorder. What begins as a biological vul- nerability may lead to a cascade of environmental events, just as what may begin as an environmental effect may become “hard-wired.” Data on the interplay of risk factors in the development of BPD do not exist at this point, primarily because BPD is not officially diagnosed until age 18 and compre- hensive longitudinal studies of the development of personality disorders beginning in infancy have yet to be conducted. However, a number of studies in domains related to BPD (e.g., childhood sexual and physical abuse,
  
Borderline Personality Disorder 181

attachment disorganization, impulsivity and depressed mood) have demon- strated that both psychological and biological influences play important roles (see Judd & McGlashan, 2003, for a review).
Two areas of research are particularly relevant to BPD. Caspi, Moffitt, and colleagues (Caspi et al., 2002; Caspi et al., 2003) have focused on gene- environment interactions in a large longitudinal sample in New Zealand. In a landmark study (2002), they found that a functional polymorphism in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter (5-HTT) gene moderated the influence of stressful life events in both childhood and adulthood on subse- quent depression. Stressful events in adulthood, as well as abuse in childhood, predicted subsequent depressive symptoms and suicidality—two features that in combination often point to the presence of BPD—in individuals with the short allele of the 5-HTT promoter as compared to individuals homozygous for the long allele. In a second study (2003), they found that a functional polymorphism in a gene regulating monoamine oxidase (MAO) moderated the relationship between child abuse and antisocial behavior in adulthood.
Research on the neurobiology of early life stress also highlights the impor- tance of the interaction of biological and environmental factors in the devel- opment of psychopathology. These studies suggest that early life stress modifies brain circuits involved in stress regulation, resulting in a type of “biological priming” that interacts with genetic vulnerabilities to increase the risk of later psychopathology (Heim, Meinlschmidt, & Nemeroff, 2003). Though none of the research to date directly addresses the development of BPD, the types of early life stress studied (notably early separation from mother in animal analogue studies and childhood sexual and physical abuse in human studies) as well as the domains of documented outcomes (e.g., depression and substance abuse) are germane to an understanding of the interaction of genetic and biological risk factors in BPD.
___________ Prognosis and Natural Course of the Disorder
A small but growing body of research on the longitudinal course of BPD exists, and although the findings of these studies are not entirely consistent, several broad characterizations emerge. One is that patients tend to lose their BPD diagnosis over time. For example, the longitudinal McLean Adult Development Study found a remission rate of 35% at 2 years, 50% at 4 years, and 69% at 6 years (Zanarini, Frankenburg, Hennen, & Silk, 2003).
To what extent the instability in the borderline diagnosis (and other PD diagnoses) is an artifact of arbitrary cutoff points for categorical diagnosis, the mixed diagnostic criteria for BPD in the DSM (enduring personality characteristics interspersed with typically stress-dependent behaviors), or the limited test-retest reliability of structured interviews at intervals beyond 6 weeks is unclear. What is clear is that dimensional assessments of both BPD (number of criteria met) and traits associated with BPD (e.g., negative
 
182
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

affectivity, neuroticism) show far more temporal stability than categorical diagnoses (Lenzenweger, Johnson, & Willett, 2004; Skodol et al., 2005).
Although longer-term follow-up studies have the disadvantage of less structured diagnostic procedures, they provide additional data suggesting a course of general improvement if BPD patients can survive their 20s and 30s. In one long-term follow-up study, Paris and Zweig-Frank (2001) found a high remission rate consistent with more recent studies using structured interviews, with only 25% of patients still meeting BPD criteria at 15 years and 7.8% meeting criteria at 27 years. McGlashan (1986) found that BPD patients discharged from an intensive inpatient program fared best two decades following discharge. These data, consistent with other data on exter- nalizing disorders such as antisocial PD and substance abuse disorders, sug- gest that BPD tends to “burn out” with age. In long-term follow-up studies by McGlashan (1986) and by Stone (1987; 1992), patients with better out- comes tended to be higher in intelligence, more talented in the arts, more physically attractive, and/or described as more likable than those with poorer outcomes. The more chronically impaired patients were more likely to have sustained problems with alcohol abuse, histories of severe physical or sexual abuse, severe problems with impulsivity, comorbidity for antiso- cial PD, or schizotypal features.
BPD, however, is marked by internalizing symptoms and interpersonal difficulties as well as the more notable externalizing symptoms. Data suggest that although the impulsive and aggressive (both other- and self-directed) features tend to improve with time, core personality attributes such as neg- ative affect, emotion dysregulation, and difficulties in intimate relationships tend to persist, whether or not the person continues to meet formal criteria for BPD. Depressive and anxious symptoms tend to remain high even among “recovered” BPD patients. Likewise, problems related to interpersonal rela- tionships, including social isolation and fear of abandonment, tend to endure over time (McGlashan, 1986; Paris & Zweig-Frank, 2001; Zanarini et al., 2003). There is some suggestion that long-term interpersonal patterns among BPD patients tend to be bimodal, with some patients tending to become socially isolated (likely as a way of regulating the intensely distress- ing interpersonal patterns that tend to exacerbate BPD symptoms), whereas others become better able to maintain committed relationships. Some patients become symptomatic again during midlife in response to separation, divorce, or death of a spouse (McGlashan, 1986; Paris & Zweig-Frank, 2001; Stone, 1987, 1992).
One of the major risks for BPD patients seen in these and other studies is suicide. Long-term studies of patients with BPD suggest a suicide rate in the range of 3% to 10% (Black, Blum, Pfohl, & Hale, 2004). Following patients over a 27-year period, Paris and Zweig-Frank (2001) found a 10.3% rate of suicide (most of which occurred before age 40). Two of the most robust vari- ables predicting greater risk of suicide in BPD patients include substance abuse and comorbid depression, although the latter is characteristic of most BPD patients (Black et al., 2004; Fyer et al., 1988; Isometsa et al., 1996).
  
Borderline Personality Disorder 183

_______________________________ Treatment Approaches
Patients with BPD are often considered difficult to treat. The first treatments for borderline patients, which began to emerge in the 1950s and 1960s, were modified forms of psychoanalytic psychotherapy, two of which have recently been tested in randomized controlled trials (RCTs). Cognitive- behavioral therapies for BPD began to emerge in the 1980s, when Linehan developed dialectical behavior therapy (DBT; Linehan, 1993). Integrative therapies have not been tested empirically but are widely practiced. We describe each treatment approach in turn.
Psychodynamic Psychotherapy
Because of centrality of the construct of personality (or character) to its con- ceptions of treatment, and because of the legacy of early theories on border- line states and personality organization, psychodynamic approaches constitute the largest body of theoretical work on the treatment of BPD. Psychodynamic approaches to BPD all share a grounding in developmental psychopathology, arguing that the only way to understand the disorder is through understand- ing the way development has gone awry in the way the patient experiences the self and others, regulates emotions, and regulates impulses (see Bradley & Westen, 2005). Although psychodynamic psychotherapy for BPD varies widely, two approaches have predominated in the clinical literature. The first is Kernberg’s (1975) approach, which represents a confluence of multiple psy- choanalytic schools of thought, particularly ego psychology (which focuses on adaptive functions) and object relations theory (which focuses on interpersonal relationships and the representation of self and others). The second, derived from the self-psychological approach of Kohut (1977), is based on work by Adler and Buie (Adler, 1981, 1989; Adler & Buie, 1979; Buie & Adler, 1982), which focuses on identity and self-soothing.
Similar to all psychodynamic approaches to the treatment of BPD, Kernberg’s approach attends to the interpersonal dynamics of BPD patients (e.g., fear of abandonment) and uses the relationship with the therapist to address distorted ways of understanding interpersonal relations. More specifically, Kernberg’s perspective focuses on the importance of aggression in borderline patients (often projected onto others) and a defensive style marked by “splitting” (a tendency to see the self and others as all good or all bad). Kernberg’s treatment focuses on confronting aggression and manipu- lation, helping patients attain more balanced views of the self and others, and interpreting conflicts impeding the capacity to love and work.
The self-psychological approach of Adler, Buie, and others assumes that borderline patients’ problems lie less in their conflicts than in psychological deficits, particularly in their capacity to self-soothe. Whereas Kernberg tends to see the aggression of borderline patients as primary, the self-psychological approach views borderline patients’ rage as secondary to other feelings, such
   
184
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

as the pain of abandonment. From this perspective, BPD is a developmental disorder derived from a failure to develop soothing images of primary care- givers that the person can call upon in times of distress. Thus the primary problem facing patients with BPD is the inability to self-soothe and the need to find others who can help them regulate their feelings as well as their self- esteem. This approach to treatment emphasizes empathic attunement with borderline patients and works to help them internalize soothing functions not developed in childhood.
Like most psychodynamic therapies, the ratio of theory to empirical out- come research is unfortunately high. However, two psychodynamic approaches to the treatment of BPD have recently been manualized (using principle- based manuals) and tested in RCTs. The first, based on Kernberg’s work, is called transference-focused psychotherapy (TFP). The second, based on Fonagy’s attachment research, is called mentalization-based treatment (MBT). Both share a primary focus on changing patients’ mental represen- tations of themselves and others.
Transference-Focused Psychotherapy
Transference-focused psychotherapy (Clarkin, Yeomans, & Kernberg, 1999; Yeomans, 2004) places primary emphasis on the poorly integrated representations that empirically are characteristic of patients with BPD (Westen, 1991b; Westen & Shedler, 1999a; Zittel & Westen, 2005). The treatment proceeds through a hierarchy of goals, moving from containment of suicidal and self-destructive behavior and establishment of a stable treat- ment frame to a focus on dominant relationship patterns. As the name implies, TFP focuses on clarification, confrontation, and interpretation within the context of the patient-therapist relationship. TFP focuses on pre- sent-oriented identification of the dominant relationship paradigm (e.g., ide- alizer-idealized, victim-victimizer) active in the patient-therapist relationship. This process includes observing and interpreting changes in this relational configuration (e.g., a switch in role from victim to victimizer or perpetrator) and increasing patient awareness of split representations of self and others.
The principle underlying TFP is that increased awareness and under- standing of distortions and expectations the patient brings to relationships will lead to more coherent, integrated views of the self and others, which will in turn generate an increased ability to regulate emotions, particularly those emerging from interpersonal interactions. A preliminary study of TFP (Clarkin et al., 2001) evaluated 23 female patients in twice-weekly TFP over the course of 12 months. Examining pre-to-post change, the study found sig- nificantly reduced levels of suicide attempts, decreased severity of injury resulting from self-harming behavior, and fewer days and numbers of hos- pitalizations. A randomized controlled trial of TFP comparing it with sup- portive psychodynamic therapy (treatment as usual at Kernberg’s site) and DBT has recently been completed (Clarkin, Levy, Lenzenweger, & Kernberg,
  
Borderline Personality Disorder 185

2004). Although all three treatments produced positive outcomes, of inter- est is that the hypothesized mediating variable (changes in the structure of representations of self and others) appeared linked to change only in the TFP condition.
Mentalization-Based Therapy
Mentalization-based therapy is a relatively recent effort to operationalize an approach to treatment grounded in attachment theory (Fonagy, Target, & Gergely, 2000). This approach focuses on developing increased mentalization capacities in BPD patients. One of the aims of the treatment approach is to help patients identify and understand emotions by clarifying and naming them, understanding immediate precipitants, understanding the emotion in the context of past and current relationships, learning to express the emo- tion appropriately, and learning to understand the response others are most likely to have in reaction to the patient’s emotional expression (Bateman & Fonagy, 2003). The therapist maintains a “mentalizing stance” by focusing on and discussing the here-and-now mental states of the therapist and patient. Transference interpretations are kept simple and made with respect to rela- tively immediate or “experience near” circumstances (e.g., the patient’s ten- dency to quit psychotherapy when she begins to feel too close to the therapist), avoiding historical interpretations (e.g., how this is related to her early expe- riences with her mother).
A preliminary study of MBT (Bateman & Fonagy, 1999) compared a con- trol group (n = 19) of patients receiving “general psychiatric services” with patients (n = 19) participating in a psychoanalytically oriented partial hospi- talization program based on an MBT approach. The maximum length of the partial hospitalization program was 18 months. All patients were assessed at 3-month intervals over an 18-month period. Results indicated decreased self- mutilation and suicide attempts; reduced length of inpatient hospitalization; and decreases in self-reported anxiety, depression, and interpersonal prob- lems. Data collected at 18-month follow-up (e.g., 36 months from start of treatment) found that these treatment benefits were maintained (Bateman & Fonagy, 2001). These promising but preliminary data led to an outpatient adaptation of MBT with a more clearly operationalized treatment manual (Bateman & Fonagy, 2004), with a treatment study currently under way.
Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy (CBT)
The first and best studied CBT approach to treatment of BPD is DBT, a modular, manualized treatment program for patients with BPD that may be implemented in inpatient or outpatient settings. DBT relies on a combination of skills training, usually implemented in group format, in four areas: mind- fulness, interpersonal effectiveness, distress tolerance, and emotion regulation.
  
186
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

In addition to skills training in a group format, DBT includes an individual therapy component with several distinctive features. These include a functional analysis of behavior (focusing particularly on self-harming and therapy inter- fering behaviors); a “validating” approach focused on depathologizing the patient’s difficulties and emphasizing how impulsive and self-harming behav- iors such as parasuicidal behavior are understandable, albeit not effective, efforts to manage distress or emotion dysregulation; and 24-hour therapist availability for suicidality coupled with behavioral principles intended to mit- igate the need for such phone calls.
DBT is the only treatment for BPD that has been widely subjected to empirical scrutiny, and it has proven effective in decreasing suicide attempts, self-injurious behaviors (e.g., cutting, binge eating), and hospitalizations (Koons et al., 2001; Linehan, Armstrong, Suarez, Allmon, & Heard, 1991; Linehan, Heard, & Armstrong, 1993; Verheul et al., 2003). Other studies have found changes in a variety of domains, such as anger and dissociation (Linehan, Tutek, Heard, & Armstrong, 1994). A full review of studies on DBT is beyond the scope of this chapter (for reviews, see Robins & Chapman, 2004; Scheel, 2000; Westen, 2000). More recently, Young (Young, Klosko, & Weishaar, 2003) and Beck, Davis, and Freeman (2004) have developed more cognitive approaches to the treatment of PDs, including BPD. A recent study of long-term treatment using Young’s schema-focused therapy produced promising results (Giesen-Bloo, et al., 2006).
Integrative Treatment
The three treatments described above represent only the most broadly known and best-evaluated approaches. A number of other psychodynamic, cognitive-behavioral, and integrative approaches to the treatment of BPD also exist but are beyond the constraints of this chapter (see Aviram, Hellerstein, Gerson, & Stanley, 2004; Blum, Pfohl, St. John, Monahan, & Black, 2002; Brown, Newman, Charlesworth, Crits-Christoph, & Beck, 2004; Ryle, 2004; Westen, 1991a, 2000). Some common elements, however, cut across most of these treatments for BPD, and make considerable sense in light of the nature of borderline psychopathology.
The first is the importance of establishing a clear framework for the treat- ment that spells out expectations and boundaries for both the therapist and the patient. A second is frequency and length of treatment, with each approach including some form of biweekly contact over the course of at least twelve months. Third, all of the treatment approaches for BPD attend closely to the dynamics of the relationship between the therapist and the patient and make discussion of this relationship a central aspect of therapy. Fourth, although the construct is framed differently, emotion and impulse dysregu- lation is at the center of virtually all approaches.
Fifth and finally, treatments tend to proceed through a series of stages, similar to those proposed in a stage-based approach to the treatment of the
  
Borderline Personality Disorder 187

near-neighbor construct of “complex posttraumatic stress disorder” (e.g., Brown, Scheflin, & Hammond, 1998; Herman, 1992). The first stage, linked to keeping the patient alive and in treatment, focuses on stabilization of behavior and reduction of emotion dysregulation (e.g., getting impulsive self-harmful behaviors under control). The second stage focuses on under- standing of past experiences with a focus on how prior life experiences are manifest in current patterns (as opposed to exploration of the past as an archaeological dig, as in more classic psychoanalytic approaches). The third stage addresses reorganization of both internal (representational and affect- regulatory) and external (behavioral) processes related to interpersonal rela- tionships. Although these principles remain untested, current funding priorities in the United States and Britain, where most of the treatment research on BPD has been conducted, do not encourage the testing of treat- ments of a duration (i.e., years) that virtually all experts on BPD, from Kernberg to Linehan, indicate are necessary for adequate, effective treatment of the disorder. In practice, however, we suspect that effective treatment of BPD likely requires flexibility and integration across treatment approaches, particularly given the mélange of personality problems and Axis I symptoms with which BPD patients typically present.
_________________ Future Directions for the Study of BPD
Research on BPD has expanded exponentially over the last 25 years. Here we briefly describe three domains we believe to be central to progress in research on BPD: identification of improved diagnostic criteria for BPD, approaches to BPD diagnosis, and exploration of potential subtypes of BPD.
How Can We Improve the Diagnostic Criteria for BPD?
Despite consensus on the problems with the current BPD diagnostic cri- teria, no such consensus exists on the best remedy. The least radical solution would be to continue with diagnostic business as usual by tinkering with the current diagnostic criteria to create a modestly improved criterion set (e.g., modifying the affective lability criterion to include both pervasive negative affect and affect dysregulation). This approach, however, has not solved the problems with the BPD diagnosis over the last 25 years, and it is unlikely that any set of 7 to 10 items will adequately capture this complex, multifac- eted disorder while distinguishing it from near-neighbor disorders.
An alternative, more radical approach is a construct validation approach, which would take a large group of candidate criteria; collect data from a large, diverse sample without assuming any preexisting diagnostic group- ings; and use statistical aggregation techniques to identify and validate emer- gent traits or configurations of traits. Practically speaking, it is unlikely that
 
188
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

the field will allow for elimination of the current BPD diagnosis (presuming this would be the result of full-scale construct validation). Although some resistance to wholesale reworking of the BPD diagnosis no doubt results from inertia and the comfort with the familiar (not to mention the time and money spent developing assessments for the current diagnosis), some rests on the fact that BPD is a clinically useful construct backed by a large body of research. Data of this sort collected with the use of self-report instruments have allowed researchers to construct a BPD prototype from the traits rep- resented in the five-factor model (Trull, Widiger, Lynam, & Costa, 2003), and similar data have identified both a trait and a personality constellation heavily marked by borderline features, called emotion dysregulation or emo- tionally dysregulated PD, in both adolescents and adults (Shedler & Westen, 2004; Westen, Dutra, & Shedler, 2005; Westen & Shedler, 1999b; Westen, Shedler, Durrett, Glass, & Martens, 2003).
A compromise between business as usual and radical overhaul would be a hybrid, or “construct validation lite,” approach. This procedure would involve selecting a group of patients with a moderate to high degree of match to the current BPD diagnosis and then identifying their most salient person- ality characteristics from an item pool that includes but is not limited to DSM-IV criteria. Using a broad range of both patients and candidate crite- ria would allow for a more comprehensive description of personality fea- tures of BPD without “throwing the baby out with the bathwater.”
How Should We Diagnose BPD?
Regardless of the criteria ultimately used to diagnose BPD, a second question regarding diagnosis remains, namely, how diagnostic criteria should be applied to individual cases. As noted by Sokal (1974), taxonomy (developing a classifi- cation) and diagnosis (identifying cases) are independent aspects of the classifi- cation process. Researchers have proposed several alternatives to the current DSM procedure of making categorical (yes/no) decisions on each diagnostic cri- terion, counting the number of criteria met, and applying arbitrary cutoffs. The least radical is simply to use the same procedure except to dimensionalize it, using number of symptoms met as a dimensional diagnosis, perhaps supple- mented by categorical diagnosis for clinical communication. A second, less con- servative method is a prototype matching approach (see Westen & Bradley, 2005; Westen & Shedler, 2000; Westen et al., 2006), in which clinicians rate the resemblance between the patient and a diagnostic prototype (e.g., in the form of a paragraph descriptive of a prototypical patient with the disorder). A recently completed study (unpublished data) finds that clinicians can make dimensional diagnoses using prototype matching with a simple five-point scale (1 = description does not apply; 2 = patient has some features of this disorder; 3 = patient has significant features of this disorder; 4 = patient has this disorder, diagnosis applies; 5 = patient exemplifies this disorder, prototypical case) with
  
Borderline Personality Disorder 189

high reliability. Other research finds that this prototype matching approach decreases diagnostic comorbidity when compared with dimensional diagnoses made by counting symptoms, with slight improvements, rather than decre- ments, in construct validity (Westen et al., 2006). Finally, the most radical approach would be to eliminate the BPD diagnosis and replace it with a four- or five-factor trait diagnosis, and to “reconstruct” the BPD diagnosis if neces- sary using four or five factor prototypes as described by Widiger and his colleagues (Trull et al., 2003). To what extent one of these approaches is more empirically valid and clinically useful than the others will require head-to-head comparisons, in which multiple approaches are all tested in the same data set. Unfortunately, to date, research has typically tested each approach in isolation or in comparison with only the current DSM-IV approach.
Do Subtypes of BPD Exist?
The heterogeneity in the clinical presentations that can yield a BPD diag- nosis raises the question of whether the diagnosis may include meaningful subtypes or subgroups. Grinker, Werble, and Drye (1968), who undertook the first empirical study of borderline pathology, conducted the first research identifying subtypes of BPD. Using a sample of 51 psychiatric inpatients, they identified four groups of BPD patients: a more psychotic group (which later influenced the schizotypal diagnosis in DSM-III), a more neurotic group, a “core” borderline group, and an “as-if” (identity-changing) group. Theorizing about subgroups of borderline patients rests primarily on clinical observation (e.g., Oldham, 2001; Stone, 1994). However, recent research using the SWAP-200 and SWAP-II has consistently produced two- and three-cluster solutions in DSM-IV–defined adults and adolescents with BPD (Bradley, Zittel, et al., 2005; Conklin, Bradley, & Westen, 2006; Conklin & Westen, 2005; Westen & Shedler, 1999b; Zittel & Westen, 2002). The first two subtypes have been replicated across all samples. The first is an inter- nalizing dysregulated subtype marked by severe dysphoria and desperate efforts to manage it (e.g., through cutting or suicide attempts). The second is an externalizing dysregulated subgroup marked by a tendency to be rageful rather than depressed and to try to self-regulate by blaming or attacking others instead of oneself. The third is a histrionic-impulsive subtype marked by a tendency to experience both intense positive and intense negative emo- tions and attempts to regulate both positive and negative affect through impulsive and sensation-seeking behavior. Across both adolescent and adult samples, these subtypes have demonstrated meaningful differences with respect to external correlates indicative of construct validity, such as adap- tive functioning and etiology. Moreover, although the DSM criteria may capture many aspects of the externalizing dysregulated subtype, the intense pain manifest in the internalizing subtype is not captured by the diagnostic criteria.
  
190 PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Conclusion ________________________________________
Borderline personality disorder is a complex disorder—or, more likely, spec- trum of pathology—whose phenomenology, etiology, prognosis, and treat- ment researchers have made great strides in understanding since its official introduction into the psychiatric nomenclature in 1980 in DSM-III. Nevertheless, the diagnosis itself is clearly in need of revision to minimize artifactual comorbidity with the majority of the other nine PDs in DSM-IV and to maximize both its construct validity and clinical utility.
References _________________________________________
Adler, G. (1981). The borderline-narcissistic personality disorder continuum. American Journal of Psychiatry, 138, 46–50.
Adler, G. (1989). Uses and limitations of Kohut’s self psychology in the treatment of bor- derline patients. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 37, 761–785. Adler, G., & Buie, D. (1979). Aloneness and borderline psychopathology: The possible relevance of child development issues. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 60,
83–96.
Agrawal, H. R., Gunderson, J. G., Holmes, B. M., & Lyons-Ruth, K. (2004).
Attachment studies with borderline patients: A review. Harvard Review of
Psychiatry, 12, 94–104.
Akiskal, H. S. (1996). The prevalent clinical spectrum of bipolar disorders: Beyond
DSM-IV. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 16, 4S–14S.
Akiskal, H. S. (2004). Demystifying borderline personality: Critique of the concept and unorthodox reflections on its natural kinship with the bipolar spectrum.
Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 110, 401–407.
Akiskal, H. S., Chen, S. E., Davis, G. C., Puzantian, V. R., Kashgarian, M., &
Bolinger, J. M. (1985). Borderline: An adjective in search of a noun. Journal of
Clinical Psychiatry, 46, 41–48.
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of men-
tal disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.
Aviram, R. B., Hellerstein, D. J., Gerson, J., & Stanley, B. (2004). Adapting sup- portive psychotherapy for individuals with borderline personality disorder who
self-injure or attempt suicide. Journal of Psychiatric Practice, 10, 145–155. Barasch, J., Kroll, J., Carey, K., & Sines, L. (1983). Discriminating borderline personal- ity disorder from other personality disorders: Cluster and analysis of the Diagnostic
Interview for Borderline. Archives of General Psychiatry, 40, 1297–1302. Bateman, A., & Fonagy, P. (1999). The effectiveness of partial hospitalization in the treatment of borderline personality disorder: A randomized controlled trial.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 1563–1569.
Bateman, A., & Fonagy, P. (2001). Treatment of borderline personality disorder with
psychoanalytically oriented partial hospitalization: An 18-month follow-up. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 36–41.
  
Borderline Personality Disorder 191

Bateman, A., & Fonagy, P. (2003). The development of an attachment-based treat- ment program for borderline personality disorder. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 67, 187–211.
Bateman, A., & Fonagy, P. (2004). Mentalization-based treatment of BPD. Journal of Personality Disorders, 18, 36–51.
Bazanis, E., Rogers, R. D., Dowson, J. H., Taylor, P., Meux, C., Staley, C., et al. (2002). Neurocognitive deficits in decision-making and planning of patients with DSM- III-R borderline personality disorder. Psychological Medicine, 32, 1395–1405.
Beck, A. T., Davis, D. D., & Freeman, A. (2004). Cognitive therapy of personality disorders (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.
Becker, D. F., Grilo, C. M., Edell, W. S., & McGlashan, T. H. (2000). Comorbidity of borderline personality disorder with other personality disorders in hospital- ized adolescents and adults. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 2011–2016.
Black, D. W., Blum, N., Pfohl, B., & Hale, N. (2004). Suicidal behavior in border- line personality disorder: Prevalence, risk factors, prediction, and prevention. Journal of Personality Disorders, 18(3), 226–239.
Blum, N., Pfohl, B., St. John, D., Monahan, P., & Black, D. W. (2002). A cognitive- behavioral systems-based group treatment for outpatients with borderline personality disorder: A preliminary report. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 43, 301–310.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment (Vol. 1). New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Separation (Vol. 2). London: Hogarth Press.
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human devel-
opment. New York: Basic Books.
Bradley, R., Jenei, J., & Westen, D. (2005). Etiology of borderline personality disor-
der: Disentangling the contributions of intercorrelated antecedents. Journal of
Nervous and Mental Disease, 193, 24–31.
Bradley, R., & Westen, D. (2005). The psychodynamics of borderline personality disorder:
A view from developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology,
17(4), 927–957.
Bradley, R., Zittel, C., & Westen, D. (2005). Borderline personality disorder in adoles-
cence: Phenomenology and subtypes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
46, 1006–1019.
Bradley, S. J. (1979). The relationship of early maternal separation to borderline
personality in children and adolescents: A pilot study. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 136, 424–426.
Brown, D., Scheflin, A. W., & Hammond, D. C. (1998). Memory, trauma treatment,
and the law. New York: W.W. Norton.
Brown, G. K., Newman, C. F., Charlesworth, S. E., Crits-Christoph, P., & Beck,
A. T. (2004). An open clinical trial of cognitive therapy for borderline personal-
ity disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 18, 257–271.
Brown, G. W., & Harris, T. O. (1989). Life events and illness. New York: Guilford Press. Buie, D. H., & Adler, G. (1982). Definitive treatment of the borderline personality.
International Journal of Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy, 9, 51–87.
Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., et al. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297,
851–854.
Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Craig, I. W., Harrington, H., et al.
(2003). Influence of life stress on depression: Moderation by a polymorphism in
the 5-HTT gene. Science, 301, 386–389.
Cassidy, J. (1988). Child-mother attachment and the self in six-year-olds. Child
  
Development, 59, 121–134.
192
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Clark, L. A., & Harrison, J. A. (2001). Assessment instruments. In W. J. Livesley (Ed.), Handbook of personality disorders: Theory, research, and treatment. New York: Guilford Press.
Clark, L. A., Livesley, W. J., & Morey, L. (1997). Personality disorder assessment: The challenge of construct validity. Journal of Personality Disorders, 11, 205–231.
Clarkin, J. F., Foelsch, P. A., Levy, K. N., Hull, J. W., Delaney, J. C., & Kernberg, O. F. (2001). The development of a psychodynamic treatment for patients with borderline personality disorder: A preliminary study of behavioral change. Journal of Personality Disorders, 15, 487–495.
Clarkin, J. F., Levy, K. N., Lenzenweger, M. F., & Kernberg, O. F. (2004). The Personality Disorders Institute/Borderline Personality Disorder Research Foundation randomized control trial for borderline personality disorder: Rationale, methods, and patient characteristics. Journal of Personality Disorders, 18, 52–72.
Clarkin, J. F., Widiger, T. A., Frances, A., Hurt, S. W., & Gilmore, M. (1983). Prototypic typology and the borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92, 263–275.
Clarkin, J. F., Yeomans, F. E., & Kernberg, O. F. (1999). Psychotherapy for border- line personality. New York: Wiley.
Clifton, A., Turkheimer, E., & Oltmanns, T. F. (2003). Self and peer perspectives on pathological personality traits and interpersonal problems. Unpublished manu- script, University of Virginia, Charlottesville.
Coccaro, E. F., Bergeman, C., & McClearn, G. E. (1993). Heritability of irritable impulsiveness: A study of twins reared together and apart. Psychiatry Research, 48(3), 229–242.
Conklin, C. Z., & Westen, D. (2005). Borderline personality disorder as seen in clin- ical practice: Implications for DSM-V. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 867–875.
Conklin, C. Z., Bradley, R., & Westen, D. (2006). Affect regulation in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 194(2), 69–77.
Cowdry, R. (1992). Psychobiology and psychopharmacology of borderline person- ality disorder. Madison, CT: International Universities Press.
Dahl, A. (1995). Borderline disorders: The validity of the diagnostic concept. Psychiatric Development, 3, 109–152.
De La Fuente, J. M., Goldman, S., Stanus, E., Vizuete, C., Morlan, I., Bobes, J., et al. (1997). Brain glucose metabolism in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 31, 531–541.
Donegan, N. H., Sanislow, C. A., Blumberg, H. P., Fulbright, R. K., Lacadie, C., Skudlarski, P., et al. (2003). Amygdala hyperreactivity in borderline personality disorder: Implications for emotional dysregulation. Biological Psychiatry, 54(11), 1284–1293.
Dong, M., Anda, R. F., Felitti, V. J., Dube, S. R., Williamson, D. F., Thompson, T. J., et al. (2004). The interrelatedness of multiple forms of childhood abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction. Child Abuse & Neglect, 28, 771–784.
Driessen, M., Herrmann, J., Stahl, K., Zwaan, M., Meier, S., Hill, A., et al. (2000). Magnetic resonance imaging volumes of the hippocampus and the amygdala in women with borderline personality disorder and early traumatization. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 1115–1122.
Borderline Personality Disorder 193

Edwards, V. J., Holden, G. W., Anda, R. F., & Felitti, V. J. (2003). Experiencing mul- tiple forms of childhood maltreatment and adult mental health: Results from the Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1453–1460.
Ellison, J., Barsky, A., & Blum, N. R. (1989). Frequent repeaters to an emergency service. Hospital & Community Psychiatry, 40, 958–960.
Fiedler, E., Oltmanns, T., & Turkheimer, E. (2004). Traits associated with person- ality disorders and adjustment to military life: Predictive validity of self and peer reports. Military Medicine, 169, 32–40.
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1997). Structured clin- ical interview for DSM-IV personality disorders (SCID-II). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Fonagy, P., Target, M., & Gergely, G. (2000). Attachment and borderline personal- ity disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 23, 103–122.
Fonagy, P., Target, M., Gergely, G., Allen, J. G., & Bateman, A. (2003). The devel- opmental roots of borderline personality disorder in early attachment relation- ships: A theory and some evidence. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 23, 412–459.
Forman, E. M., Berk, M. S., Henriques, G. R., Brown, G. K., & Beck, A. T. (2004). History of multiple suicide attempts as a behavioral marker of severe psy- chopathology. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 437–443.
Fossati, A., Madeddu, F., & Maffei, C. (1999). Borderline personality disorder and childhood sexual abuse: A meta-analytic study. Journal of Personality Disorders, 13(3), 268–280.
Frank, H., & Paris, J. (1981). Recollections of family experience in borderline patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 1031–1034.
Fyer, M., Frances, A., Sullivan, T., Hurt, S., & Clarkin, J. (1988). Comorbidity of borderline personality disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 45, 348–352.
Giesen-Bloo, J., van Dyck, R., Spinhoven, P., van Tilburg, W., Dirksen, C., van Asselt, T., et al. (2006). Outpatient psychotherapy for borderline personality disorder: Randomized trial of schema-focused therapy vs. transference-focused psychotherapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 649–658.
Goldberg, R. L., Mann, L. S., Wise, T. N., & Segall, E. A. (1985). Parental qualities as perceived by borderline personality disorders. Hillside Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 7, 134–140.
Goodman, M., & Yehuda, R. (2002). The relationship between psychological trauma and borderline personality disorder. Psychiatric Annals, 32(6), 337–345. Grilo, C. M., Shea, M. T., Sanislow, C. A., Skodol, A. E., Gunderson, J. G., Stout, R. L., et al. (2004). Two-year stability and change of schizotypal, borderline, avoidant, and obsessive-compulsive personality disorders. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 72(5), 767–775.
Grinker, R. R., Werble, B., & Drye, R. (1968). The borderline syndrome: A behav-
ioral study of ego functions. New York: Basic Books.
Gross, R., Olfson, M., Gameroff, M., Shea, S., Feder, A., Fuentes, M., et al. (2002).
Borderline personality disorder in primary care. Archives of Internal Medicine,
162, 53–60.
Gunderson, J. G. (2001). Borderline personality disorder: A clinical guide.
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.
Gunderson, J. G., Kerr, J., & Englund, D. W. (1980). The families of borderlines: A
  
comparative study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 37, 27–33.
194
PERSONALITY DISORDERS
 
Gunderson, J. G., & Kolb, J. E. (1978). Discriminating features of borderline patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 135, 792–796.
Gunderson, J. G., Kolb, J. E., & Austin, V. (1981). The diagnostic interview for bor- derline patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 138, 896–903.
Gunderson, J. G., & Phillips, K. A. (1991). A current view of the interface between borderline personality disorder and depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148(8), 967–975.
Gunderson, J. G., & Sabo, A. N. (1993). “Borderline personality disorder and PTSD”: Reply. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150(12), 1906–1907.
Gunderson, J. G., & Singer, M. T. (1975). Defining borderline patients: An overview. American Journal of Psychiatry, 132, 1–10.
Gunderson, J. G., Zanarini, M. C., & Kisiel, C. (1991). Borderline personality dis- order: A review of data on DSM-III-R descriptions. Journal of Personality Disorders, 5, 340–352.
Gunderson, J. G., Zanarini, M. C., & Kisiel, C. (1995). Borderline personality dis- order. In W. J. Livesley (Ed.), The DSM-IV personality disorders (pp. 141–157). New York: Guilford Press.
Heim, C., Meinlschmidt, G., & Nemeroff, C. B. (2003). Neurobiology of early-life stress. Psychiatric Annals, 33, 18–26.
Herman, J. L. (1992). Complex PTSD: A syndrome in survivors of prolonged and repeated trauma. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 5, 341–377.
Herman, J. L., Perry, J., & van der Kolk, B. A. (1989). Childhood trauma in bor- derline personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146(4), 490–495. Herpertz, S. C., Dietrich, T. M., Wenning, B., Krings, T., Erberich, S. G., Willmes, K., et al. (2001). Evidence of abnormal amygdala functioning in borderline per- sonality disorder: A functional MRI study. Biological Psychiatry, 50(4),
292–298.
Isometsa, E. T., Henriksson, M. M., Heikkinen, M. E., Aro, H. M., Marttunen,
M. J., Kuoppasalmi, K. I., et al. (1996). Suicide among subjects with personal-
ity disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153(5), 667–673.
Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Brown, J., Smailes, E., & Bernstein, D. P. (1999). Childhood maltreatment increases risk for personality disorders during early
adulthood. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56(7), 600–606.
Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Smailes, E., Skodol, A. E., Brown, J., & Oldham, J. M. (2001). Childhood verbal abuse and risk for personality disorders during ado-
lescence and early adulthood. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 42, 16–23.
Judd, P. H., & McGlashan, T. (2003). A developmental model of borderline per- sonality disorder: Understanding variations in course and outcome. Arlington,
VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.
Kaplan, C. J. (1987). Children’s responses to the loss of a parent: The interaction
between the family and the intrapsychic mourning process. Unpublished manu-
script, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.
Kernberg, O. (1967). Borderline personality organization. Journal of the American
Psychoanalytic Association, 15, 641–685.
Kernberg, O. (1975). Borderline conditions and pathological narcissism. Northvale,
NJ: Jason Aronson.
Klein, D. N. (2003). Patients’ versus informants’ reports of personality disorders in
predicting 71⁄2-year outcome in outpatients with depressive disorders. Psychological Assessment, 15, 216–222.
 
Borderline Personality Disorder 195

Klonsky, E. D., Oltmanns, T. F., & Turkheimer, E. (2002). Informant-reports of per- sonality disorder: Relation to self-reports and future research directions. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 9, 300–311.
Knight, R. P. (1953). Borderline states. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 17, 1–12. Knight, R. P. (1954). Psychoanalytic psychiatry and psychology. In clinical and the-
oretical papers from the Austen Riggs Center, Vol.1.
Kohut, H. (1977). The restoration of the self. Madison, CT: International
Universities Press.
Koons, C. R., Robins, C. J., Tweed, J., Lynch, T. R., Gonzalez, A. M., Morse, J. Q.,
et al. (2001). Efficacy of dialectical behavior therapy in women veterans with
borderline personality disorder. Behavior Therapy, 32(2), 371–390. Lenzenweger, M. F., Clarkin, J. F., Fertuck, E. A., & Kernberg, O. F. (2004). Executive neurocognitive functioning and neurobehavioral systems indicators in borderline personality disorder: A preliminary study. Journal of Personality
Disorders, 18(5), 421–438.
Lenzenweger, M. F., Johnson, M. D., & Willett, J. B. (2004). Individual growth
curve analysis illuminates stability and change in personality disorder features: The longitudinal study of personality disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(10), 1015–1024.
Leyton, M., Okazawa, H., Diksic, M., Paris, J., Rosa, P., Mzengeza, S., et al. (2001). Brain regional alpha-[11C]methyl-L-tryptophan trapping in impulsive subjects with borderline personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(5), 775–782.
Linehan, M. M. (1993). Skills-training manual for treatment of borderline personal- ity disorder. New York: Guilford Press.
Linehan, M. M., Armstrong, H. E., Suarez, A., Allmon, D., & Heard, H. L. (1991). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of chronically parasuicidal borderline patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 1060–1064.
Linehan, M. M., Heard, H. L., & Armstrong, H. E. (1993). Naturalistic follow-up of a behavioral treatment for chronically parasuicidal borderline patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 971–974.
Linehan, M. M., Tutek, D., Heard, H., & Armstrong, H. (1994). Interpersonal out- come of cognitive-behavioral treatment for chronically suicidal borderline patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151(12), 1771–1776.
Links, P. S., Steiner, M., Offord, D. R., & Eppel, A. B. (1988). Characteristics of bor- derline personality disorder: A Canadian study. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 33, 336–340.
Ludolph, P., Westen, D., Misle, B., Jackson, A., Wixom, J., & Weiss, F. C. (1990). The borderline diagnosis in adolescents: Symptoms and developmental history. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 470–476.
Lynam, D. R., & Widiger, T. A. (2001). Using the five-factor model to represent the DSM-IV personality disorders: An expert consensus approach. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(3), 401–412.
Lyons-Ruth, K., & Jacobvitz, D. (1999). Attachment disorganization: Unresolved loss, relational violence, and lapses in behavioral and attentional strategies. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications (pp. 550–554). New York: Guilford Press.
Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(1–2), 66–104.
  
196
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Mann, J. (1998). The role of in vivo neurotransmitter system imaging studies in understanding major depression. Biological Psychiatry, 44(11), 1077–1078.
McGlashan, T. H. (1986). The Chestnut Lodge follow-up study: III. Long-term out- come of borderline personalities. Archives of General Psychiatry, 43, 20–30.
McLean, L. M., & Gallop, R. (2003). Implications of childhood sexual abuse for adult borderline personality disorder and complex posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 369–371.
Meehl, P. E. (1995). Bootstraps taxometrics: Solving the classification problem in psychopathology. American Psychologist, 50(4), 266–275.
Mellsop, G., Varghese, F., Joshua, S., & Hicks, A. (1982). The reliability and Axis II of DSM-III. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 1366–1367.
Nakash-Eisikovits, O., Dutra, L., & Westen, D. (2002). Relationship between attachment patterns and personality pathology in adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 1111–1123.
Nigg, J. T., & Goldsmith, H. (1994). Genetics of personality disorders: Perspectives from personality and psychopathology research. Psychological Bulletin, 115(3), 346–380.
Nigg, J. T., Lohr, N. E., Westen, D., Gold, L. J., & Silk, K. R. (1992). Malevolent object representations in borderline personality disorder and major depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 61–67.
Ogata, S. N., Silk, K. R., Goodrich, S., Lohr, N. E., Westen, D., & Hill, E. M. (1990). Childhood sexual and physical abuse in adult patients with borderline person- ality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 1008–1013.
Oldham, J. M. (2001). Integrated treatment planning for borderline personality dis- order. In J. Kay (Ed.), Integrated treatment of psychiatric disorders (Vol. 20, pp. 51–77). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.
Oldham, J. M., Skodol, A. E., Kellman, H. D., Hyler, S., Rosnick, L., & Davies, M. (1992). Diagnosis of DSM-III-R personality disorders by two semistructured interviews: Patterns of comorbidity. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 213–220.
Oltmanns, T. F., Melley, A. H., & Turkheimer, E. (2002). Impaired social function- ing and symptoms of personality disorders assessed by peer and self-report in a nonclinical population. Journal of Personality Disorders, 16(5), 437–452.
Paris, J. (1997). Childhood trauma as an etiological factor in the personality disor- ders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 11(1), 34–49.
Paris, J. (2003). Personality disorders over time: Precursors, course, and outcome. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.
Paris, J., Nowlis, D., & Brown, R. (1988). Developmental factors in the outcome of borderline personality disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 29, 147–150.
Paris, J., & Zweig-Frank, H. (2001). The 27-year follow-up of patients with borderline personality disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 42(6), 482–487.
Pfohl, B., Blum, N., Zimmerman, M., & Stangl, D. (1989). Structured interview for DSM-III-R personality—revised (SIDP-R). Iowa City University of Iowa.
Pfohl, B., Coryell, W., Zimmerman, M., & Stangl, D. (1986). DSM-III personality disorders: Diagnostic overlap and internal consistency of the individual DSM-III criteria. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 27, 21–34.
Pilkonis, P. A., Heape, C. L., Proietti, J. M., Clark, S. W., McDavid, J. D., & Pitts, T. E. (1995). The reliability and validity of two structured diagnostic interviews for personality disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52, 1025–1033.
  
Borderline Personality Disorder 197

Pilkonis, P. A., Heape, C. L., Ruddy, J., & Serrao, P. (1991). Validity in the diagno- sis of personality disorders: The use of the LEAD standard. Psychological Assessment, 31, 46–54.
Plomin, R., Chipuer, H. M., & Loehlin, J. C. (1990). Behavioral genetics and per- sonality. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 225–243). New York: Guilford Press.
Reich, J., Boerstler, H., Yates, W., & Nduaguba, M. (1989). Utilization of medical resources in persons with DSM-III personality disorders in a community sample. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 19(1), 1–9.
Robins, C. J., & Chapman, A. L. (2004). Dialectical behavior therapy: Current status, recent developments, and future directions. Journal of Personality Disorders, 18(1), 73–89.
Rothschild, L., Cleland, C., Haslam, N., & Zimmerman, M. (2003). A taxometric study of borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(4), 657–666.
Ryle, A. (2004). The contribution of cognitive analytic therapy to the treatment of borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 18, 3–35. Salzman, J. P., Salzman, C., & Wolfson, A. N. (1997). Relationship of childhood
abuse and maternal attachment to the development of borderline personality disorder. In M. C. Zanarini (Ed.), Role of sexual abuse in the etiology of bor- derline personality disorder (pp. 71–91). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Samuels, J., Eaton, W. W., Bienvenu, J., Clayton, P., Brown, H., Costa, P. T., Jr., et al. (2002). Prevalence and correlates of personality disorders in a community sample. British Journal of Psychiatry 180, 536–542.
Satorius, N., Kaelber, C., Cooper, J. E., Roper, M. T., Rae, D. S., Gulbinat, W., et al. (1993). Progress toward achieving a common language in psychiatry. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 115–124.
Scheel, K. R. (2000). The empirical basis of dialectical behavior therapy: Summary, critique, and implications. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 7(1), 68–86.
Schmahl, C. G., Elzinga, B. M., Ebner, U. W., Simms, T., Sanislow, C., Vermetten, E., et al. (2004). Psychophysiological reactivity to traumatic and abandonment scripts in borderline personality and posttraumatic stress disorders: A preliminary report. Psychiatry Research, 126(1), 33–42.
Schmahl, C. G., Vermetten, E., Elzinga, B. M., & Bremner, J. (2003). Magnetic res- onance imaging of hippocampal and amygdala volume in women with child- hood abuse and borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 122(3), 193–198.
Shedler, J., & Westen, D. (2004). Dimensions of personality pathology: An alterna- tive to the five-factor model. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 1743–1754. Siever, L., & Davis, K. (1991). A psychobiological perspective on the personality dis-
orders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148(12), 1647–1658.
Silk, K. R., Lee, S., Hill, E. M., & Lohr, N. E. (1995). Borderline personality disor- der symptoms and severity of sexual abuse. American Journal of Psychiatry,
152, 1059–1064.
Silk, K. R., Lohr, N. E., Ogata, S. N., & Westen, D. (1990). Borderline inpatients
with affective disorder: Preliminary follow-up data. Journal of Personality Disorders, 4(2), 213–224.
  
198
PERSONALITY DISORDERS
 
Skodol, A. E., Gunderson, J. G., McGlashan, T. H., Dyck, I. R., Stout, R. L., Bender, D. S., et al. (2002). Functional impairment in patients with schizotypal, border- line, avoidant, or obsessive-compulsive personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 276–283.
Skodol, A. E., Gunderson, J. G., Pfohl, B., Widiger, T. A., Livesley, W. J., & Siever, L. (2002). The borderline diagnosis: I. Psychopathology, comorbidity, and per- sonality structures. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 936–950.
Skodol, A. E., Gunderson, J. G., Shea, M., McGlashan, T. H., Morey, L. C., Sanislow, C. A., et al. (2005). The Collaborative Longitudinal Personality Disorders Study (CLPS): Overview and implications. Journal of Personality Disorders, 19(5), 487–504.
Skodol, A. E., Oldham, J. M., Rosnick, L., Kellman, D., & Hyler, S. (1991). Diagnosis of DSM-III-R personality disorders: A comparison of two structured interviews. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 1, 13–26.
Sokal, R. R. (1974). Classification: Purposes, principles, progress, prospects. Science, 185(4157), 1115–1123.
Soloff, P. H., Lynch, K. G., Kelly, T. M., Malone, K. M., & Mann, J. J. (2000). Characteristics of suicide attempts of patients with major depressive episode and borderline personality disorder: A comparative study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 601–608.
Soloff, P. H., Meltzer, C. C., Greer, P. J., Constantine, D., & Kelly, T. M. (2000). A fenfluramine-activated FDG-PET study of borderline personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 47(6), 540–547.
Soloff, P. H., & Millward, J. W. (1983). Developmental histories of borderline patients. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 24, 574–588.
Solomon, J., George, C., & De Jong, A. (1995). Children classified as controlling at age six: Evidence of disorganized representational strategies and aggression at home and at school. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 447–463.
Spitzer, R. L., Endicott, J., & Gibbon, M. (1979). Crossing the border into border- line personality and borderline schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 36, 17–24.
Stern, A. (1938). Psychoanalytic investigation and therapy in the borderline group of neuroses. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 467–489.
Stone, M. H. (1987). Psychotherapy of borderline patients in light of long-term fol- low-up. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 51, 231–247.
Stone, M. H. (1992). Borderline personality disorder: Course of illness. In J. F. Clarkin, E. Marziali, & H. Munroe-Blum (Eds.), Borderline personality disor- der: Clinical and empirical perspectives. New York: Guilford Press.
Stone, M. H. (1993). Long-term outcome in personality disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 299–313.
Stone, M. H. (1994). Characterologic subtypes of the borderline personality disor- der: With a note on prognostic factors. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 17, 773–784.
Stuart, S., Pfohl, B., Battaglia, M., Bellodi, L., Grove, W., & Cadoret, R. (1998). The co-occurrence of DSM-III-R personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 12, 302–315.
Thomas, C., Turkheimer, E., & Oltmanns, T. F. (2003). Factorial structure of patho- logical personality as evaluated by peers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 81–91.
 
Borderline Personality Disorder 199

Torgersen, S. (1980). The oral, obsessive, and hysterical personality syndromes: A study of hereditary and environmental factors by means of the twin method. Archives of General Psychiatry, 37(11), 1272–1277.
Torgersen, S., Kringlen, E., & Cramer, V. (2001). The prevalence of personality disorders in a community sample. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 590–596. Torgersen, S., Lygren, S., Oien, P. A., Skre, I., Onstad, S., Edvardsen, J., et al. (2000). A twin study of personality disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 41(6),
416–425.
Trull, T. J. (2001). Structural relations between borderline personality disorder
features and putative etiological correlates. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
110(3), 471–481.
Trull, T. J., Widiger, T. A., Lynam, D. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (2003). Borderline per-
sonality disorder from the perspective of general personality functioning.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology 112(2), 193–202.
Tyrer, P. J. (1996). Establishing the severity of personality disorder. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 153(12), 1593–1597.
Verheul, R., van den Bosch, L. M. C., Koeter, M. W. J., De Ridder, M. A. J., Stinjnen,
T., & van den Brink, W. (2003). Dialectical behaviour therapy for women with borderline personality disorder: 12-month, randomised clinical trial in the Netherlands. British Journal of Psychiatry, 182, 135–140.
Weaver, T. L., & Clum, G. A. (1993). Early family environments and traumatic experiences associated with borderline personality disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61(6), 1068–1075.
Westen, D. (1991a). Cognitive-behavioral interventions in the psychoanalytic psy- chotherapy of borderline personality disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 211–230.
Westen, D. (1991b). Social cognition and object relations. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 429–455.
Westen, D. (1997). Divergences between clinical and research methods for assessing personality disorders: Implications for research and the evolution of Axis II. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 895–903.
Westen, D. (2000). Integrative psychotherapy: Integrating psychodynamic and cognitive-behavioral theory and technique. In C. R. Snyder & R. Ingram (Eds.), Handbook of psychological change: Psychotherapy processes and practices for the 21st century (pp. 217–242). New York: Wiley.
Westen, D., & Bradley, R. (2005). Prototype diagnosis of personality. In S. Strack (Ed.), Handbook of personology and psychopathology (pp. 238–256). New York: Wiley.
Westen, D., Bradley, R., & Shedler, J. (2005). Refining the borderline construct: Diagnostic criteria and trait structure. Unpublished manuscript, Emory University, Atlanta, GA.
Westen, D., Dutra, L., & Shedler, J. (2005). Assessing adolescent personality pathol- ogy: Quantifying clinical judgment. British Journal of Psychiatry, 186, 227–238. Westen, D., Lohr, N., Silk, K. R., Gold, L., & Kerber, K. (1990). Object relations and social cognition in borderlines, major depressives, and normals: A thematic
apperception test analysis. Psychological Assessment, 2, 355–364.
Westen, D., Ludolph, P., Block, M. J., Wixom, J., & Wiss, F. C. (1990). Developmental history and object relations in psychiatrically disturbed adoles-
  
cent girls. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 1061–1068.
200
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Westen, D., Ludolph, P., Misle, B., Ruffins, S., & Block, J. (1990). Physical and sex- ual abuse in adolescent girls with borderline personality disorder. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 60, 55–66.
Westen, D., & Muderrisoglu, S. (2003). Reliability and validity of personality disor- der assessment using a systematic clinical interview: Evaluating an alternative to structured interviews. Journal of Personality Disorders, 17, 350–368.
Westen, D., & Muderrisoglu, S. (2006). Clinical assessment of pathological person- ality traits. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 1285–1287.
Westen, D., Muderrisoglu, S., Fowler, C., Shedler, J., & Koren, D. (1997). Affect reg- ulation and affective experience: Individual differences, group differences, and measurement using a Q-sort procedure. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 429–439.
Westen, D., & Shedler, J. (1999a). Revising and assessing Axis II: 1. Developing a clinically and empirically valid assessment method. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 258–272.
Westen, D., & Shedler, J. (1999b). Revising and assessing Axis II: 2. Toward an empirically based and clinically useful classification of personality disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 273–285.
Westen, D., & Shedler, J. (2000). A prototype matching approach to diagnosing per- sonality disorders toward DSM-V. Journal of Personality Disorders, 14, 109–126.
Westen, D., Shedler, J., & Bradley, R. (2006). A prototype approach to personality diagnosis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 838–848.
Westen, D., Shedler, J., Durrett, C., Glass, S., & Martens, A. (2003). Personality diagnosis in adolescence: DSM-IV Axis II diagnoses and an empirically derived alternative. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 952–966.
Westen, D., Thomas, C., Nakash, O., & Bradley, R. (2006). Clinical assessment of attachment patterns and personality disorder in adolescents and adults. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 1065–1085.
White, C. N., Gunderson, J. G., Zanarini, M. C., & Hudson, J. I. (2003). Family studies of borderline personality disorder: A review. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 11(1), 8–19.
Widiger, T. A. (1995). Deletion of self-defeating and sadistic personality disorders. In W. J. Livesley (Ed.), The DSM-IV personality disorders: Diagnosis and treat- ment of mental disorders (pp. 359–373). New York: Guilford Press.
Yen, S., Shea, M. T., Battle, C. L., Johnson, D. M., Zlotnick, C., Dolan-Sewell, R., et al. (2002). Traumatic exposure and posttraumatic stress disorder in border- line, schizotypal, avoidant, and obsessive-compulsive personality disorders: Findings from the collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 190, 510–518.
Yeomans, F. E. (2004). Transference-focused psychotherapy in borderline personal- ity disorder. Psychiatric Annals, 34, 449–454.
Young, J. E., Klosko, J. S., & Weishaar, M. (2003). Schema therapy: A practitioner’s guide. New York: Guilford Press.
Zanarini, M. C. (1997). Role of sexual abuse in the etiology of borderline personal- ity disorder. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.
Zanarini, M. C., & Frankenburg, F. R. (1997). Pathways to the development of bor- derline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 11(1), 93–104.
Borderline Personality Disorder 201

Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., DeLuca, C. J., Hennen, J., Khera, G. S., & Gunderson, J. G. (1998). The pain of being borderline: Dysphoric states specific to borderline personality disorder. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 6, 201–207.
Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Hennen, J., & Silk, K. R. (2003). The longitu- dinal course of borderline psychopathology: 6-year prospective follow-up of the phenomenology of borderline personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(2), 274–283.
Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Hennen, J., & Silk, K. R. (2004). Mental health service utilization by borderline personality disorder patients and Axis II com- parison subjects followed prospectively for 6 years. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 65(1), 28–36.
Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Vujanovic, A. A., Hennen, J., Reich, D. B., & Silk, K. R. (2004). Axis II comorbidity of borderline personality disorder: Description of 6-year course and prediction to time-to-remission. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 110(6), 416–420.
Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Yong, L., Raviola, G., Reich, D. B., Hennen, J., et al. (2004). Borderline psychopathology in the first-degree relatives of bor- derline and Axis II comparison probands. Journal of Personality Disorders, 18, 449–447.
Zanarini, M. C., Gunderson, J. G., Marino, M. F., Schwartz, E. O., & Frankenburg, F. R. (1989). Childhood experiences of borderline patients. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 30, 18–25.
Zanarini, M. C., Yong, L., Frankenburg, F. R., Hennen, J., Reich, D., Marino, M. F., et al. (2002). Severity of reported childhood sexual abuse and its relationship to severity of borderline psychopathology and psychosocial impairment among borderline inpatients. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 190(6), 381–387.
Zelkowitz, P., Paris, J., Guzder, J., & Feldman, R. (2001). Diathesis and stressors in borderline pathology of childhood: The role of neuropsychological risk and trauma. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(1), 100–105.
Zimmerman, M., & Mattia, J. I. (1999). Axis I diagnostic comorbidity and border- line personality disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 40(4), 245–252.
Zittel, C., & Westen, D. (1998). Conceptual issues and research findings on border- line personality disorder: What every clinician should know. In Session, 4, 5–20. Zittel, C., & Westen, D. (2002). Subtyping borderline personality disorder. Presentation given at New Directions in Borderline Personality Disorder II, a conference jointly sponsored by the National Institutes of Mental Health and the
Borderline Personality Disorder Research Foundation, New York.

Histrionic 8 Personality Disorder
Pavel S. Blagov Emory University
Katherine A. Fowler Emory University
Scott O. Lilienfeld Emory University

Histrionic personality disorder (HPD) is familiar to clinicians who attend to personality pathology and to literature devotees who ponder the nature of superficially dramatic, manipulative, and insatiably attention-seeking characters such as Blanche DuBois in Tennessee Williams’s play A Streetcar Named Desire. Millon, Grossman, Millon, Meagher, and Ramnath (2004) provided the following description of HPD across nine clin- ical domains: behavioral acts are dramatic, interpersonal conduct is atten- tion seeking, cognitive style is flighty, self-image is gregarious, representations of others are shallow, regulatory mechanisms rely on dissociation, the psychic structure is disjointed, and the mood and temperament are fickle. Despite its familiar feel, HPD remains enshrouded in a degree of historical confusion and conceptual uncertainty that researchers have not yet resolved. In this chapter, we track some of the theoretical and empirical efforts in clarifying the nature of HPD, summarize the available literature on treatment recom- mendations for the disorder, and propose directions for future research.
203
204 PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Current Definition __________________________________
The current edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2000) defines HPD primarily as a pervasive pattern of excessive emotionality and attention seeking that begins in early adulthood and presents across situa- tions. Individuals may receive the diagnosis (code 301.50) if they meet five or more of eight criteria (APA, 2000, p. 714):
(1) is uncomfortable in situations in which he or she is not the center of attention
(2) interaction with others is often characterized by inappropriate sex- ually seductive or provocative behavior
(3) displays rapidly shifting and shallow expression of emotions (4) consistently uses physical appearance to draw attention to self
(5) has a style of speech that is excessively impressionistic and lacking in detail
(6) shows self-dramatization, theatricality, and exaggerated expression of emotion
(7) is suggestible, i.e., easily influenced by others or circumstances (8) considers relationships to be more intimate than they actually are
Thus a person with HPD may behave theatrically, dress provocatively, make up stories, engage in flattery and flirtation, complain of dramatic ills, throw a tantrum, or make a suicidal gesture all to draw attention to the self. The individual may fail to focus attention on the self, in which case the associated features of manipulativeness, inappropriate seductiveness, and dependency may become apparent. These features, along with a cognitive/ emotional style characterized by shallow investment, excitement seeking, and shifting interests may thwart the person’s relationships, thus depriving him or her from much-sought attention and leading to distress and depres- sion. The DSM-IV estimates a 2% to 3% prevalence in the general popula- tion and a 10% to 15% prevalence in mental health settings (see Blashfield & Davis, 1993, for a higher estimate of 24%). HPD prevalence appears to be equal in men and women (Nestadt, Romanoski, Chalel, & Marchant, 1990) in nonclinical settings, even though in clinical ones more women received the diagnosis than did men. Means of attention seeking and dra- matic expressiveness may vary across culture, gender, and age. Because of its hypothesized pervasive nature and resistance to change, the disorder is coded on Axis II (Cluster B).
  
Histrionic Personality Disorder 205

____________________________________ Historical Roots
Hysteria
HPD shares a divergent history with the Axis I somatoform conditions called conversion disorder (a sensory or motor deficit due to psychological causes that mimics a neurological disorder, code 300.11) and somatization disorder (recurring and clinically significant somatic complaints other than pain not fully explained by a general medical condition, code 300.81). Historically, they are all linked to the ancient concept of hysteria, or “wan- dering womb.” Such authors as Hippocrates and Plato attributed conversion symptoms and emotional outbursts in women to a displaced uterus caused by sexual discontent, reflecting Greek society’s derogatory view of women as irrational creatures of lust. Similarly, Christian asceticism during the Middle Ages and the Reformation blamed women’s mental illness on involvement in witchery due to sexual hunger and moral weakness in resist- ing Satanic temptation. During the Enlightenment and through the 19th cen- tury, medicine struggled to divorce itself from theological doctrines of possession. Explanations of hysteria shifted to a constitutional weakness of women’s nervous system caused by their biological sex. Thus, whether physical or spiritual, and despite the fact that hysteria was diagnosed with increasing frequency in men, the construct tended to reflect the predominant sociocultural stereotype of women as inherently vulnerable, inferior, and emotionally uncontrolled. The extent to which the current criteria for HPD reflect gender bias remains the subject of a controversy today (see below).
Conversion Hysteria and Hysterical Personality
The term hysteria has been used to denote a variety of conditions ranging from extreme reactions to stress to isolated conversion symptoms, somatiza- tion, immaturity, a personality disorder (PD), or a personality trait (Easser & Lesser, 1965; Lazare, 1971; Pfohl, 1991). The differentiation between conver- sion hysteria and hysterical personality began with psychoanalytic literature on character as well as with the writings of Kraepelin, Schneider, and Kretschmer (see Bornstein, 1999). Freud’s famed case work in this area (e.g., Freud & Breuer, 1895/2000) dealt primarily with conversion hysteria (e.g., the neuro- logically inexplicable loss of sensation in part of the body), which he explained as a neurotic compromise among sexual urges and internalized societal prohi- bitions. Psychoanalytic authors sought the origins of hysteria in the psycho- sexual development of the child (e.g., Abraham, 1927/1948; Fenichel, 1945; Marmor, 1953) and speculated about the importance of character (or person- ality) to the formation of symptoms. Reich (1933, 1949) drew attention to hysteria as a set of personality characteristics and differentiated conversion hysteria as a transient functional disorder from hysterical character (see Baumbacher & Amini, 1980–1981; Shapiro, 1965). The early analytic con- ceptualizations of both kinds of hysteria carried notions of women’s deficiency
   
206
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

due to penis envy and feelings of castration. In this way, they paralleled the antiwoman sentiment seen throughout the history of hysteria (Chodoff, 1982). Current psychoanalytic thought regarding histrionic personality has evolved considerably, and will be discussed in the section on psychotherapy.
The concept of hysterical personality was well developed by the middle of the 20th century (Alam & Merskey, 1992) and strongly resembled the current definition of HPD. DSM-I (APA, 1952) featured a symptom-based (neurotic) category, “hysteria” (conversion), and a personality-based category, “emo- tionally unstable personality.” DSM-II (APA, 1968) distinguished between hysterical neurosis (conversion reaction and dissociative reaction) and hysteri- cal (parenthetically, histrionic) personality. DSM-I and DSM-II received much criticism for their poor psychometric properties (Nathan, 1998). Nevertheless, they contributed to clarifying the distinction between conversion symptoms and hysterical personality, a concept that later found support in early empiri- cal studies (e.g., Luisada, Peele, & Pittard, 1974).
Somatization and Briquet’s Syndrome
Hysterical personality underwent further differentiation as findings emerged that, despite some overlap between hysterical personality and som- atization, the two were not the same (Pollak, 1981). For example, in a sur- vey of 91 psychiatric residents and faculty, Slavney (1978) found that all considered self-dramatization, followed by attention seeking, emotional instability, and seductiveness, to be the most diagnostically important and reliably recognized features of hysterical personality. These participants ranked conversion symptoms among the least important and reliable symp- toms. The notion that hysterical personality features covaried with somati- zation symptoms persisted for some time in the diagnostic entity of Briquet’s syndrome. Difficulties in distinguishing patients with isolated somatization features from patients with the more severe and pervasive Briquet’s syn- drome (e.g., Cloninger, Martin, Guze, & Clayton, 1986; Liskow, Clayton, Woodruff, Guze, & Cloninger, 1977) contributed to abandonment of the notions of hysteria and Briquet’s syndrome and to the emergence of somati- zation disorder in DSM-III (APA, 1980).
HPD may not be as strongly associated with somatization disorder (SD) as might be expected from their shared historical roots. Estimates of HPD preva- lence in individuals with SD have varied from 7.4% to 81.8% across samples (Rost, Akins, Brown, & Smith, 1992). The heterogeneity in these estimates is probably attributable to differences in both criteria used to operationalize SD and the characteristics of each sample. Using a structured interview (the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R Personality Disorders [SCID-II; Spitzer, Williams, & Gibbon, 1987]) with 94 patients with SD recruited from primary practices in Arkansas, Rost and her colleagues found prevalence rates of 12.8% for full-criteria HPD and 10.6% for subthreshold HPD (com- pared with an HPD prevalence of less than 5% in the general medical popu- lation). However, 61% of the patients had a co-occurring PD, the most
  
Histrionic Personality Disorder 207

prevalent PDs being avoidant, paranoid, self-defeating, and obsessive- compulsive. The authors speculated that these latter disorders may predispose patients to SD due to an overall discomfort with feelings, inability to view oneself as deserving of good health, or concerns over becoming sick. These PDs may go undocumented in patients with SD because they often do not prompt clinicians in general medical settings to consider a consultation with a mental health professional. On the other hand, patients with histrionic, antisocial, and borderline PDs, due to their interpersonally disruptive out- bursts, are more likely to be referred for a psychiatric evaluation, and hence these disorders’ co-occurrence with SD relative to that of other PDs may have been overestimated in older studies.
Histrionic Personality in DSM-III and DSM-III-R
In DSM-III (APA, 1980), the term ”hysterical personality” was changed to “histrionic personality” to emphasize the histrionic (derived from the Latin word histrio, or actor) behavior pattern and to reduce the confusion caused by the historical links of the term hysteria to conversion symptoms (Chodoff, 1974; Spitzer, Williams, & Skodol, 1980). Some DSM-III criteria dropped out of the DSM-III-R (APA, 1987; i.e., craving for activity and excitement; irrational, angry outbursts or tantrums; and proneness to manipulative suici- dal attempts) as a way of reducing the diagnostic overlap with other condi- tions, particularly borderline PD. Criteria 2 (inappropriate seductiveness) and 8 (impressionistic speech) in the DSM-III-R, in contrast, had been absent in the DSM-III and represent a return to the historical roots of the disorder.
Livesley and Schroeder (1991) examined the factorial structure of dimen- sional self-report ratings of PD symptoms in a sample of 274 heterogeneous volunteers and 133 patients with a primary diagnosis of PD and no major DSM-III-R Axis I diagnosis. Participants rated themselves on a five-point scale along symptoms of antisocial, borderline, histrionic, and narcissistic PDs as defined by the consensual judgment of a sample of psychiatrists familiar with the literature. The solution for HPD yielded four factors, two that appeared to overlap with other disorders (an exploitativeness factor sim- ilar to one seen in BPD, and a dependency factor similar to the DSM- III-R construct of dependent PD) and two that appeared specific to HPD: one representing the hysterical cognitive style and one related to dramatization.
According to Pfohl (1991), all criteria but 1 (constant demands for reas- surance and praise) and 7 (self-centeredness and low frustration tolerance) for HPD in the DSM-III-R had good sensitivity coefficients and contributed to the overall internal consistency of the diagnosis.
_____________________________ External Validity of HPD
Pollak (1981) and Pfohl (1991) summarized research findings on HPD and concluded that sufficient evidence had accrued for the disorder’s external
  
208
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

validity. One concern, however, is that many of the older findings may be based on operationalizations of HPD that are not fully consistent with the current definition. Furthermore, many of the findings have not been suffi- ciently replicated. Unlike other PDs such as antisocial and borderline, we know little about the relation of HPD, particularly in the general population, to some of the most theoretically and clinically relevant external criteria. For example, we know little about its links to relationship satisfaction and sta- bility, employment history, general health, and life satisfaction. Below we summarize some of the more recent findings that modestly build upon the reviews by Pollak (1981) and Pfohl (1991).
Projective Testing and Defense Style ___________________
Blais, Hilsenroth, and Fowler (1998) correlated DSM-IV symptom counts for HPD, as well as borderline, antisocial, and narcissistic PDs, with Rorschach variables in an archival study of 79 records of PD patients. Consistent with previous literature, only HPD symptoms correlated with the FC + CF + C and T indices from the Exner system (rs = .35 and .30, p < .01) with controlling for the number of responses (F denotes “form” responses, C denotes “color” responses, and T denotes “texture” responses). The FC + CF + C variable measures the number of responses based primarily on color features of the blots and has been hypothesized to reflect emotionality, whereas T reflects the use of texture by the respondent and may be indicative of loneliness and inter- personal neediness. FC + CF + C tended to correlate with ratings of the DSM-IV symptoms of seductiveness, shallow emotion, impressionistic speech, and focus on appearance, whereas T was linked to attention seeking, self-dramatization, misconstruing of the intimacy of relationships, and impressionistic speech. In addition, the attention seeking symptoms and scale 3 (Hysteria) of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory–2 (MMPI-2) were linked to Lerner’s denial (DEN) index for the Rorschach.
In Cramer’s (1999) study of 91 adults of age 23 (45 men and 46 women), ratings of denial and projection based on the Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) correlated with the extent to which participants’ California Q-Set pro- files matched expert prototypes of borderline, psychopathic, narcissistic, and histrionic pathology. When defenses were coded as mature versus immature, immature denial and identification predicted histrionic features, with imma- ture identification predicting histrionic pathology but not features of the other three disorders. The findings contradicted the proposition that histri- onic features would be linked to maturity more so than would those of other PDs. Instead, like other kinds of personality dysfunction, histrionic personal- ity exhibited a continuum of severity such that the more histrionic the person, the more immature his or her defense style. The nonclinical nature of the sam- ple limits further interpretation. Nevertheless, it should be noted that the evi- dence linking C responses to emotionality is at best equivocal (Frank, 1990).
  
Histrionic Personality Disorder 209

Neurocognitive Findings
The literature on neurocognitive and other biological markers of HPD is limited. Shapiro (1965) proposed that a global cognitive-perceptual style predisposes people to a hysterical personality, and Millon (1981) suggested that hysterical individuals lack cognitive-perceptual integration. Tests of derivative hypotheses have generally not found links between measures of histrionicity and perceptual variables (see Cale & Lilienfeld, 2002; Maynard & Meyer, 1996; Yovel, Revelle, & Mineka, 2005). In comparison with people with obsessive-compulsive tendencies, participants with histrionic personalities appear to be less rigid in their cognitive style but not necessar- ily inattentive to detail or distractible (Sacco & Olczak, 1996).
Biological Findings
Svanborg, Evenden-Matilla, Gustavsson, Uvnas-Moberg, and Asberg (2000) recruited 99 participants who were either beginning or undergoing nonpharmaco- logicaltreatmentformood(n =37)oranxiety(n=29)disorders.Theparticipants completed personality self-report questionnaires and a modified SCID-II screening questionnaire (Ekselius, Lindstrom, von Knorring, Bodlund, & Kullgren, 1994; Spitzer et al., 1987). There were no significant associations between symptom and personality measures and plasma levels of insulin or glucagon. Low fasting plasma glucose levels were associated with low Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF) scores, high scores on an impulsivity questionnaire, and a high number of self- assessed histrionic and narcissistic traits in men. In women, the relationship between glucose level and histrionic traits was also significant but positive. In light of previous research linking diminished glucose levels to cognitive impairment (Taylor & Rachman, 1988), social-emotional distress (Messer, Morris, & Gross, 1990), and antisocial behavior (Virkkunen, 1986), Svanborg and his colleagues (2000) suggested that the relationship between histrionicity and low glucose in men may indicate that the traits are maladaptive for men. The reverse relationship in women may suggest that for them such traits are linked to better adaptation. The conclusions remain speculative and call for attempts to replicate these studies.
Discriminant Validity, ______________________ Co-occurrence, and Comorbidity
When a PD such as HPD is diagnosed with other DSM disorders in the same individuals, it may be difficult to judge whether the issue at stake is poor dis- criminant validity of the diagnostic criteria (e.g., diagnostic overlap), true comor- bidity, or co-occurrence (Lilienfeld, Waldman, & Israel, 1994). This is especially true of HPD, due to the limited amount of research literature and variability and inconsistency in the use of assessment methods and diagnostic criteria.
  
210
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Borderline and Narcissistic Personality Disorders
In the 1980s, researchers observed substantial overlap between borderline PD and other PDs (e.g., Widiger & Rogers, 1989). In a review of studies con- ducted from 1983 to 1990, Grueneich (1992) found that HPD was the PD most frequently associated with borderline personality disorder (BPD), with a median reliability of .44 for four studies using DSM-III criteria. Grueneich noted that the association between HPD and BPD appeared to be lower in studies using DSM-III-R criteria, probably due to the removal of two overlapping items: angry outbursts and manipulative suicidal gestures. In studies of emotional regulation and identity disturbance, Westen (see Westen & Heim, 2003; Wilkinson-Ryan & Westen, 2000) reported a subtype of BPD with salient histrionic features. Thus, the links between HPD and BPD are likely to be revis- ited in the future. Criterion discrimination between HPD and narcissistic per- sonality disorder (NPD) is still poor in the DSM-IV (Blais & Norman, 1997).
Dependent Personality Disorder (DPD)
The notion that dependency characterizes HPD disappeared from the DSM under pressure to improve discriminant validity and to reduce the overlap between HPD and DPD. Nevertheless, the psychodynamic notion that under- lying dependency motivates attention seeking in HPD has not been ruled out. In a sample of 491 psychology students, Bornstein (1998) found that those who met criteria for HPD and DPD on the Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire— Revised (PDQ-R; Hyler, Skodol, Kellman, Oldham, & Rosnick, 1990) had comparable scores on an implicit dependency index derived from the Rorschach. Furthermore, students with HPD and DPD scored higher on this measure than did students with other PDs or no PDs. The dependency scores on a self-report inventory did not differ among students with HPD, students with other PDs, and students with no PDs, whereas students with DPD scored higher than the other three groups on this explicit measure.
Bipolar Disorder
Even though a small body of literature reveals overlap between HPD and mania, the finding is limited to structured interviews that rely heavily on ret- rospective self-report (Strakowski, McElroy, Keck, & West, 1994) and may reflect method bias. Histrionic personality and hypomanic states may share impulsivity and extraversion. Indeed, Schotte, De Doncker, Maes, Cluydts, and Cosyns (1993) observed that high scores on scale 9 (Ma) and low scores on scale 0 (Si) of the MMPI were most indicative of DSM-III-R HPD in an inpatient Dutch sample (assessed using the SCID-II). Furthermore, the abil- ity of scales 9 and 0 as well as Morey’s HST Scale (Morey, Waugh, & Blashfield, 1985) to predict HPD could be derived from their assessment of introversion-extraversion. In addition, bipolar spectrum disorders and HPD
   
Histrionic Personality Disorder 211

share features of mood lability and emotional dysregulation, as Perugi et al. (1998) observed in a study of atypical depression. Within their sample of individuals with mood disorder and comorbid DSM-IV HPD, none met cri- teria for unipolar depression, whereas most met some criteria for bipolar II or cyclothymic disorder. Similarly, Westen and Shedler’s (1999) empirical prototype for HPD contains items describing emotional dysregulation (i.e., “unable to soothe or comfort self when distressed,” “emotions tend to spi- ral out of control,” “emotions tend to change rapidly and unpredictably,” and “tends to become irrational when strong emotions are stirred up”). The possibility that HPD and mania share an underlying cause or that HPD may predispose individuals to bipolar disorder requires further study.
Depression and Anxiety
HPD has been thought to make sufferers vulnerable to depression, anxiety, and parasuicidality due to its interference with obtainment of the relationships and attention such individuals so strongly desire. In a study of the relationship between PD symptomatology and Axis I disorders in an ethnically heteroge- neous community sample of adolescent girls (Daley et al., 1999), 20.31% of the patients, (n = 63) met cutoffs for HPD on the Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire (PDQ; Hyler, Reider, Spitzer, & Williams, 1982) for DSM-III or the PDQ-R (Hyler et al., 1990) for DSM-III-R. Overall PD scores predicted concurrent depression scores as well as depression scores at follow-up 2 years later. Cluster B PD symptoms added to predictive power. HPD’s relative con- tribution was unclear, but HPD was deemed present in 59% of the patients with a Cluster B disorder, who, in turn, comprised 34.38% of the sample. Thus, HPD may have been a factor in the development of depression (see also Crawford, Cohen, & Brook, 2001b) in light of the fact that the DSM-III and DSM-III-R criteria for HPD overlapped with those for BPD, which may be a better predictor of future Axis I symptoms than HPD. Using the SCID for the DSM-III-R, Ampollini and his colleagues (1999) selected 42 outpatients with panic disorder, 18 with major depression, 29 with both, and 48 healthy con- trols. Evaluations with the revised version of the Structured Interview for DSM-III-R Personality Disorders (SIDP-R; Pfohl, Blum, Zimmerman, & Stangl, 1989) by clinicians blind to Axis I diagnosis revealed that patients with panic disorder and patients with both panic disorder and major depression had significantly higher rates of HPD compared to controls or to patients with major depression alone. These and other studies support the notion that HPD may be comorbid with depressive and anxiety disorders.
____________________________________________ Etiology
Little is known about the etiology of HPD. The clinically intuitive belief that HPD may develop in children whose strong needs for attention were met
  
212
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

inconsistently by their parents is prevalent (Kraus & Reynolds, 2001), but it has not received rigorous tests. Coid (1999) examined the histories of 260 adults in forensic settings for specific risk factors related to DSM-III Axis II pathology and was unable to establish associations between HPD and devel- opmental adversity or temperamental factors. Below we summarize some of the positive findings.
Genetics
Research on genetic contributions specific to HPD is insufficient. According to Maier, Franke, and Hawellek (1998), evidence for genetic con- tributions to personality pathology has accrued overall, but the findings vary across different disorders. Cluster B disorders, in particular, have not emerged as highly heritable. Research on the genetics of neurochemical mechanisms governing the dopaminergic and serotonergic systems of the brain has sparked interest, but it requires further study and replication. Torgersen and his colleagues (2000) interviewed 221 adult twin pairs (92 monozygotic [MZ] and 129 dizygotic [DZ]) about their lifetime histories of mental disorders. Using data collected with the SCID-II (Spitzer et al., 1987), they compared the prevalence of HPD among MZ and DZ probands and their co-twins. HPD yielded a substantial heritability coefficient of .67 in an AE model (which includes additive genetic factors and excludes shared environment) and a .52 effect for shared environment in a CE model (which includes shared environ- ment and excludes genetic factors). Thus even though the study’s authors emphasized the genetic factors, their findings suggest that both genetic and environmental factors are influential in the development of HPD. The study was limited by the primarily Norwegian sample and the fact that MZ status may be confounded in the classic behavior genetics model (e.g., by evocative genotype-environment correlations). For example, the higher concordance of HPD in MZ twins may be due to their more similar treatment by adults and peers in childhood and adolescence based on their similar appearance.
Developmental Considerations
Clinicians have long theorized about the contribution of developmental events and family environment to the development of HPD, but the links remain speculative. In this section we review four studies that point to pos- sible developmental trajectories and developmental contributors to HPD.
Cohen and his colleagues (1994) compared 473 persons 55 years old or younger to 289 older individuals in a community sample, and they found that the two cohorts differed significantly on the prevalence of two DSM-III PDs: antisocial and histrionic. HPD prevalence was 4.3% in the younger and 2.2% in the older sample, consistent with the common view that PDs are observed less frequently in older populations because of either lesser prevalence or lesser
   
Histrionic Personality Disorder 213

severity. Possible explanations include cohort effects, age bias in diagnostic criteria, early mortality due to risky or impulsive behavior or suicide, social- emotional development due to learning, cortical maturation, and age-related neurochemical changes leading to decreased impulsivity or emotionality.
Baker, Capron, and Azorlosa (1996) screened undergraduates to find women who scored high on only the histrionic or the dependent scale of the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory (MCMI; Millon, 1982) and also met DSM-III-R criteria for HPD or DPD but no other PD (as assessed by the SCID-II). Thus, 15 young women with “clean” HPD, 15 with DPD, and 15 controls completed a Family Environment Scale with 10 dimensions. The groups were similar on family cohesion, expansiveness, conflict, active- recreational orientation, moral-religious emphasis, and organization. The HPD group was higher than the DPD group and similar to controls on inde- pendence and achievement orientation. Participants with HPD rated the intellectual-cultural orientation of their families higher than did those with DPD or the controls. Both women with HPD and those with DPD rated their families higher in control than did controls. The study may be limited by the nonclinical nature of the sample and by the retrospective nature of the mea- sures. In particular, people with HPD may recollect their family environ- ments in ways consistent with their personality traits or interpersonal goals.
Crawford, Cohen, and Brook (2001a) assessed a large mixed-gender sample before age 10 and at ages 13, 15, and 20 using symptom scales for HPD, NPD, and BPD derived from items on the PDQ (Hyler et al., 1982), SCID-II (Spitzer et al., 1987), and other instruments. Overall, Cluster B pathology proved stable over time, yielding stability estimates of .63 in boys and .69 in girls over 8 years old. Specific disorder categories were less stable, with the likelihood of receiving a histrionic or narcissistic diagnosis declin- ing between ages 13 and 18. Overall, Cluster B symptoms were strongly linked to internalizing symptoms in girls and to externalizing symptoms in both boys and girls, but the authors did not provide a breakdown of specific PDs. Subsequent analyses of the same data (Crawford et al., 2001b) indi- cated that in girls, internalizing symptoms at ages 10 to 14 predicted Cluster B symptoms in midadolescence, whereas externalizing symptoms at ages 12 to 17 predicted Cluster B pathology in young adulthood.
Cultural Factors
DSM-IV-TR cautions that cultural factors may affect specific manifesta- tions of HPD symptoms, but it has been criticized for its lack of specificity regarding the nature of such factors and their specific impact on the diagnosis of most PDs and HPD in particular (Alarcon, 1996). For example, several authors hypothesize that HPD may be diagnosed less frequently in Asian and other cultures that discourage overt sexualization and more frequently in Hispanic and Latin American cultures that sanction overt sexuality (Johnson, 1993; Padilla, 1995; Trull & Widiger, 2003). However, there has been no
  
214
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

systematic study of cultural differences in the presentation of HPD or of cul- tural factors in its etiology. Such factors have been examined in relation to the other two descendants of hysteria (conversion and somatization). In a tran- scultural psychiatric study of “hysterical structure” with a sample of 30 African and 30 Belgian patients matched for age, sex, and socioeconomic status, Pierloot and Ngoma (1988) found that the African patients tended to experience somatic symptoms resembling organic ones (e.g., headaches), while the Belgian patients were more likely to experience psychic symptoms (e.g., apathy). Additionally, while African patients were more likely to attribute their problems to supernatural forces, Belgian patients were more likely to cite internal causes. While this study examined differences in cultural manifesta- tions of “hysterical structure” (a construct comprising mixed features of HPD and somatization disorder), its findings can be considered a preliminary indi- cation that further examination of cultural differences in HPD is needed.
Conceptual and Diagnostic Issues _____________________
Seductiveness and Sexualization
DSM-II described the hysterical or histrionic personality as “often seduc- tive” for the purpose of self-dramatization (with or without awareness), whereas DSM-III omitted the seductiveness criterion due to critiques citing gender bias. Seductiveness reappeared in DSM-III-R and DSM-IV, phrased as an objective criterion with an emphasis on sexualized and seductive behavior under inappropriate circumstances. Such items as “tends to be overly sexually seductive or provocative, whether consciously or unconsciously,” “fantasizes about finding ideal, perfect love,” and “tends to choose sexual or romantic partners who seem inappropriate” were highly descriptive of the HPD proto- type derived empirically by Westen and Shedler (1999).
Despite its centrality to the description of the disorder, the nature of seduc- tiveness in HPD has received little rigorous empirical attention. Psychoanalytic writers observed that the coquetry of people with hysterical personalities eas- ily turns into disparagement (e.g., Reich, 1949). These authors suggested that hysterical individuals may deal with their anxiety surrounding close relationships by “acting it out” in the form of pseudohypersexuality (Fenichel, 1945) while avoiding true intimacy and remaining sexually unsat- isfied. Modern psychodynamic thinkers similarly view sexualization as a counterphobic defense (McWilliams, 1994). This hypothesis has not received rigorous empirical tests and seems difficult to falsify, as doing so would require the development of a valid measure of sexual avoidance.
Comparing 33 women with HPD with 33 randomly selected women with similar demographics (all participants were drawn from a marital therapy program), Apt and Hurlbert (1994) found meaningful differences on self- report measures of sexual attitudes. Women with HPD reported greater sexual self-esteem, but also greater levels of sexual boredom, erotophobia,
 
Histrionic Personality Disorder 215

orgasmic dysfunction, lower sexual desire, and more frequent extramarital affairs. Apt and Hurlbert proposed that “sexual narcissism” characterizes patients with HPD, whose preoccupation with receiving attention and being attractive leads them to believe that they are also great lovers. Because they engage in sexuality as a means of ego gratification but do not experience inti- macy or reciprocate, their sexual experiences are unsatisfying.
In hierarchical cluster analyses by Turner (1994), seductiveness loaded with self-centeredness and excessive concern with attractiveness, lending support to the notion of sexuality serving a narcissistic function. Excessive need for attention, however, loaded on a separate cluster. Finally, Reise and Wright (1996) did not find a relationship between histrionic features on the California Q-Set and uncommitted sexual relations in 195 undergraduates, although the tendency toward uncommitted sex correlated significantly with narcissism and psychopathy prototypes in men. Together, these findings sug- gest that sexualization in HPD may be a facet of self-centeredness or a means of regulating self-esteem.
Histrionic Personality and Gender
The extent to which gender influences or biases the diagnosis of HPD remains debated. Bias may influence clinical decisions more easily when clin- icians make global decisions without considering individual diagnostic crite- ria separately. Ford and Widiger (1989) obtained 381 psychologists’ ratings (24% women) of a case history of a person with HPD, antisocial personal- ity disorder (APD), or balanced features. The sex of the person was either male, female, or unspecified. Clinicians rated a number of Axis I and II disorders on a seven-point scale, with ratings of 5–7 indicating presence of the disorder. For balanced histories, clinicians made the HPD and APD diag- noses with approximately equal frequency across conditions of patient gen- der. When the history was consistent with APD, clinicians tended to diagnose it much less frequently in a woman (15%) than they did in a man (42%) or a person with unspecified gender (48%), but they tended to diag- nose HPD (46%) in women. When the history was that of HPD, they diag- nosed it at very high rates in women (76%), lower rates for unspecified gender (64%), and the lowest rates in men (44%). The HPD history of a man elicited relatively high ratings of APD compared with the other gender conditions, although this difference was not statistically significant. Clinicians’ ratings of individual diagnostic criteria, however, did not differ by sex. One possible alternative to a “sex bias” interpretation of these find- ings is that clinicians may have correctly taken into account the base rates of APD (higher among men) and HPD (which may be higher among women in clinical settings) when making overall diagnoses.
Other studies have yielded inconclusive results concerning sex bias in HPD diagnosis. Belitsky et al. (1996) asked 96 psychiatric residents (45 women) to rate Ford and Widiger’s (1989) case histories described above.
   
216
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Participants who received the APD history were more likely to diagnose APD correctly if the patient was a man and to underdiagnose it when the patient was a woman. The sex of the patient did not influence HPD diagno- sis in this study, although it may have influenced ratings of prognosis (HPD received a more favorable prognosis if the sex of the patient was female, but female sex of the patient also led to more favorable prognosis overall).
Bias may be present not only in clinician’s judgments but also in the extent to which HPD criteria reflect feminine versus masculine gender roles. Sprock (2000) compiled lists of HPD behavioral examples for the DSM-III-R and DSM-IV criteria by asking 120 undergraduates (60 women) to read the criteria and provide examples for each. Counterbalancing for student gender, one third of the undergraduates received instructions to write behaviors that would typ- ify men with HPD, one third wrote about women, and one third received gen- der-unspecified instructions. Sprock then sent subsets of behaviors to 157 clinicians (58 psychiatrists and 97 psychologists) and asked them to rate the extent to which the behaviors were representative of either the diagnostic crite- ria or their own overall conceptualization of HPD. Masculine behaviors received significantly lower representativeness ratings when compared with feminine or gender-unspecified behaviors in both the HPD criteria condition and the overall conceptualization of HPD. Following this, bias concerns may be addressed by writing diagnostic criteria thought to capture masculine manifes- tations of HPD (e.g., bragging or hypermasculinity), or as suggested by some (e.g., Sprock, 2000), criteria that have been found empirically to represent HPD equally well in men and women (e.g., dramatic exaggeration in speech or jeal- ousy at inattention). Some researchers have voiced concerns, however, that such an approach may lower the validity of the construct (Widiger, 1998).
Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy
The description of histrionic personality, especially in men (e.g., Luisada et al., 1974), shares a resemblance with APD. Warner (1978) demonstrated that various mental health professionals are likely to label a patient descrip- tion as hysterical personality if the patient’s sex was said to be female, but to diagnose APD and hysterical personality at nearly equal rates if the sex was said to be male. Thus, Warner suggested that APD and hysterical per- sonality could be gender-typed forms of the same condition. Indeed, APD and HPD appear to co-occur in individuals more often than would be expected if they each occurred by chance (Lilienfeld, VanValkenburg, Larntz, & Akiskal, 1986), and histrionic men report high rates of antisocial acts (Luisada et al., 1974). APD overlaps moderately with the construct of psychopathy (Cleckley, 1941/1982), as do certain features of HPD, such as shallow expression of emotion, seductiveness, manipulativeness, and dis- honesty. Hart and Hare (1989) reported a significant correlation between HPD and scores on an interview-based psychopathy measure.
  
Histrionic Personality Disorder 217

Standage, Bilsbury, Jain, and Smith (1984) predicted that histrionic women, like psychopathic individuals, may have low perspective taking abilities. They compared 20 female inpatients meeting DSM-III criteria for histrionic person- ality with 20 depressed female inpatients matched for age, expressive vocabu- lary, and depression scores. The groups did not differ significantly on a paper-and-pencil measure of perspective taking, but the histrionic women scored significantly lower on the Socialization scale of the California Psychological Inventory and higher on the Psychoticism and Lie scales of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. Thus the histrionic inpatients scored high on measures that may be expected to relate to psychopathy and APD, either due to overlap between these constructs and HPD or due to co-occurrence of APD and HPD features in the sample. Additionally, the histrionic women in Standage et al.’s (1984) study had greater rates of features often associated with APD, such as attempted suicide, drug and alcohol abuse, unemployment, and first-degree relatives with a criminal history, than did depressed controls.
In a group of 180 undergraduates (90 women; 64% Caucasian), Hamburger, Lilienfeld, and Hogben (1996) tested the hypotheses that psychopathy underlies both APD and HPD and that the relationship of APD and HPD with psychopa- thy is moderated by both biological sex and gender roles. Scores on the Psychopathic Personality Inventory (PPI; Lilienfeld, 1990) predicted scores on measures of both APD (r = .48 and .59) and HPD (r = .16 and .49) with an alpha of less than .05. Furthermore, the relationship between PPI scores and APD scores was stronger among men than it was among women, whereas the rela- tionship between PPI and HPD scores was stronger among women. The classi- fication of participants as masculine, feminine, androgynous, or undifferentiated, however, did not emerge as a moderator of the relationship between psychopa- thy and APD or HPD.
Cale and Lilienfeld (2002) examined the hypothesis that APD and HPD could be behavioral manifestations of psychopathy moderated by biological sex in a nonclinical sample of 75 actors (36 women). The authors sampled actors because they deemed it likely to find high levels and a wide range of HPD symptoms such as emotionality, attention seeking, and dramatization. Correlational analyses of self-report and peer ratings revealed that psychopa- thy was more strongly associated with HPD in women and with APD in men.
Thus, even though the association between HPD, APD, and psychopathy has been replicated, the extent to which HPD and APD are gender-typed manifestations of the same latent construct or simply co-occur remains unclear and calls for further study.
________________________Theoretical Conceptualizations
Psychodynamic and cognitive conceptualizations of HPD have been the most influential in the clinical field and have had the greatest impact on treatment. Therefore, we address these conceptualizations in the section on psychotherapy
  
218
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

below. The dynamic and cognitive theories have met with increasing competi- tion from neurobehavioral, trait, and attachment conceptualizations among researchers.
Neurobehavioral Model
According to Depue and Lenzenweger’s (2001) neurobehavioral dimen- sional model, PDs are combinations of extremes on orthogonal traits of emotion, affiliation, and control. The ratio of agentic extraversion (or posi- tive emotionality, mediated by the neurotransmitter dopamine in the ventral tegmental area) and neuroticism (or negative emotionality/anxiety, mediated by the central amygdala and the neurotransmitter norepinephrine) comprises one emotional dimension. Harm avoidance (fear, mediated by the stria termi- nalis and norepinephrine) comprises a second emotional dimension, whereas impulsivity (nonaffective constraint, mediated by serotonin) and affiliation (mediated by oxytocin and other gonadal steroids) are the nonaffective traits. In this model, HPD is characterized by a high ratio of agentic extra- version to neuroticism, low nonaffective constraint, and moderate to high affiliation. Theoretically, the high dopamine and low serotonin in these indi- viduals predispose them to gregariousness and emotional lability. In a related evolutionary neurochemical model, Klein (1999) hypothesized that a noradrenergic primate regulatory mechanism responsive to social cues of interest or rejection may become dysregulated in some individuals.
Trait Models
Trait theorists conceptualize PDs as constellations of maladaptive or extreme variants of otherwise normative personality dispositions (Widiger & Bornstein, 2001). According to Widiger and Frances (1994), the five-factor model (FFM) may be useful in describing and diagnosing PDs. As part of a larger study, Lynam and Widiger (2001) asked 19 experts on HPD to think of and rate a prototypical person with HPD on a scale of 1 to 5 along items used to measure dimensions of the FFM. Ratings for the five broad domains, neu- roticism (N), extraversion (E), openness (O), agreeableness (A), and conscien- tiousness (C), were neither high nor low, suggesting that clinicians thought that patients with HPD tended to be average or unremarkable on these dimen- sions. Ratings comprising the facets within each domain were more informative. On average, clinicians rated HPD as low on self-consciousness and high on impulsiveness (within the N domain); high on gregariousness, activity, excite- ment seeking, and positive emotions (within E); high on fantasy, feelings, and actions (within O), and low on self-discipline and deliberation (within C). In contrast, Brieger, Sommer, Bloink, and Marneros (2000) reported that, in a sample of 229 former patients and controls, high N, high E, and low C pre- dicted dimensional scores on a histrionic personality dimension based on the criteria of the tenth revision of the International Classification of Disorders
   
Histrionic Personality Disorder 219

(ICD-10; World Health Organization, 1993). Given such heterogeneous find- ings, application of the FFM in conceptualizing and diagnosing HPD may be premature.
Attachment Model
From an attachment perspective (Bartholomew, Kwong, & Hart, 2001), HPD is consistent with what is referred to as a preoccupied attachment style. Such individuals are theorized to hold a positive mental model of others (pre- disposing them to approach others) and a negative model of the self (predis- posing them to feel vulnerable, especially to rejection). People with this attachment style are thought to have strong relational needs and actively seek relationships, but may be so hypervigilant that they respond to the slightest interpersonal stress or disappointment with increased attachment behaviors, which can overwhelm and scare away potential partners. Attachment theo- rists see the emotionality and need for attention in individuals with HPD as evidence for attachment anxiety, whereas they see the extraversion and per- haps sexualization as evidence of intense relational needs.
_________________ Assessment and Differential Diagnosis
No specialized instrument exists for the screening and assessment of HPD. However, two commonly used broad PD measures include HPD scales. The general psychometric properties of these instruments as well as those of their HPD scales are outlined below.
The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders (SCID-II; First, Gibbon, Spitzer, Williams, & Benjamin, 1997) has one item per criterion for each of the PD diagnoses, to be rated during the interview on a scale from 1 (“absent or false”) to 3 (“threshold or true”). Most research on the SCID-II was conducted using its previous version, the DSM-III-R SCID-II (e.g., Dreessen & Arntz, 1998; Renneberg, Chambless, Dowdall, Fauerbach, & Gracely, 1992; Spitzer, Williams, Gibbon, & First, 1990), and indicates acceptable levels of interrater reliability and internal consistency. At least one paper (Maffei et al., 1997) reports adequate levels of interrater reli- ability (kappa = .92; intraclass correlation [ICC] = .95) and internal consis- tency (Armor’s theta = .87) for HPD using the DSM-IV version of the SCID-II.
The Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire 4+ (PDQ-4+; Hyler & Rieder, 1994) is a self-report measure that (as the SCID-II) assesses DSM-IV criteria for the 10 primary PDs and the two designated for further study. Correlations among all PDQ-4+ and SCID-II scales were low to moderate, but significant (r = .19–.42), and PDQ-4+ scales exhibited mediocre internal consistencies. Only two scales (antisocial and dependent) showed strong powers of discrimination.
A larger concern in evaluating HPD is its differentiation from other dis- orders, and common comorbid conditions.
 
220 PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Treatments for HPD _________________________________
With the exception of Linehan’s (1993) study of dialectical behavior therapy for BPD, the literature offers little rigorous research on the treatment of Axis II disorders. What follows is a description of the extant research, which varies greatly in scope and quality.
General Characteristics of Treatment
A panel of three experts nominated and elected by the psychiatric com- munity in Australia (Quality Assurance Project, 1991) concluded that long- term psychotherapy would be beneficial to HPD patients and also likely be cost-effective by minimizing adverse economic outcomes of the disorder. Nevertheless, it should be noted that they were not citing research evidence specific to HPD to buttress this assumption. They conceptualized DSM- III-R HPD as a variant of BPD and asserted that the dramatizing and sexu- alizing behavior of HPD patients is a way of asserting a false sense of identity to counteract the feelings of emptiness and lack of identity underlying border- line pathology. The limited empirical record (primarily single- and multiple-case reports) suggested that three or more years of relatively intense (e.g., twice- weekly) therapy may be needed for good outcomes in one half to two thirds of patients. This panel recommended outpatient individual, group, or combined therapy with hospitalization and medication only during crisis.
While acknowledging that brief therapy for HPD may not be optimal, Dorfman (2000) recommended increasing its effectiveness in the context of managed care by taking an integrative stance and maintaining a specific focus on short-term goals (dealing with acute distress) and long-term changes in inter- personal behavior. He related the case of a 49-year old woman who at the start of treatment communicated in vague and self-dramatizing ways, experienced herself as a victim and others as unsympathetic, suffered from turbulent abu- sive relationships, and complained of feelings of anxiety, depression, empti- ness, and inadequacy. The sessions focused initially on creating a working alliance, then on helping the client label beliefs about dependency and aban- donment in order to help her understand their impact on her relationships, and finally on replacement of dramatic manipulations with alternative inter- personal behavior. Dorfman conducted 13 biweekly sessions with this patient and gave her the option of boosters. Several months after termina- tion, she reported only occasional dysphoria, avoided abusive relationships, and maintained some insight into her need for attention, but she continued to be emotionally reactive under stress and overly concerned with her appearance. The efficacy and cost-effectiveness of such brief treatments remain largely unexplored using formal methods.
Other researchers have investigated the influence of HPD on treatment characteristics such as termination. Hilsenroth, Holdwick, Castlebury, and Blais (1998) reviewed literature suggesting that PDs are common in psychiatric patients and adversely affect the outcome of treatment for Axis I disorders, and
 
Histrionic Personality Disorder 221
 
sought in particular to examine the links between Cluster B PDs and psy- chotherapy termination. In 90 patients with different PDs, the number of DSM-IV criteria for HPD met did not correlate significantly with the number of psychotherapy sessions. In a stepwise regression, HPD criterion 8, “consid- ers relationships to be more intimate than they really are,” predicted the number of therapy sessions independent of other significant predictors, such as the BPD criterion “frantic efforts to avoid real or imagined abandonment” and the NPD criterion “requires excessive admiration.” Unfortunately, stepwise regression tends to capitalize on chance findings, and these findings may be dif- ficult to replicate. Their generalizability may also be limited by the atypical nature of the sample, which included patients meeting criteria for only one PD, which is not usually the case in the community. Other researchers (Tyrer, Mitchard, Methuen, & Ranger, 2003) have found patients with HPD not par- ticularly likely to either seek or reject therapy.
Expressive Psychodynamic Therapy
From a psychodynamic perspective, HPD is a hysterical character style (McWilliams, 1994; Shapiro, 1965) presenting at the borderline level of dys- function (Kernberg, 1970, 1975). The hysterical style is characterized by dif- fuse, global, and impressionistic cognition that makes motivated inattention easy and self-understanding difficult. It can present at a neurotic, borderline, or psychotic level of severity (see also Pollak, 1981; Zisook, DeVaul, & Gammon, 1979), with borderline implying that the person has difficulty establishing a stable sense of self. Kernberg (2004) considers most histrionic patients to be in the high (meaning healthier) borderline range (see Kernberg, 2004, for the distinction between BPD and borderline personality organiza- tion.) Because patients’ representations of the self and others are split into all- good and all-bad fragments, under conditions of stress and negative emotion the experience of self as all bad may lead to intense suffering and self-harm, whereas projective identification and the experience of others as evil may lead to rage. Yet the patient may depend on relationships with others to feel whole. Thus, manipulativeness and acting out may be self-defeating ways to maintain the attention of others. This psychodynamic conceptualization has a phenomenological appeal but remains wanting for empirical validation.
Psychodynamic therapists are likely to view such pathology as arrested development rather than conflict (Mitchell, 1988; Mitchell & Black, 1995; Quality Assurance Project, 1991). Therefore, they recommend modifications to the classical technique of therapist neutrality. Instead, they suggest more directive and expressive approaches (McWilliams, 1994, 2004) in which the therapist shows empathy, helps the client label and understand his or her dreaded feelings, promotes tolerance for anxiety and ambiguity, and helps establish a complex and realistic understanding of the self, others, and rela- tionships. Because of these patients’ fragile sense of identity and the dramatic shifts in how they feel about themselves and the therapist, firm boundaries and clear, consistent conditions of therapy are essential. For similar reasons,
 
222
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

psychodynamic therapists often work with such patients face-to-face instead of using the couch, and they see them only once or twice a week. Research has not yet addressed the efficacy of the above recommendations.
The therapist attempts to promote healthy maturation by responding to elements of the patient’s expression that are likely to be genuine and ignoring those that are impersonal or mendacious (Quality Assurance Project, 1991). Early in the treatment, the therapist focuses on building the alliance. Inevitably, the patient’s unstable representations begin to create disruptions in the relationship due to idealization or devaluation, at which point the ther- apist points them out to the patient and links them to past and recent experi- ences. Patients learn to reflect on their feelings and interactions and realize that they can share negative aspects of the self with the therapist without falling apart or being abandoned, which is intended to help build self-esteem. The therapist gradually challenges the patient to link different aspects of his or her ways of experiencing the self and others to establish a sense of identity, continuity, and authenticity. Patients may use pseudoinsight, or superficial insights that are quickly forgotten and primarily aim to please and flatter the therapist, posing a challenge in later phases of treatment (Chodoff, 1978).
In a meta-analysis of controlled studies of psychodynamic (n = 14) and cognitive (n = 11) therapy outcome for PDs employing measures such as the Beck Depression Inventory, the Symptom Checklist–90 (SCL-90), the Health- Sickness Rating Scale, and the Global Adjustment Scale, Leichsenring and Leibing (2003) found large effect sizes for both kinds of therapy on self-report as well as observer ratings of outcome. More recently, Svartberg, Stiles, and Seltzer (2004) reported similar results. In addition, some evidence emerged for the long-term effectiveness of psychodynamic therapies (Leichsenring & Leibing, 2003). Nevertheless, the effectiveness of psychodynamic therapies has not yet been demonstrated in controlled studies for HPD per se. Similarly, the question of whether these therapies work by means of their hypothesized mechanisms needs to be addressed empirically.
Cognitive Therapy
Cognitive therapists conceptualize PDs as attributable to dysfunctional schemas that develop out of people’s tendencies to cope in certain ways with interpersonal challenges (Beck & Freeman, 1990). A person who develops HPD may have had strong dependency and rejection sensitivity tendencies as a child and may have received strong reinforcement from others for atten- tion seeking behavior or the exaggerated display of gender-typed behaviors. The former tendencies may result in a core belief that “I am basically unat- tractive,” and the reinforcement history may lead to a core belief that “I need others to admire me in order to be happy.” The individual compensates by believing “I am lovable, entertaining, and interesting,” “I am entitled to admiration,” and “People are there to admire me” (Beck & Freeman, 1990, p. 50). Thus, according to this framework, persons with HPD view them- selves as deserving of unfaltering attention and view others positively as long
  
Histrionic Personality Disorder 223

as they provide it. Their overt cheerfulness conceals underlying anxiety and rejection sensitivity. People with HPD deal pragmatically with the belief that they are unattractive by seeking minute-to-minute interaction and attention in an attempt to counter this fear. They quickly act on their frustration if a person contradicts their compensatory beliefs by ignoring them. Thus, the person with HPD operates on conditional beliefs such as “Unless I captivate others, I am nothing,” “If I can’t entertain people, they will abandon me,” and “If people don’t respond, they are rotten.” An associated belief is “I can go by my feelings,” which gives one the freedom to express immediate feel- ings of affection or aggression toward others in exaggerated ways, regardless of their appropriateness. Thus, the histrionic approach to life is based on global, impressionistic, and fleeting cognitions that are at odds with the problem solving and focused set that cognitive therapy teaches.
Work in the cognitive approach begins by gradually helping the patient identify feelings and thoughts and then challenging irrational thoughts, first in relation to specific symptoms and gradually at a more global level. Patients with HPD may find homework assignments boring, which is why Beck and Freeman (1990) suggest that therapists encourage patients to use vivid imagination when working on homework (e.g., by phrasing their rational thoughts in dramatic ways). It may be helpful to clarify to patients that treatment will not eradicate the strong emotions they value but will make them more constructive. Emotional outbursts can be dealt with by clarifying the goal (e.g., to receive positive attention), listing the pros and cons of an outburst in relation to the goal, and coming up with alternative ways of reaching it. Such interventions may help stabilize the patient’s rela- tionships, but ultimately the therapy must help patients address their sense of identity and core beliefs. Patients may gradually write down things that describe themselves as a way of building a stable identity, and they may engage in small behavioral experiments to challenge their belief that they depend on others’ care and attention. A rational argument has been made that this work could last for up to 3 years. However, the specific formula- tions and treatment recommendations of cognitive therapy for HPD, like those of psychodynamic therapy, have not been investigated extensively.
Cognitive therapy for PDs has been described in several volumes (e.g., Beck & Freeman, 1990; Beck, Freeman, & Davis, 2004; Young, 1999), but its empirical support still comes primarily from case studies (e.g., Bernstein, 2002; Morrison, 2000) and a small number of clinical trials (Leichsenring & Leibing, 2003; Svartberg et al., 2004), in which its effectiveness was compa- rable with that of psychodynamic therapy. The field is still wanting for empir- ical evaluations of the effectiveness of cognitive therapy specifically for HPD.
Other Therapies
A few other proposed therapies for HPD are worth mentioning, although they will not be discussed at length for brevity’s sake. Horowitz (1997) described a rationally derived cognitive/psychodynamic approach that starts
  
224
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

with a configurational assessment of symptoms, states of mind, defense styles, person schemas, and the level of severity. This approach involves three primary phases, one focusing on attainment of emotional and behavioral stabilization, one focusing on improving communication, and one focusing on modifying defenses. Horowitz did not specify treatment duration, but his framework implies that it would depend upon the level of severity. A related cognitive/ analytic approach proposed by Ryle (1997) has received limited research sup- port in Europe for the treatment of BPD (Garyfallos et al., 2002; Kerr, 1999).
The application of functional analytic therapy with histrionic patients as described by Callaghan, Summers, and Weidman (2003) may appeal to pro- ponents of both interpersonal and behavioral approaches. They apply func- tional analysis principles within the therapeutic relationship by extinguishing or punishing interpersonally problematic behaviors and reinforcing desirable behaviors idiosyncratic for the patient. For example, therapists may share with clients the way their behavior made them feel, acting under the assump- tion that the client enacts the same maladaptive behaviors that occur outside of therapy and that their modification through feedback in therapy will gen- eralize to other relationships. Although descriptive case studies implementing this approach with histrionic patients have been cited (e.g., Callaghan et al., 2003), sufficient empirical support for its effectiveness is still lacking.
While no clinical trials of pharmacotherapy for HPD currently exist, Kool, Dekker, Duijsens, De Jonghe, and Puite (2003) report some potentially rele- vant findings regarding pharmacotherapy for personality pathology in general. They compared two groups, one receiving pharmacotherapy only for depres- sion (a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor, a tricyclic antidepressant, or a reversible monoamine oxidase inhibitor for 6 months; n = 25), and one receiv- ing pharmacotherapy and brief psychodynamic therapy (n = 47). Using a self- report inventory to measure personality pathology before treatment and 40 weeks later, they found that overall personality pathology (including histri- onicity) decreased in the combined treatment group regardless of their improve- ment on depression, while only those patients who improved on depression in the pharmacotherapy group scored lower on personality pathology. The authors reported that patients with Cluster B pathology showed the least reduc- tion in PD scores, but the role of HPD in this was unclear, as was the propor- tion of patients who had HPD. Other authors (e.g., Grossman, 2004) have suggested that, given the emotional regulation difficulties of patients with HPD, medications such as selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, mood stabilizers, and anticonvulsants deserve research consideration.
Questions for Future Research ________________________
Many research questions remain pertaining to the conceptualization, etiol- ogy, and treatment of HPD. We will highlight four that we consider most significant as we move toward DSM-V:
  
Histrionic Personality Disorder 225

1. What are the real-world outcomes of HPD? As noted earlier in this chapter, little is known about the clinical predictive power of HPD for outcomes such as relationship satisfaction and stability, employment history, general health, and life satisfaction. Furthermore, empirical studies should examine the degree to which factors such as Axis I comorbidity and severity of HPD presentation affect the degree to which negative life outcomes are experienced. Relatedly, some researchers (e.g., Svanborg et al., 2000) have cited preliminary biological evidence that histrionic features may be adaptive for some individuals. The con- ditions under which features of HPD can serve an adaptive function are another interesting direction for further research.
2. Can endophenotypic markers of HPD help elucidate its degree of dif- ferentiation from allied conditions? Continued overlap between HPD and other Axis II conditions such as BPD, NPD, and DPD calls for an examination of the degree to which these disorders can be differenti- ated as well as their incremental validity over and above each other in predicting clinically relevant outcomes. Endophenotypic markers, as assessed by laboratory measures, are an as of yet relatively unexplored but potentially interesting avenue for doing so.
3. What is the current status of cultural differences in the presentation and course of HPD? Not only is there a lack of empirical literature regarding the nature of cultural differences in HPD, but theoretical writings pertaining to this topic are likely outdated by now. For example, changes in the cultural and economic conditions in different global regions may have changed hypothesized prevalence rates of HPD. Furthermore, the degree to which HPD is more or less preva- lent or may have distinctive features within cultural enclaves in non- native countries (e.g., the U.S.) is also an unexplored question.
4. What is the best course of treatment for HPD? While the proposed treatments for HPD reviewed in this chapter are appealing from a phenomenological standpoint, it is clear that more rigorous treatment outcome research must examine these and other proposed therapies. Additionally, it is important to compare proposed treatments with one another on well-operationalized outcome variables in order to examine the possible differences in efficacy. When selecting targeted outcomes, in keeping with suggestions regarding personality consid- erations in treatment planning (see Harkness & Lilienfeld, 1997), clinicians and researchers should consider selecting some reflective of “characteristic adaptations” (behaviors arising from maladaptive traits) in addition to reduction of trait levels themselves. Such an approach is implied in therapies utilizing functional analytic (Callaghan et al., 2003) and interpersonal components (Dorfman, 2000) and awaits large-scale empirical validation.
  
226 PERSONALITY DISORDERS

References _________________________________________
Abraham, K. (1927/1948). Manifestations of the female castration complex (D. Bryan & A. Strachey, Trans.). London: Hogarth.
Alam, C. N., & Merskey, C. (1992). The development of the hysterical personality. History of Psychiatry, 3(2), 135–165.
Alarcon, R. D. (1996). Personality disorders and culture in DSM-IV: A critique. Journal of Personality Disorders, 10(3), 260–270.
American Psychiatric Association. (1952). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1968). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of men- tal disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of men- tal disorders (3rd ed., revised). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.
Ampollini, P., Merchesi, C., Signifredi, R., Ghinaglia, E., Scardovi, F., Codeluppi, S., et al. (1999). Temperament and personality features in patients with major depression, panic disorder and mixed conditions. Journal of Affective Disorders, 52, 203–207.
Apt, C., & Hurlbert, D. F. (1994). The sexual attitudes, behavior, and relationships of women with histrionic personality disorder. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 20(2), 125–133.
Baker, J. D., Capron, E. W., & Azorlosa, J. (1996). Family environment characteris- tics of persons with histrionic and dependent personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 10(1), 82–87.
Bartholomew, K., Kwong, M. J., & Hart, S. D. (2001). Attachment. In W. J. Livesley (Ed.), Handbook of personality disorders: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 196–230). New York: Guilford Press.
Baumbacher, G., & Amini, F. (1980–1981). The hysterical personality disorder: A proposed classification of a diagnostic dilemma. International Journal of Psycho- analytic Psychotherapy, 8, 501–532.
Beck, A. T., & Freeman, A. (1990). Cognitive therapy of the personality disorders. New York: Guilford.
Beck, A. T., Freeman, A., & Davis, D. D. (2004). Cognitive therapy of personality disorders (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.
Belitsky, C. A., Toner, B. B., Ali, A., Yu, B., Osborne, S. L., & deRooy, E. (1996). Sex-role attitudes and clinical appraisal in psychiatry residents. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 41, 503–508.
Bernstein, D. P. (2002). Cognitive therapy of personality disorders in patients with histories of emotional abuse or neglect. Psychiatric Annals, 32(10), 618–628.
Blais, M. A., Hilsenroth, M. H., & Fowler, J. C. (1998). Rorschach correlates of the DSM-IV histrionic personality disorder. Journal of Personality Assessment, 70(2), 355–364.
Blais, M. A., & Norman, D. K. (1997). A psychometric evaluation of the DSM-IV personality disorder criteria. Journal of Personality Disorders, 11, 168–176.
  
Histrionic Personality Disorder 227

Blashfield, R. K., & Davis, R. T. (1993). Dependent and histrionic personality disorders. In P. B. Sutker & H. E. Adams (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of psychopathology (pp. 395–409). New York: Plenum.
Bornstein, R. F. (1998). Implicit and self-attributed dependency needs in dependent and histrionic personality disorders. Journal of Personality Assessment, 71(1), 1–14.
Bornstein, R. F. (1999). Dependent and histrionic personality disorders. In T. Millon, P. H. Blaney, & R. D. Davis (Eds.), Oxford textbook of psychopathology (pp. 535–554). New York: Oxford University Press.
Brieger, P., Sommer, S., Bloink, R., & Marneros, A. (2000). The relationship between five-factor personality measurements and ICD-10 personality disorder dimen- sions: Results from a sample of 229 subjects. Journal of Personality Disorders, 14(3), 282–290.
Cale, E. M., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2002). Histrionic personality disorder and antiso- cial personality disorder: Sex-differentiated manifestations of psychopathy. Journal of Personality Disorders, 16(1), 52–72.
Callaghan, G. M., Summers, C. J., & Weidman, M. (2003). The treatment of histri- onic and narcissistic personality disorder behaviors: A single-subject demon- stration of clinical improvement using functional analytic psychotherapy. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 33(4), 321–339.
Chodoff, P. (1974). The diagnosis of hysteria: An overview. American Journal of Psychiatry, 131, 1073–1078.
Chodoff, P. (1978). Psychotherapy of the hysterical personality disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis, 6(4), 497–510.
Chodoff, P. (1982). Hysteria and women. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139(5), 545–551.
Cleckley, H. (1941/1982). The mask of sanity. St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
Cloninger, R., Martin, R. L., Guze, S., & Clayton, P. (1986). A prospective follow-up and family study of somatization in men and women. American Journal of Psychiatry,
143, 873–878.
Cohen, B. J., Nestadt, G., Samuels, J. F., Romanoski, A. J., McHugh, P. R., &
Rabins, P. V. (1994). Personality disorder in later life: A community study.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 493–499.
Coid, J. W. (1999). Aetiological risk factors for personality disorders. British Journal
of Psychiatry, 174, 530–538.
Cramer, P. (1999). Personality, personality disorders, and defense mechanisms.
Journal of Personality, 67(3), 535–554.
Crawford, T. N., Cohen, P., & Brook, J. S. (2001a). Dramatic-erratic personality dis-
order symptoms: I. Continuity from early adolescence into adulthood. Journal
of Personality Disorders, 15(4), 319–335.
Crawford, T. N., Cohen, P., & Brook, J. S. (2001b). Dramatic-erratic personality
disorder symptoms: II. Developmental pathways from early adolescence to
adulthood. Journal of Personality Disorders, 15(4), 336–350.
Daley, S. E., Hammen, C., Burge, D., Davila, J., Paley, B., Lindberg, N., et al. (1999). Depression and Axis II symptomatology in an adolescent community sample: Concurrent and longitudinal associations. Journal of Personality Disorders,
13(1), 47–59.
Depue, R. A., & Lenzenweger, M. F. (2001). A neurobehavioral dimensional model.
In W. J. Livesley (Ed.), Handbook of personality disorders: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 136–176). New York: Guilford Press.
228
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Dorfman, W. I. (2000). Histrionic personality disorder. In M. Hersen & M. Biaggio (Eds.), Effective brief therapies: A clinician’s guide (pp. 355–370). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Dreessen, L., & Arntz, A. (1998). Short-interval test-retest interrater reliability of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R Personality Disorders (SCID-II) in outpatients. Journal of Personality Disorders, 12(2), 138–148.
Easser, B., & Lesser, S. (1965). Hysterical character and psychoanalysis. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 34, 390–405.
Ekselius, L., Lindstrom, E., von Knorring, L., Bodlund, O., & Kullgren, G. (1994). SCID-II interviews and the SCID Screen questionnaire as diagnostic tools for per- sonality disorders in DSM-III-R. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 90, 120–123.
Fenichel, O. (1945). The psychoanalytic theory of neurosis. New York: W. W. Norton. First, M. B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., & Benjamin, L. S. (1997). User’s guide for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. (1995). Structured clinical inter- view for DSM-IV Axis I disorders. New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute. Ford, M. R., & Widiger, T. A. (1989). Sex bias in the diagnosis of histrionic and anti- social personality disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
57(2), 301–305.
Frank, G. (1990). Research on the clinical usefulness of the Rorschach: I. The diag-
nosis of schizophrenia. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 71(2), 573–578.
Freud, S., & Breuer, J. (1895/2000). Studies on hysteria. In J. Strachey (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud
(Vol. 2). London: Hogarth.
Garyfallos, G., Adamopoulous, A., Karastergious, A., Voikli, M., Zlatanos, D., &
Tsifida, S. (2002). Evaluation of cognitive-analytic therapy (CAT) outcome: A
4–8 year follow up. European Journal of Psychiatry, 16(4), 197–209. Grossman, R. (2004). Pharmacotherapy of personality disorders. In J. J. Magnavita (Ed.), Handbook of personality disorders: Theory and practice (pp. 331–355).
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Grueneich, R. (1992). The borderline personality disorder diagnoses: Reliability,
diagnostic efficiency, and covariation with other personality disorder diagnoses.
Journal of Personality Disorders, 6(3), 197–212.
Hamburger, M. E., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Hogben, M. (1996). Psychopathy, gender,
and gender roles: Implications for antisocial and histrionic personality disorders.
Journal of Personality Disorders, 10(1), 41–55.
Harkness, A. R, & Lilienfeld, S. O. (1997). Individual differences science for treat-
ment planning: Personality traits. Psychological Assessment, 9(4), 349–360. Hart, S. D., & Hare, R. D. (1989). Discriminant validity of the Psychopathy Checklist
in a forensic psychiatric population. Psychological Assessment, 1, 211–218. Hilsenroth, M. H., Holdwick, D. J. J., Castlebury, F. D., & Blais, M. A. (1998). The effects of DSM-IV Cluster B Personality Disorder symptoms on the termination
and continuation of psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 35(2), 163–176. Horowitz, M. (1997). Psychotherapy for histrionic personality disorder. Journal of
Psychotherapy Practice and Research, 6(2), 93–104.
Hyler, S. E., & Rieder, R. O. (1996). Personality diagnostic questionnaire (4th revi-
sion). New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute.
Hyler, S., Reider, R., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. (1982). The personality diag-
nostic questionnaire (PDQ). New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute.
Histrionic Personality Disorder 229

Hyler, S., Skodol, A. E., Kellman, H. D., Oldham, J. M., & Rosnick, L. (1990). Validity of the Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire—Revised: Comparison with two structured interviews. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 1043–1048.
Johnson, F. F. (1993). Dependency and Japanese socialization. New York: New York University Press.
Kernberg, O. (1970). A psychoanalytic classification of character pathology. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 18, 51–85.
Kernberg, O. (1975). Borderline conditions and pathological narcissism. Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson.
Kernberg, O. (2004). Borderline personality disorder and borderline personality organization: Psychopathology and psychotherapy. In J. J. Magnavita (Ed.), Handbook of personality disorders: Theory and practice (pp. 92–119). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Kerr, I. B. (1999). Cognitive-analytic therapy for borderline personality disorder in the context of a community mental health team: Individual and organizational psychodynamic implications. British Journal of Psychiatry, 15(4), 425–438.
Klein, D. F. (1999). Harmful dysfunction, disorder, disease, illness, and evolution. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 421–429.
Kool, S., Dekker, J., Duijsens, I. J., De Jonghe, F., & Puite, B. (2003). Changes in per- sonality pathology after pharmacotherapy and combined therapy for depressed patients. Journal of Personality Disorders, 17(1), 60–72.
Kraus, G., & Reynolds, D. J. (2001). The “A-B-C’s” of the Cluster B’s: Identifying, understanding, and treating Cluster B personality disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 21(3), 345–373.
Lazare, A. (1971). The hysterical character in psychoanalytic theory. Archives of General Psychiatry, 25, 131–137.
Leichsenring, F., & Leibing, E. (2003). The effectiveness of psychodynamic therapy and cognitive behavior therapy in the treatment of personality disorders: A meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1223–1232.
Lilienfeld, S. O. (1990). Development and preliminary validation of a self-report measure of psychopathic personality. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis.
Lilienfeld, S. O., VanValkenburg, C., Larntz, K., & Akiskal, H. S. (1986). The rela- tionship of histrionic personality disorder to antisocial personality and somati- zation disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 143(6), 718–722.
Lilienfeld, S. O., Waldman, I., & Israel, A. C. (1994). A critical examination of the use of the term and concept of comorbidity in psychopathology research. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 1, 71–83.
Linehan, M. (1993). Cognitive behavioral therapy for borderline personality disor- ders. New York: Guilford.
Liskow, B. L., Clayton, P., Woodruff, R., Guze, S., & Cloninger, R. (1977). Briquet’s syndrome, hysterical personality, and the MMPI. American Journal of Psychiatry, 134(10), 1137–1139.
Livesley, W. J., & Schroeder, M. L. (1991). Dimensions of Personality Disorder: The DSM- III-R Cluster B diagnoses. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 179(6), 320–328. Luisada, P. V., Peele, R., & Pittard, E. A. (1974). The hysterical personality in men.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 131(5), 518–522.
Lynam, D. R., & Widiger, T. A. (2001). Using the five-factor model to represent the
DSM-IV personality disorders: An expert consensus approach. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(3), 401–412.
230
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Maffei, C., Fossati, A., Agostoni, I., Barraco, A., Bagnato, M., Deborah, D., et al. (1997). Interrater reliability and internal consistency of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II), version 2.0. Journal of Personality Disorders, 11(3), 279–284.
Maier, W., Franke, P., & Hawellek, B. (1998). Special feature: Family genetic research strategies for personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 12(3), 262–276.
Marmor, J. (1953). Orality in the hysterical personality. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 1, 656–671.
Maynard, R. E., & Meyer, G. E. (1996). Visual information processing with obsessive- compulsive and hysterical personalities. Personality and Individual Differences, 20(3), 389–399.
McWilliams, N. (1994). Psychoanalytic diagnosis: Understanding personality struc- ture in the clinical process. New York: Guilford Press.
McWilliams, N. (2004). Psychoanalytic psychotherapy: A practitioner’s guide. New York: Guilford Press.
Messer, S. C., Morris, T. L., & Gross, A. M. (1990). Hypoglycemia and psychopathol- ogy: A methodological review. Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 631–648.
Millon, T. (1981). Disorders of personality, DSM-III, Axis II. New York: Wiley. Millon, T. (1982). Millon clinical multiaxial inventory manual (2nd ed.).
Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems.
Millon, T., Grossman, S., Millon, C., Meagher, S., & Ramnath, R. (2004).
Personality disorders in modern life (2nd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Mitchell, S. A. (1988). Relational concepts in psychoanalysis: An integration.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Mitchell, S. A., & Black, M. J. (1995). Freud and beyond: A history of modern psy-
choanalytic thought. New York: Basic Books.
Morey, L. C., Waugh, M. H., & Blashfield, R. K. (1985). MMPI scales for DSM-III
personality disorders: Their derivation and correlates. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 49(3), 245–251.
Morrison, N. (2000). Schema-focused cognitive therapy for complex long-standing
problems: A single case study. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 28(3),
269–283.
Nathan, P. (1998). The DSM-IV and its antecedents: Enhancing syndromal diagno-
sis. In J. Barron (Ed.), Making diagnosis meaningful: Enhancing evaluation and treatment of psychological disorders (pp. 3–27). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Nestadt, G., Romanoski, A. J., Chalel, R., & Marchant, A. (1990). An epidemiologi- cal study of histrionic personality disorder. Psychological Medicine, 20, 413–422. Padilla, A. M. (1995). Hispanic psychology: Critical issues in theory and research.
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Perugi, G., Akiskal, H. S., Lattanzi, L., Cecconi, D., Mastrocinque, C., Patronelli, A.,
et al. (1998). The high prevalence of “soft” bipolar (II) features in atypical
depression. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 39(2), 63–71.
Pfohl, B. (1991). Histrionic personality disorder: A review of available data and rec-
ommendations for DSM-IV. Journal of Personality Disorders, 5(2), 150–166. Pfohl, B., Blum, N., Zimmerman, M., & Stangl, D. (1989). Structured interview for
personality disorders, revised version. Iowa City: University of Iowa.
Pierloot, R. A., & Ngoma, M. (1988). Hysterical manifestations in Africa and
Europe: A comparative study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 112–115.
Histrionic Personality Disorder 231

Pollak, J. M. (1981). Hysterical personality: An appraisal in light of empirical research. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 104(71–105).
Quality Assurance Project. (1991). Treatment outlines for borderline, narcissistic, and histrionic personality disorders. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 25, 392–403.
Reich, W. (1933). Charakteranalyse [Character analysis]. Leipzig, Germany: Verlag. Reich, W. (1949). Character analysis. New York: Farrar, Straus, & Giroux.
Reise, S. P., & Wright, T. M. (1996). Personality traits, Cluster B personality disor-
ders, and sociosexuality. Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 128–136. Renneberg, B., Chambless, D. L., Dowdall, D. J., Fauerbach, J. A., & Gracely, E. J. (1992). The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R, Axis II and the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory: A concurrent validity study of personal- ity disorders among anxious outpatients. Journal of Personality Disorders,
6(2), 117–124.
Rost, K. M., Akins, R. N., Brown, F. W., & Smith, G. R. (1992). The comorbidity
of DSM-III-R personality disorders in somatization disorder. General Hospital
Psychiatry, 14(5), 322–326.
Ryle, A. (1997). Cognitive analytic therapy and borderline personality disorder.
Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Sacco, J., & Olczak, P. V. (1996). Personality and cognition: Obsessivity, hystericism,
and some correlates. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 11(1), 165–176. Schotte, C., De Doncker, D., Maes, M., Cluydts, R., & Cosyns, P. (1993). MMPI assessment of the DSM-III-R histrionic personality disorder. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 60(3), 500–510.
Shapiro, D. (1965). Neurotic styles. New York: Basic Books.
Slavney, P. R. (1978). The diagnosis of hysterical personality disorder: A study of
attitudes. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 19(6), 501–507.
Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B., & Gibbon, M. (1987). Structured clinical interview for
DSM-III-R personality disorders (SCID-II). New York: New York State
Psychiatric Institute.
Spitzer, R. L, Williams, J. B. W., Gibbon, M., & First, M. B. (1990). Structured clin-
ical interview for DSM-III-R Axis II disorders (SCID-II). Washington, DC:
American Psychiatric Press.
Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B., & Skodol, A. E. (1980). DSM-III: The major achieve-
ments and an overview. American Journal of Psychiatry, 137(2), 151–164. Sprock, J. (2000). Gender-typed behavioral examples of histrionic personality disor-
der. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 22(2), 107–121. Standage, K., Bilsbury, C., Jain, S., & Smith, D. (1984). An investigation of role- taking in histrionic personalities. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 29,
407–411.
Strakowski, S. M., McElroy, S. L., Keck, P. W., & West, S. A. (1994). The co-occur-
rence of mania with medical and other psychiatric disorders. International
Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 24(4), 305–328.
Svanborg, P., Evenden-Mattila, M., Gustavsson, P. J., Uvnas-Moberg, K., & Asberg,
M. (2000). Association between plasma glucose and DSM-III-R Cluster B per-
sonality traits in psychiatric outpatients. Neuropsychobiology, 41, 79–87. Svartberg, M., Stiles, T. C., & Seltzer, M. H. (2004). Randomized, controlled trial of the effectiveness of short-term dynamic psychotherapy and cognitive therapy for Cluster C personality disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161(5),
810–817.
232
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Taylor, L. A., & Rachman, S. J. (1988). The effects of blood sugar level changes on cognitive function, affective state, and somatic symptoms. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 11, 279–291.
Torgersen, S., Lygren, S., Oien, P. A., Skre, I., Onstad, S., Edvardsen, J., et al. (2000). A twin study of personality disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 41(6), 416–425. Trull, T. J., & Widiger, T. A. (2003). Personality disorders. In G. Stricker, T. A. Widiger, & I. B. Wiener (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Clinical psychology
(Vol. 8, pp. 149–172). New York: Wiley.
Turner, R. M. (1994). Borderline, narcissistic, and histrionic personality disorders.
In M. Hersen & R. T. Ammerman (Eds.), Handbook for prescriptive treatments
for adults (pp. 393–420). New York: Pergamon Press.
Tyrer, P., Mitchard, S., Methuen, C., & Ranger, M. (2003). Treatment rejecting and
treatment seeking personality disorders: Type R and type S. Journal of Personality
Disorders, 17(3), 263–268.
Virkkunen, M. (1986). Reactive hypoglycemic tendency among habitually violent
offenders. Nutrition, 44, 94–103.
Warner, R. (1978). The diagnosis of antisocial and hysterical personality disorder.
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 166(12), 839–845.
Westen, D., & Heim, A. K. (2003). Disturbances of self and identity in personality disorders. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and iden-
tity (pp. 643–664). New York: Guilford Press.
Westen, D., & Shedler, J. (1999). Revising and assessing Axis II: II. Toward an
empirically based and clinically useful classification of personality disorders.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 273–285.
Widiger, T. A. (1998). Invited essay: Sex biases in the diagnosis of personality disor-
ders: An example of sex bias. Journal of Personality Disorders, 12(2), 95–118. Widiger, T. A., & Bornstein, R. F. (2001). Histrionic, narcissistic, and dependent personality disorders. In P. B. Sutker & H. E. Adams (Eds.), Comprehensive
handbook of psychopathology (pp. 509–531). New York: Plenum.
Widiger, T. A., & Frances, A. J. (1994). Toward a dimensional model for the per- sonality disorders. In P. T. Costa, Jr., & T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality dis- orders and the five-factor model of personality (pp. 19–39). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Widiger, T. A., & Rogers, J. H. (1989). Prevalence and comorbidity of personality
disorders. Psychiatric Annals, 19, 132–136.
Wilkinson-Ryan, T., & Westen, D. (2000). Identity disturbance in borderline per-
sonality disorder: An empirical investigation. American Journal of Psychiatry,
157, 528–541.
World Health Organization. (1993). The ICD-10 classification of mental and behav-
ioural disorders: Diagnostic criteria for research. Geneva, Switzerland: Author. Young, J. E. (1999). Cognitive therapy for personality disorders: A schema-focused
approach (3rd ed.). Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource Press.
Yovel, I., Revelle, W., & Mineka, S. (2005). Who sees the trees before the forest? The obsessive-compulsive style of visual attention. Psychological Science, 16(2),
123–129.
Zisook, S., DeVaul, R. A., & Gammon, E. (1979). The hysterical facade. American
Journal of Psychoanalysis, 39(2), 113–122.

Narcissistic 9 Personality Disorder
Kenneth N. Levy Pennsylvania State University
Joseph S. Reynoso City University of New York
Rachel H. Wasserman Pennsylvania State University
John F. Clarkin Joan and Sanford I. Weill Medical College
_________________________________________ Introduction
Clinical theorists across various orientations describe individuals diagnosed with narcissistic personality disorder as those characterized by a pervasive pat- tern of grandiosity, a sense of privilege or entitlement, an expectation of pref- erential treatment, an exaggerated sense of self-importance, and arrogant or haughty behaviors or attitudes (Westen & Shedler, 1999). Despite common
 
Authors’ notes: The authors would like to thank Janine Schiavi for her helpful com- ments regarding an earlier draft of the paper. We would also like to thank Alec Baker, Courtney Fatemi-Badi, Whitney Francis, Lindsay L. Hill, Stephane Machado, Shelia Sherazi, Camille Vaughn, and especially Jennifer Hernandez, Denise Liu, and Jill McLeod for their clerical assistance. Correspondence concerning this manuscript should be addressed to Dr. Kenneth Levy, Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania 16802. E-mail: klevy@psu.edu
233
234
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

clinical usage, the concept of narcissistic personality disorder is highly contro- versial and of uncertain validity (Frances, 1980; Maier, Lichtermann, Klingler, Heun, & Hallmayer, 1992; Siever & Klar, 1986; Vaillant & Perry, 1985). The vast majority of the literature on narcissistic personality disorder has been the- oretical and clinical rather than empirical. The research that does exist, with a few important exceptions, has not been carried out programmatically. In this chapter, we will summarize and integrate the best available scientific evidence bearing on the etiology, assessment, diagnosis, course, and treatment of the disorder. We will begin with a brief history of the concept of narcissistic per- sonality disorder, then review and evaluate a number of conceptualizations, and conclude with recommendations for further research on unresolved con- ceptual and methodological issues as we look toward DSM-V.
Brief History of the Concept _________________________
The term narcissism is derived from the Greek myth of Narcissus, who, mis- taking his own image for another, fell in love with that image and died when it failed to love him back.1 The legend of Narcissus, originally written as Homeric hymns in the seventh or eighth century BCE (Hamilton, 1942) and popularized in Ovid’s Metamorphoses (8/1958), has arisen from a relatively obscure beginning to become one of the prototypical myths of our times, with the coining of such terms as “culture of narcissism” and “me genera- tion” (Lasch, 1979; Wolfe, 1976).
Pioneering English psychologist and sex researcher Havelock Ellis (1898) first invoked this myth in a case study of a man who engaged in excessive mas- turbation to illustrate a psychological state whereby an individual becomes the object of his own sexual desire. Following Ellis, Freud first used the term “nar- cissistic” in a footnote in his 1905 paper (Freud, 1905/1957) entitled Three Essays on Sexuality in discussing the choice of sexual partners that have qual- ities similar to oneself. Thus, one of the earliest psychiatric meanings of narcis- sism had to do with sexual behavior (see Pulver, 1970, and van der Waals, 1965, for reviews). In 1911, Otto Rank wrote the first paper exclusively on narcissism, linking it to vanity and self-admiration. In 1914, Freud published On Narcissism: An Introduction (Freud, 1914/1957), in which he noted the dynamic characteristic in narcissism of consistently keeping out of awareness any information or feelings that would diminish one’s sense of self. In this paper he also discussed, from a developmental perspective, the movement from the normal but relatively exclusive focus on the self to mature relatedness. In all of these early papers, narcissism was described as a dimensional psycholog- ical state in much the same way that contemporary trait theorists describe pathological manifestations of normal traits (although Rank and Freud viewed narcissism as dynamic—that is, they saw grandiosity as a defense against feel- ing insignificant). In all these writings, narcissism was conceptualized as a process or state rather than a personality type or disorder.2
  
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 235

The concept of a narcissistic personality or character was first articulated by Waelder in 1925. Waelder described individuals with narcissistic person- ality as condescending, feeling superior to others, preoccupied with them- selves and with admiration, and exhibiting a marked lack of empathy, often most apparent in their sexuality, which is based on purely physical pleasure rather than combined with emotional intimacy. Although Freud had not discussed narcissism as a personality type in his 1914 paper, in 1931, fol- lowing Waelder, he described the narcissistic character type. In 1939, Karen Horney distinguished healthy self-esteem from pathological narcissism and suggested that the term narcissism be restricted to unrealistic self-inflation. Following this work, Jones (1955) described pathological narcissistic traits, Abraham (1924/1949) drew a link between envy and narcissism, and Reich (1960) suggested that narcissism is a pathological form of self-esteem regu- lation in which self-inflation and aggression are used to protect one’s self-concept.
In 1961, Nemiah explicitly described narcissism not only as a personal- ity type but as a disorder when he coined the term “narcissistic character disorder.” In 1967, Kernberg, as part of his articulation of borderline per- sonality organization, presented a clinical description of what he called “narcissistic personality structure.” In a later paper, Kernberg (1970) fur- ther articulated explicit descriptions of clinical characteristics and the bases for the diagnosis on readily observable behavior, distinguishing between normal and pathological narcissism. Kernberg’s (1967) initial paper was followed by a paper by Kohut (1968), who introduced the term “narcissis- tic personality disorder.” Kernberg’s (1967) and Kohut’s (1968) writings on narcissism were, in part, a reaction to increased clinical interest in treat- ing these patients. Their papers in turn stimulated increased clinical interest in the concept. However, these clinical trends also paralleled trends in crit- ical social theory (Adorno, 1967, 1968; Blatt, 1983; Horkheimer, 1936; Horkheimer & Adorno, 1944/1998; Lasch, 1979; Marcuse, 1955; Nelson, 1977; Stern, 1980; Westen, 1985; Wolfe, 1976).
Although Kohut and Kernberg disagreed on the etiology of narcissistic personality disorder, they agreed on much of its expression, particularly for those patients in the healthier range. Both of these authors have been influ- ential in shaping the concept of narcissistic personality disorder, not only among psychoanalysts but also among contemporary personality researchers and theorists (Baumeister, Bushman, & Campbell, 2000; Campbell, 1999; Dickinson & Pincus, 2003; Emmons, 1981, 1984, 1987, 1989; John & Robins, 1994; Raskin & Hall, 1979; Raskin, Novacek, & Hogan, 1991a, 1991b; Raskin & Terry, 1988; Robins & John, 1997a, 1997b; Rose, 2002; Wink, 1991, 1992a, 1992b).
Narcissistic personality disorder (NPD) was first introduced into the offi- cial diagnostic system in the third edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III; American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1980), owing to clinicians’ widespread use of the concept and the
  
236
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

identification of narcissism as a personality factor in a number of psycho- logical studies (Ashby, Lee, & Duke, 1979; Block, 1971; Cattel, Horn, Sweney, & Radcliffe, 1964; Exner, 1969, 1973; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1975; Harder, 1979; Leary, 1957; Murray, 1938; Pepper & Strong, 1958; Raskin & Hall, 1979; Serkownek, 1975; see Frances, 1980). Nevertheless, the DSM-III definition of and criteria for NPD were developed by consensus of a committee of psychiatrists and psychologists from a summary of the pre- 1978 literature, without the benefit of empirical evaluation by clinical study groups. The criteria represented amalgamations of the theoretical and clin- ical work of Kernberg (1966, 1970, 1975a), Kohut (1968, 1971), and Millon (1969), with “expert” input (see Frances, 1980, for description). DSM-III-R (APA, 1987) followed the criteria for DSM-III rather closely. However, the criteria were changed from mixed polythetic and monothetic criteria to entirely polythetic criteria. The interpersonal criterion, which had originally included four parts (entitlement, interpersonal exploitativeness, alternation between extremes of overidealization and devaluation of self and others, and lack of empathy), was reduced to three parts through the elimination of the part regarding alternation between extremes of overide- alization and devaluation of self and others. The criterion that included both grandiosity and uniqueness was split into two separate criteria, and a criterion about preoccupation with feelings of envy was added. Recently, Westen and Shedler (1999) surveyed a large group of experienced psychia- trists and psychologists of varying clinical orientations regarding the per- sonality characteristics of patients with varying personality disorders, including narcissistic personality disorder. Using factor-analytic procedures to derive an empirical profile, they found that the Axis II work groups cap- tured most of the important features of narcissistic personality disorder as seen in clinical practice. However, they noted that narcissistic patients as described by clinicians appear to be more controlling, more likely to get into power struggles, and more competitive than DSM-IV suggests. (Of course, these findings may have resulted from a referral bias, and thus DSM would be accurate.)
Prevalence _________________________________________
The prevalence of narcissistic personality disorder in the general population is estimated to be between less than 1% and 5.3%, with an estimated median rate of 0.8% and a mode of 0.0% (Black, Noyes, Pfohl, Goldstein, & Blum, 1993; Bodlund, Ekselius, & Lindström, 1993; Drake, Adler, & Vaillant, 1988; Ekselius, Tillfors, Furmark, & Fredrikson, 2001; Klein et al., 1995; Maier et al., 1992; Moldin, Rice, Erlenmeyer-Kimling, & Squires- Wheeler, 1994; Reich, Yates, & Nduaguba, 1989; Samuels, Nestadt, Romanoski, Folstein, & McHugh, 1994; Torgersen, Kringlen, & Cramer, 2001; Zimmerman & Coryell, 1990). In clinical populations, the estimated
  
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 237

prevalence has ranged from 1.3% to 17%, with prevalence rates being reported as higher in samples of personality-disordered patients (Andreoli, Gressot, Aapro, Tricot, & Gognalons, 1989; Clarkin, Levy, Lenzenweger, & Kernberg, 2004; Crosby & Hall, 1992; Dahl, 1986; Frances, Clarkin, Gilmore, Hurt, & Brown, 1984; Grilo et al., 1998; Hilsenroth, Holdwick, Castlebury, & Blais, 1998; Loranger et al., 1991; Loranger et al., 1994; McGlashan et al., 2000; Skodol, Buckley, & Charles, 1983; Zanarini, Frankenburg, Chauncey, & Gunderson, 1987).
In a survey of a community sample in Baltimore, Maryland, approxi- mately 0.1% of the sample met criteria for NPD (Samuels et al., 1994). The use of NPD as the primary clinical diagnosis is probably relatively unusual in outpatient clinic settings compared to inpatient settings. For instance, Fabrega, Ulrich, Pilkonis, and Mezzich (1992) found that less than a 0.3% of 18,179 individuals screened while seeking outpatient psychiatric evalua- tion over a 6-year period were assigned a diagnosis of NPD. Only 10 indi- viduals were given a primary diagnosis of NPD. In contrast, Grilo et al. (1998), in a consecutively admitted inpatient sample, found that 4% of ado- lescents and 6% of adults were diagnosed with NPD using structured inter- views. Clarkin et al. (2004) found that 17% of patients reliably diagnosed with borderline personality disorder were also diagnosed with NPD using a structured interview (the International Personality Disorder Examination [Loranger, 1999]). In a study of military personnel, Crosby and his col- leagues (Bourgeois, Crosby, Drexler, & Hall, 1993; Crosby & Hall, 1992) found that 20% (60 of 300) of active-duty outpatients had significant nar- cissistic features and that 4% met criteria for narcissistic personality disor- der. NPD was the most common personality disorder in this sample, representing 39% of patients with any personality disorder, and narcissis- tic traits were the most prevalent personality traits among those referred for psychiatric evaluation by superior officers. Fava, Grandi, Zielezny, Rafanelli, and Canestrari (1996) found that NPD was significantly more common in patients with early-onset depression than in those with late- onset depression.
However, given the extensive literature within psychoanalytic and psy- chotherapeutic journals, the prevalence of NPD may be higher in outpatient private practice settings than in hospital outpatient departments. For example, Westen (1997) found that 76% of 1,901 clinicians (838 psychody- namic, 300 cognitive-behavioral, and 639 eclectic) randomly selected from the American Psychiatric Association and American Psychological Association reported treating patients with NPD. Doidge et al. (2002) sur- veyed 510 psychoanalytically oriented clinicians, who reported on over 1,700 patients in the United States, Australia, and Ontario, Canada. Psychoanalysts across the three countries reported that about 20% of their patients suffered from NPD, making it the top-ranked disorder overall in the U.S. and Ontario and the second-ranked disorder overall in Australia. Westen and Arkowitz-Westen (1998) surveyed 238 experienced clinicians
  
238
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

(36.4% psychiatrists, 63.6% psychologists; 44.8% psychodynamic, 16.1% cognitive-behavioral, and 34.3% eclectic) about patients in their practices. Using a diagnostic Q-sort procedure, these clinicians evaluated 714 patients, of whom 8.5% were reported to have NPD. Psychodynamic clinicians reported that 11.2% of their patients had narcissistic personality disorder, whereas eclectic clinicians reported that 5.7% of their patients had NPD and cognitive-behavioral clinicians reported that 3.9% of their patients had NPD. This difference in rates among clinicians of different orientations may reflect greater sensitivity to the disorder by psychodynamic clinicians, an overdiagnosis of or selective attention to the disorder by psychodynamic clinicians, or different conceptualizations of the disorder. Alternatively, it may reflect relatively accurate base rates in different types of clinical prac- tices. Interestingly, DiGiuseppe, Robin, Szeszko, and Primavera (1995) reported that 107 of 742 patients (14.4%) presenting over an 18-month period met the cutoff for NPD on the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory—II (MCMI-II; Millon, 1987) in a private nonprofit cognitive- behavioral outpatient clinic in Manhattan.
In sum, the prevalence of NPD appears to vary according to clinical set- ting and type of practice. The ambiguity in defining and interpreting the cri- teria may lead some clinicians to be reluctant to use the diagnosis, whereas other clinicians, who find the clinical formulations of Kernberg, Kohut, and others useful, may make more frequent use of the diagnosis. In addition, the diagnosis is more likely to be used in private practices and small clinics than in larger psychiatric hospital clinics and inpatient units. Finally, there may be geographic differences in the rates of NPD.
Diagnosis __________________________________________
The diagnosis of NPD has been the focus of controversy since its introduction in the DSM-III. One of the major issues in this controversy has been whether NPD is a distinct diagnostic entity. Studies have generally confirmed the valid- ity of some of the overt characteristics of NPD as defined in DSM-IV, such as grandiosity, grandiose fantasy, desire for uniqueness, need for admiring atten- tion, and arrogant, haughty behavior (Morey & Jones, 1998; Ronningstam & Gunderson, 1990; Westen, 1990a). In addition to the DSM criteria set for NPD, a number of other diagnostic schemes have been developed. Gunderson and his colleagues developed the Diagnostic Interview for Narcissism (DIN; Gunderson, Ronningstam, & Bodkin, 1990), a semistructured interview that evaluates 33 characteristics of pathological narcissism. Using the DIN, Ronningstam and Gunderson (1991) found that the following characteristics could discriminate narcissistic patients from other psychiatric patients: boastful and pretentious behavior, self-centered and self-referential behavior, and the belief that other people envy them because of their special talents or unique abilities. There are a number of psychoanalytic conceptions of NPD, although those of Akhtar
  
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 239

and Thomson (1982) and Kernberg (1983) are probably the most systemati- cally described.
Beck and Freeman (1990) have proposed a cognitive model for diagnos- ing and assessing personality disorders based on the assumption that each personality disorder can be classified by the unique cognitive content of cognitive distortions and maladaptive core and conditional beliefs. The cog- nitive contents are inferred on the basis of the patients’ behaviors and traits. According to Beck and Freeman (1990), the narcissistic individual’s core beliefs include “Since I am special, I deserve special dispensations, privi- leges, and prerogatives”; “I am superior to others and they should acknowl- edge this”; and “I’m above the rule” (pp. 50–51). Nelson-Gray, Huprich, Kissling, and Ketchum (2004) examined the relationship between specific dysfunctional thought patterns (or beliefs) and personality disorder. Although specific dysfunctional thought patterns were generally related to corresponding personality disorders, most thought patterns lacked speci- ficity. For example, in addition to narcissistic thought pattern scores, histri- onic, avoidant, dependent, paranoid, and obsessive-compulsive thought pattern scores were also significantly related to NPD scores (histrionic thought pattern scores being the most highly correlated). Beck, Butler, Brown, Dahlsgaard, and Beck (2001) also reported on the relationship of dysfunctional beliefs to personality disorders. They found that narcissistic dysfunctional beliefs were higher in patients diagnosed with NPD compared to those diagnosed with avoidant, dependent, obsessive-compulsive, and paranoid personality disorders. However, they did not examine whether narcissistic dysfunctional thought patterns were higher in patients with NPD compared to histrionic, antisocial, and borderline patients, which would have provided a more stringent test of specificity. Narcissistic dysfunctional thought patterns were highly correlated with histrionic and antisocial dys- functional thought patterns. Young (1994) developed a schema-focused approach to the treatment of personality disorder by hypothesizing that personality disorders are the result of one of 18 early maladaptive schemas. Young (1998) suggested that those with NPD are characterized by three core maladaptive schemas (entitlement, emotional deprivation, and defec- tiveness) and a number of secondary schemas (e.g., approval seeking, sub- jugation, mistrust, avoidance) that are clustered into separate aspects of the self (special self, vulnerable child, and self-soother), which all alternate in reaction to changes and events in the environment. Although Young (1994) developed a measure to assess which schemas are present or active, he was not specific regarding the association between a particular schema and its corresponding personality disorder, nor has there been any research exam- ining the validity of this model.
A number of authors have recently described assessment and diagnosis of personality disorders from a radical behavioral framework (Koerner, Kohlenberg, & Parker, 1996; Nelson-Gray & Farmer, 1999; Turkat, 1990; Turkat & Maisto, 1985; Turner, 1994; van Velzen & Emmelkamp, 1996).
  
240
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Koerner et al. (1996) describe a functional analytic assessment procedure in which, in addition to patient reports of their behavior toward others, the therapist’s private reactions and feelings are central to diagnosis. They note that if a therapist feels demeaned and belittled, the patient may have fea- tures of NPD. It is interesting to note that the approach advocated by these authors is very similar to traditional psychoanalytic approaches in which clinicians are encouraged to improve their diagnostic accuracy by focusing on their own countertransference responses to patients (Gunther, 1976; Kernberg, 1975a; Wolf, 1979).3
Assessment ________________________________________
There have been several attempts to construct measures to assess narcissism. The earliest measure was a self-report questionnaire developed by Henry Murray: the Thematic Apperception Test (TAT; Murray, 1943). Broadly speaking, clinicians and researchers can draw from six information sources when assessing personality disorders: self-report inventories, rating scales and checklists, clinical interviews and ratings, projective techniques, infor- mants, and physiological measurements (neurotransmitter or hormone levels; Millon & Davis, 2000). Only the first three methods will be discussed here, as little data exist on the last three sources.
The self-report instruments for personality disorders most widely used in assessing NPD are the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory (MCMI-III; Millon, Davis, & Millon, 1997), the Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire— Revised (PDQ-R; Hyler, Kellman, Oldham, & Skodol, 1992), the Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI; Morey, 1992), and the Dimensional Assessment of Personality Pathology—Basic Questionnaire (Livesley, Reiffer, Sheldon, & West, 1987). Other personality disorder measures with narcissism scales include the Schedule of Nonadaptive and Adaptive Personality (SNAP; Clark, 1989), the OMNI Personality Inventory (OMNI; Loranger, 2000), the Personality Inventory Questionnaire (PIQ-II; Widiger, 1987), the Wisconsin Personality Disorders Inventory–IV (WIPSI-IV; Klein et al., 1993), and the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory 2—Personality Disorder Scales (MMPI 2-PD; Morey, Waugh, & Blashfield, 1985). There have been a number of self-report scales developed specifically to assess narcissism. Some have been based on DSM-III criteria. These include the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin & Hall, 1979) and the Narcissistic Personality Disorders Scale (NPDS; Ashby et al., 1979). A number of scales have been developed based on MMPI items (Ashby et al., 1979; Morey et al., 1985; Pepper & Strong, 1958; Raskin & Novacek, 1989; Serkownek, 1975; Wink & Gough, 1990). Raskin and Novacek’s scale consists of 42 items selected from the MMPI item pool, using the NPI as an empirical criterion. Two scales, Serkownek’s (1975) Narcissism-Hypersensitivity Scale and Pepper and Strong’s (1958) Ego- Sensitivity Scale, were based on factor analyses of MMPI items. Wink and
   
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 241

Gough also developed scales from the California Psychological Inventory (CPI; Gough, 1957, 1987). The most widely investigated narcissism scale is proba- bly the NPI.
There have been a number of self-report scales developed from theoreti- cal perspectives other than that of the DSM. Murray’s (1938) narcissism scale assesses the dual dynamics of self-absorption and vulnerability.4 The O’Brien Multiphasic Narcissism Inventory (O’Brien, 1987) is based on Alice Miller’s (1981) conception of narcissism, which includes a healthy authentic experience of seeing how the world relates to oneself and a more dependent, controlling, self-serving way of relating. Robbins and his colleagues devel- oped the Superiority and Goal Instability Scales based on Kohut’s (1971) theory (Robbins, 1989, Robbins & Patton, 1985, Robbins & Schwitzer, 1988). Millon’s narcissism scale from the MCMI is based on his own social learning theory. Phares and Erskine (1984) have developed a 28-item scale designed to measure the construct of “selfism” within a social-learning framework. The Bell Object Relations and Reality Testing Inventory (BOR- RTI; Bell, Billington, & Becker, 1986) includes an egocentrism scale. Other narcissism measures include the Margolis-Thomas Measure of Narcissism (MTMN; Mullins & Kopelman, 1988), the narcissistic-competitive items of the Interpersonal Checklist (ICL; LaForge & Suczek, 1955), and the Dynamic Personality Inventory (DPI; Grygier & Grygier, 1976).
Some theorists have recently suggested that the five-factor model of per- sonality may be relevant to conceptualizing personality disorders. Although this model is controversial (see Davis & Millon, 1993, and Westen, 1996, for critiques), review of a number of studies (Ball, Kranzler, Poling, Rounsaville, & Tennen, 1997; Blais, 1997; Bradlee & Emmons, 1992; Cloninger & Svrakic, 1994; Coolidge, Aksamit, & Becker, 1994; Costa & McCrae, 1990; Duggan et al., 2003; Duijsens & Diekstra, 1996; Dyce & O’Connor, 1998; Hendin & Cheek, 1997; Hyer, Matheson, Ofman, & Retzlaff, 1994; Lehne, 1994; Soldz, Budman, Demby, & Merry, 1993; Trull, 1992; Yeung, Lyons, Waternaux, Faraone, & Tsuang, 1993; see Saulsman & Page, 2004, for a review) suggests that there is a strong positive correlation between NPD and extraversion, a strong negative correlation between NPD and agreeableness, and a moderate negative correlation between NPD and conscientiousness. Findings regarding NPD’s relationship with neuroticism and openness to experience are inconsistent. Some researchers have found that NPD is posi- tively related to neuroticism, whereas others have found a negative relation- ship (Bradlee & Emmons, 1992; Buss & Chiodo, 1991). The inconsistent findings regarding NPD’s relationship to neuroticism may be related to the distinction between overt and covert narcissism (with covert narcissism being positively related to neuroticism and overt narcissism being negatively related to neuroticism). The overall picture of the narcissistic individual in the five- factor model is that of one who is extraverted yet disagreeable, and low in anxiety. This profile can be distinguished from antisocial and histrionic pat- terns, but not from a passive-aggressive one (Trull, 1992).5
  
242
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Research examining the facets underlying the five factors could provide a sharper picture of the disorder and better validity data. For instance, it is likely that within the overarching extraversion factor, the facet of dominance is related to narcissism but the facet of warmth is not. If this is so, NPD can be discriminated from histrionic personality disorder. Likewise, research examining more differentiated aspects of NPD may help to better character- ize the disorder (see Bradlee & Emmons, 1992). For example, in examining the relationship between the NPI subscales and the five-factor model’s neu- roticism, extraversion, and openness measure (NEO-PI; Costa & McCrae, 1985), Bradlee and Emmons (1992) found that the authority subscale of the NPI was positively related to the conscientiousness factor on the NEO-PI and that the superiority subscale of the NPI was positively related to the openness to experience factor on the NEO-PI. Loranger (2000), using factor analysis, found that exhibitionism, assertiveness, and ambition loaded positively on the narcissism scale and that modesty and sincerity loaded negatively.6
Shedler and Westen (2004) examined the comprehensiveness of the five- factor model compared to an expanded criteria set. Using the items restricted to the five-factor model, they could replicate the factor structure on a clinical sample. However, they found that the expanded criteria set provided a conceptually richer factor solution that did not resemble the five-factor model but instead resulted in 12 factors: psychological health, psychopathy, hostility, narcissism, emotion dysregulation, dysphoria, schizoid orientation, obsessionality, thought disorder, histrionic sexualiza- tion, dissociation, and sexual conflict. They concluded that although the five-factor model is useful for layperson descriptions of normal-range per- sonality features, it omits important clinical constructs and does not cap- ture the complexity of personality pathology.
Overall, self-report measures appear best suited either to assessing NPD at the dimensional level, particularly examining multidimensional aspects of narcissism, or as a screening measure for identifying individuals who might be likely to have a personality disorder. However, they are much less useful for identifying a specific disorder, such as NPD, due to problems with overendorsement of items, especially among distressed individuals, and the potential for underendorsement of items by defensive individuals.
There are also a number of observer-rated measures. Wink (1992a) devel- oped a California Q-Set narcissism prototype. Patton, Connor, and Scott (1982) developed 10 observer-rated scales to measure Kohut’s (1971) formu- lations of narcissism based on self-psychology, although this measure is not well researched.
There are a number of structured interviews for DSM personality disor- ders that assess NPD, including the Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality (SIDP-IV; Pfohl, Blum, & Zimmerman, 1997), Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders (SCID-II; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1995), International Personality Disorder Examination (IPDE; Loranger, 1999), Personality Disorder Interview—IV (PDI-IV; Widiger, 1995), Diagnostic Interview for Personality Disorders
  
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 243

(DIPD; Zanarini, Frankenburg, Chauncey, & Gunderson, 1987; Zanarini et al., 2000), and Personality Assessment Schedule (PAS; Tyrer et al., 1988). Two additional instruments with unique properties are the Personality Assessment Form (PAF; Shea, Glass, Pilkonis, Watkins, & Docherty, 1987) and the Shedler-Westen Assessment Procedure (SWAP; Westen & Shedler, 1999). The PAF presents a brief paragraph that describes important fea- tures of each personality disorder, and the individual’s similarity to the description is rated by an evaluator using a six-point scale. The PAF is not a structured interview in that it does not provide systematic assessment or questions for evaluation. The SWAP is a 200-item Q-set of personality- descriptive statements designed to quantify clinical judgment. Clinicians are directed to arrange the 200 items (presented on separate index cards) into eight categories with a fixed distribution, ranging from those that are not descriptive of the patient to those that are highly descriptive of the patient. One important finding with use of the SWAP is a reduction of comorbidity with other personality disorders, especially Cluster B personality disorders. This reduction in comorbidity appears important, because lack of discrete- ness of Axis II disorders has been a frequent criticism of the construct valid- ity of these disorders. Gunderson’s DIN (discussed above) appears to be the
only interview measure designed to exclusively assess NPD.
In terms of projective measures, Exner (1969) discussed the pair response as an indicator of narcissism and later developed the Egocentricity Index (EGOI) as an index of excessive self-concern. Harder (1979) constructed a projective narcissism scale for use with the Early Memory Test, the TAT, or the Rorschach. The validity of the EGOI scale as a measure of narcissism is equiv- ocal at best (Exner, 1995; Hilsenroth, Fowler, Padawer, & Handler, 1997; Nezworski & Wood, 1995), and the Harder scale has not been used widely by other investigators. A number of studies have examined Rorschach vari- ables as they relate to the diagnosis of narcissism (Berg, Packer, & Nunno, 1993; Farris, 1988; Gacono, Meloy, & Berg, 1992; Hilsenroth, Hibbard, Nash, & Handler, 1993; Hilsenroth et al., 1997). Findings from these studies are difficult to interpret; however, consistent with the writings of Kernberg and Kohut, narcissistic patients generally look healthier psycho- logically on various outcome measures than patients with borderline per-
sonality disorder.
In sum, both self-report and interview measures have satisfactory relia-
bility; however, at present the evidence for the concurrent validity of these methods to the diagnosis of NPD is only limited. In addition, there is only poor to moderate agreement across personality disorder diagnostic mea- sures and methods (Perry, 1992). Generally, self-report measures result in much higher frequencies of NPD diagnosis than do clinicians or structured interviews. Structured interviews may underdiagnose NPD because of the face validity of the questions and the tendency of individuals with the dis- order to deny pathology. Therefore, interviews that allow for the use of clinical judgment may have more validity for NPD diagnosis. Likewise, observation of behavior may be needed and more useful than self-report.
  
244 PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Some interviews specifically utilize behavioral observation. For further dis- cussion of the psychometrics of these measures, see Hilsenroth, Handler, and Blais (1996).
Subtypes __________________________________________
The definition of NPD articulated in the DSM-III and its successors, DSM- III-R, DSM-IV (APA, 1994), and DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000), has been criti- cized for failing to capture the intended clinical phenomena (Cooper & Ronningstam, 1992; Gabbard, 1989; Gunderson, Ronningstam, & Smith, 1991). These authors have noted that the DSM criteria, following the con- ceptual approaches of Kernberg and Millon, have emphasized a more overt form of narcissism. More recently, Cooper (1981), Akhtar and Thomson (1982), Gabbard (1989), and Wink (1991) have suggested that there are two subtypes of NPD: an overt form, also referred to as grandiose, oblivious, willful, exhibitionist, thick skinned, or phallic; and a covert form, also referred to as vulnerable, hypersensitive, closet, or thin skinned (Bateman, 1998; Britton, 2000; Gabbard, 1989; Masterson, 1981; Rosenfeld, 1987). The overt type is characterized by grandiosity, attention seeking, entitle- ment, arrogance, and little observable anxiety. These individuals can be socially charming despite being oblivious of others’ needs, interpersonally exploitative, and envious. In contrast, the covert narcissist is hypersensitive to others’ evaluations, inhibited, manifestly distressed, and outwardly modest. Gabbard (1989) describes these individuals as shy and “quietly grandiose,” with an “extreme sensitivity to slight” which “leads to an assiduous avoid- ance of the spotlight” (p. 527). Both types are extraordinarily self-absorbed and harbor unrealistic, grandiose expectations of themselves. This overt- covert distinction has been empirically supported in at least six studies using factor analyses and correlational methods (Dickinson & Pincus, 2003; Hendin & Cheek, 1997; Hibbard & Bunce, 1995; Rathvon & Holmstrom, 1996; Rose, 2002; Wink, 1992b). Rather than distinguishing between overt and covert types as discrete forms of narcissism, Kernberg (1992) notes that the overt and covert expressions of narcissism may be different clinical man- ifestations of the disorder, with some traits being overt and others being covert. Kernberg contends that narcissistic individuals hold contradictory views of the self, which vacillate between the clinical expression of overt and covert symptoms. Thus the overtly narcissistic individual most frequently presents with grandiosity, exhibitionism, and entitlement. Nevertheless, in the face of failure or loss, these individuals will become depressed, depleted, and feel painfully inferior. The covertly narcissistic individual will often pre- sent as shy, timid, and inhibited, but, upon closer contact, reveal exhibition- istic and grandiose fantasies.
Using cluster analysis, DiGiuseppe et al. (1995) found three clusters of narcissistic patients in an outpatient setting. They named these clusters
   
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 245

(1) the true narcissist, (2) the compensating narcissist, and (3) the detached narcissist. Patients in all three clusters exhibited self-centeredness and entitlement. However, patients in the true and detached groups reported experiencing little emotional distress. In contrast, patients in the compensat- ing group reported high levels of emotional vulnerability. The true and detached groups were similar except that the detached group was character- ized by extreme interpersonal avoidance.
Other distinctions in the expression of NPD have been noted. Bursten (1973) proposed four types of narcissistic personalities: craving individuals, who are clinging, demanding, and needy; paranoid individuals, who are critical and suspicious; manipulative individuals, who derive satisfaction from conscious and deliberate deception of others; and phallic narcissists, who are aggressive, exhibitionistic, reckless, and daring. These distinctions seem overly broad and may include other disorders but generally corre- spond to the overt-covert distinction (e.g., paranoid and phallic correspond to the grandiose type and craving corresponds to the vulnerable type). In addition, these distinctions lack the complexity and sophistication of the dynamics noted by Kernberg and others. When discussing narcissism in the context of interpersonal relations, Kohut and Wolf (1978) distinguished among “merger-hungry” individuals, who must continually attach and define themselves through others; “contact shunning” individuals, who avoid social contact due to fear that their behaviors will not be admired or accepted; and “mirror-hungry” individuals, who tend to display themselves in front of others. Millon (1998) conceptualized NPD as a prototype and distinguished among several variations, or subtypes, in which the basic per- sonality style may manifest itself. These subtypes represent configurations of a dominant basic personality style (e.g., NPD) and traits of other basic personality styles. For example, in addition to meeting criteria for NPD, his amorous subtype would show elevations in histrionic traits, his unprinci- pled subtype would show elevations in antisocial traits, and his compen- satory subtype would show elevations in avoidant and/or passive-aggressive traits. To date, little research has been performed to establish the reliability or validity of Millon’s distinctions.
Kernberg (1975a) classified narcissism along a dimension of severity from normal to pathological and distinguished among three levels of patho- logical narcissism based on the degree of differentiation and integration of representation. These three levels correspond to high, middle, and low func- tioning groups. At the highest level are those patients whose talents are ade- quate to achieve the levels of admiration necessary to gratify their grandiose needs. These patients may function successfully for a lifetime, but they are susceptible to breakdowns with advancing age as their grandiose desires go unfulfilled. At the middle level are patients with NPD proper, who present with a grandiose sense of self and little interest in true intimacy. At the low- est level are the continuum of patients who have comorbid borderline per- sonality, whose sense of self is generally more diffuse and less stable and
  
246
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

thus more frequently vacillates between pathological grandiosity and suici- dality. These individuals’ lives are generally more chaotic. Finally, Kernberg distinguished a type of NPD that he called “malignant narcissism.” These patients are characterized by the typical NPD; however, they also display antisocial behavior, tend toward paranoid features, and take pleasure in their aggression and sadism toward others. Kernberg (1992) posited that these patients are at high risk for suicide, despite the absence of depression. He suggested that suicidality for these patients represents sadistic control over others, a dismissal of a denigrated world, or a display of mastery over death. Despite the richness of Kernberg’s descriptions, we could find no direct research on malignant narcissism. It will be important to differenti- ate malignant narcissism from NPD proper (as well as from antisocial, paranoid, and borderline personality disorders) and to show that patients meeting Kernberg’s criteria for malignant narcissism are at risk for the kinds of difficulties Kernberg described.
Comorbidity _______________________________________
A number of authors have suggested the importance of understanding the co-occurrence or comorbidity of theoretically discrete disorders (Blatt & Levy, 1998; Caron & Rutter, 1991; Carson, 1991; Clarkin & Kendell, 1992; Westen & Shedler, 1999). NPD has had problematically high overlap with other Axis II disorders, most notably antisocial, histrionic, borderline, and passive-aggressive personality disorders (Becker, Edell, Grilo, & McGlashan, 2000; Gunderson, Ronningstam, & Smith, 1991; Morey, 1988; Oldham et al., 1992; Stuart et al., 1998; Widiger et al., 1991; Zanarini et al., 1998), with rates often exceeding 50%. Most patients (80%) meeting criteria for NPD also meet criteria for borderline personality disorder (Pfohl, Coryell, Zimmerman, & Stangl, 1986). Of course, the comorbidity between NPD and borderline personality disorder is not surprising, given that Kernberg’s origi- nal clinical formulations of NPD, from which many of the DSM-III criteria were adopted, were based on a selected clinical sample of patients with a pri- mary diagnosis of borderline personality disorder (Kernberg, 1967, 1970, 1975a). However, the co-occurrence or comorbidity of NPD with other dis- orders, although not unique to NPD, is a major problem in justifying and maintaining its validity as a distinct clinical entity (McGlashan & Heinssen, 1989; Morey & Jones, 1998; Ronningstam & Gunderson, 1989).
Comorbidity can be examined within samples of patients diagnosed with NPD or by examining the rates of NPD among individuals with other dis- orders. Using the former approach, Ronningstam and Gunderson (1989), in two samples of NPD patients—38 inpatients and outpatients and 34 con- secutively admitted outpatients—reported that 42% and 50% of the patients, respectively, had comorbid depressive mood disorder, 24% and 50% had comorbid substance abuse disorder, and 5% and 11% had comorbid bipolar disorder.
  
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 247

Ronningstam (1996) reviewed the available literature on concomitant NPD and Axis I disorders and found that between 12% and 38% of patients with substance use disorder and between 4% and 47% of those with bipolar disorder were diagnosed with NPD. Thus a lot of variation exists in the comorbidity between NPD and other disorders. The reasons are unclear, but one interpretation that can be distilled from these findings is that no single Axis I disorder is most often associated with NPD. A number of studies have examined the comorbidity of NPD with bipolar disorder (Bieling et al., 2003; Brieger, Ehrt, & Marneros, 2003; Carpenter, Clarkin, Glick, & Wilner, 1995; Corruble, Ginestet, & Guelfi, 1996; George, Miklowitz, Richards, Simoneau, & Taylor, 2003; Kutcher, Marton, & Korenblum, 1990; O’Connell, Mayo, & Sciutto, 1991; Pica et al., 1990; Pesslow, Sanfilipo, & Fieve, 1995; Stormberg, Ronningstam, Gunderson, & Tohen, 1998; Turley, Bates, Edwards, & Jackson, 1992). Generally, these studies found higher than average prevalence rates for NPD. These prevalence rates were much higher when the patients were actively hypomanic or manic compared to when they were euthymic, suggesting that mania should be considered in the differential diagnosis of NPD or that the criteria for NPD and mania need to be more clearly differentiated.
With regard to Axis II disorders, McGlashan and his colleagues (McGlashan et al., 2000), in conducting the Collaborative Longitudinal Personality Disorder Study, found that 8% of borderline personality disor- der patients had comorbid NPD. This percentage did not differ from the percentage of patients with comorbid schizotypal personality disorder or the percentage of those with obsessive-compulsive personality disorder but was significantly higher than the percentage of patients with comorbid avoidant personality disorder.
__________________________ Gender and Age Differences
Gender
The DSM-IV states that NPD is more common in men than in women. The analysis of gender differences in narcissism is complicated by the fact that the DSM’s definition of NPD is based on clinical descriptions of case studies of male patients (Kernberg, 1975a; Kohut, 1971, 1977).7 Consequently, several theorists have raised questions regarding whether narcissism as defined by the DSM can be generalized to women (e.g., Akhtar & Thomson, 1982; Philipson, 1985). A number of authors (Haaken, 1983; Harder & Lewis, 1987; Harder & Zalma, 1990; Hárnik, 1924; O’Leary & Wright, 1986; Philipson, 1985; Reich, 1953, 1960; Richman & Flaherty, 1988, 1990) have suggested that the distinction between covert and overt narcissism may exist along gender lines, with the grandiose type being stereotypically male and the hypersensitive type being stereotypically female. However, the empirical support for this contention remains equivocal.
  
248
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Although a number of studies have found greater prevalence rates for NPD in men (Alnaes & Torgerson, 1988; Golomb, Fava, Abraham & Rosenbaum, 1995; Grilo et al., 1996; Ronningstam & Gunderson, 1991; Stone, 1989), these findings are inconsistent with many studies failing to find gender differences in the rates of NPD (Black, Noyes, Pfohl, Goldstein, & Blum, 1993; Grilo et al., 1996; Kass, Spitzer, & Williams, 1983; Plakun, 1989; Reich, 1987; Torgersen, Kringlen, & Cramer, 2001; Zimmerman & Coryell, 1989). Torgersen et al. (2001) found no differences between men and women in the prevalence of NPD, and Grilo et al. (1996), in an adult sample, found no gender differences in NPD. Ekselius, Bodlund, Knorring, Lindstrom, and Kullgren (1996) and Richman and Flaherty (1990) found no differences between men and women in narcissism at the categorical diagnostic level; however, both groups of researchers found gender differ- ences at the criteria level. Richman and Flaherty (1990) found that men scored significantly higher on five of the six traits, whereas women scored significantly higher only on the criterion of becoming upset over slights. Ekselius et al. (1996) found gender differences on five criteria: (1) self- importance; (2) fantasies of unlimited power, success, beauty; (3) believes self to be special or only understood by special people; (4) lacks empathy; and (5) envious of others or believes others are envious of them. However, women scored higher on three of the four criteria, the exception being cri- terion 3 (believes self to be special or only understood by special people).
The findings regarding gender differences in dimensional scores on narcis- sism are also inconsistent (Auerbach, 1984; Emmons, 1984, 1987; Jackson, Ervin, & Hodge, 1992; Raskin et al., 1991a; Rhodewalt & Morf, 1995, 1998; Weirzbicki & Goldade, 1993). One somewhat consistent finding is that men score higher on average than women on dimensional measures of narcis- sism (Carroll, 1987; Farwell & Wohlwend-Lloyd, 1998; Gabriel, Critelli, & Ee, 1994; Narayan, 1990; Stangl, Pfohl, Zimmerman, Bowers, & Corenthal, 1985; Tschanz, Morf, & Turner, 1998; Watson, Grisham, Trotter, & Biderman, 1984). In a sample of predominantly Mormon men and women, Tschanz et al. (1998) found that although men and women showed highly similar patterns of narcissism, the NPI exploitativeness-entitlement factor was not as well integrated into the profile of narcissism for women. Some studies have found that women score higher on dimensional measures than men (Hynan, 2004; McCann et al., 2001). When gender differences were found, these tended to be small and of questionable meaningfulness (e.g., Buss & Chiodo, 1991; Carroll, 1987; McCann & Biaggio, 1989). In addition, it is unclear if and how gender moderates the relationship between narcissism and behavior, or other important variables, in systematic ways. In sum, the empir- ical support for gender differences remains ambiguous.
Age
The presence of narcissistic disturbances has been demonstrated in both children (Abrams, 1993; Bardenstein, 1994; Weise & Tuber, 2004) and
  
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 249

adolescents (Bernstein et al., 1993; Grilo et al., 1998; Kernberg, Hajal, & Normandin, 1998; Westen, Shedler, Durrett, Glass, & Martens, 2003). In a consecutively admitted inpatient sample of adolescents, Grilo et al. (1998) found that 4% of the inpatients were reliably diagnosed with NPD using structured interviews. Bernstein et al. (1993), in a longitudinal study, found that the rates of NPD decreased from ages 11–14 to ages 18–21. NPD has been found in the elderly (Abrams, Alexopoulos, & Young, 1987; Abrams, Rosendahl, Card, & Alexopoulous, 1994; Ames and Molinari, 1994; Berezin, 1977; Kernberg, 1977).
_____________________________________________ Etiology
The empirical data on the etiology of NPD are extremely limited. The theo- retical causes of narcissism are mainly derived from the psychodynamic object relations theories of Kernberg (1975a), Kohut (1971, 1977), and Masterson (1990). Most of the work by psychoanalytic theorists in this area has been based on inferences drawn in the clinical setting from patients’ rec- ollections of childhood family dynamics and/or analysis of how patients relate to therapists during session. More recently, Fonagy, Gergely, Jurist, and Target (2002) and Schore (1994) have discussed the development of narcissism from a psychoanalytic perspective, using a combination of object relations theory and more explicit integration of empirical findings from developmental psychology.
From an attachment theory perspective, Bowlby (1973, 1979) conceptual- ized narcissism as the result of an insecure attachment style between child and caretaker, which affects the child’s emerging self-concept and developing view of the social world. Bowlby postulated that insecure attachment lies at the center of disordered personality traits and linked the overt expression of felt insecurity to specific characterological or personality disorders. Bowlby (1979) believed that attachment difficulties increase the vulnerability to psy- chopathology and that different types of insecure attachment patterns are linked to specific types of difficulties that may arise later in development. For instance, Bowlby (1973) connected anxious ambivalent attachment to “a ten- dency to make excessive demands on others and to be anxious and clingy when they are not met, such as is present in dependent and hysterical person- alities” and avoidant attachment to “a blockage in the capacity to make deep relationships, such as is present in affectionless and psychopathic personali- ties” (p. 14). Avoidant attachment, Bowlby (1973) postulated, results from constantly being rebuffed in one’s appeals for comfort or protection, and such individuals “may later be diagnosed as narcissistic” (p. 124).
In Kernberg’s (1966, 1975a, 1975b, 1984, 1992) view, narcissism devel- ops as a consequence of parental rejection, devaluation, and an emotionally invalidating environment in which parents are inconsistent in their invest- ment in their children or often interact with their children to satisfy their own needs. For example, at times a parent may be cold, dismissive, and
  
250
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

neglectful of a child, and then at other times, when it suits the parent’s needs, be attentive and even intrusive. This parental devaluation hypothesis states that because of cold and rejecting parents, the child defensively with- draws and forms a pathologically grandiose self-representation. This self- representation, which combines aspects of the real child, the fantasized aspects of what the child wants to be, and the fantasized aspects of an ideal, loving parent, serves as an internal refuge from the experience of the early environment as harsh and depriving. The negative self-representation of the child is disavowed and not integrated into the grandiose representation, which is the seat of agency from which the narcissist operates. This split-off unacceptable self-representation can be seen in the emptiness, chronic hunger for admiration and excitement, and shame that also characterize the narcissist’s experience (Akhtar & Thomson, 1982).
What Kernberg (1975a) sees as defensive and compensatory in the estab- lishment of the narcissist’s grandiose self-representation, Kohut (1971) views as a normal development process gone awry. Kohut sees pathological narcissism as resulting from failure to idealize the parents because of rejec- tion or indifference. For Kohut, childhood grandiosity is normal and can be understood as a process by which the child attempts to identify with and become like his idealized parental figures. The child hopes to be admired by taking on attributes of perceived competence and power that he or she admires in others. In normal development, this early grandiose self eventu- ally contributes to an integrated, vibrant sense of self, complete with realis- tic ambitions and goals. However, if this grandiose self is not properly modulated, what follows is the failure of the grandiose self to be integrated into the person’s whole personality. According to Kohut, as an adult, a per- son with narcissism rigidly relates to others in “archaic” ways that befit a person in the early stages of proper self-development. Others are taken as extensions of the self (Kohut’s term is “selfobject”) and are relied upon to regulate one’s self-esteem and anxieties regarding a stable identity. Because narcissists are unable to sufficiently manage the normal fluctuations of daily life and its affective correlates, other people are unwittingly relegated to roles of providing internal regulation for them (by way of unconditional support admiration and total empathic attunement), the same way a parent would provide internal regulation for a young child.
In contrast to Kernberg and Kohut, Millon (1981) articulated an evolution- based social learning theory of narcissism. Millon sees narcissism developing not as a response to parental devaluation but rather as a consequence of parental overvaluation. According to Millon, as a child, the narcissistic individual is treated as a special person, given much attention, and led by parents to believe he or she is lovable and perfect. Millon (1981) contends that such unrealistic overvaluation will lead to self-illusions that “cannot be sustained in the outer world” (p. 165). According to Millon, firstborn and only children are more vulnerable to narcissism because they tend to receive an abundance of attention and special treatment. However, the evidence is
  
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 251

mixed regarding birth order, and there is no evidence that only-child status is related to narcissism. Two studies found that narcissism was related to firstborn status (Curtis & Cowell, 1993; Joubert, 1989); however, two studies found that narcissism was not related to birth order (Eyring & Sobelman, 1996; Narayan, 1990). Watson and Biderman (1989) also did not find any relationship between only-child status and narcissism. It is likely that the constructs of firstborn status and only-child status are not sensitive enough to account for the multiplicity of reasons that a parent may identify and treat a child as special or as threatening.
Also from a psychoanalytic perspective but attempting to bridge theory and empirical research is the promising work of Peter Fonagy (Fonagy et al., 2002). In drawing from scientific research to propose etiologic theories of NPD that conform to the most modern understanding of child develop- ment, Fonagy’s work escapes some of the heavy criticism inspired by the developmental inconsistencies implicit in Kernberg’s and Kohut’s models (Auerbach, 1993; Westen, 1985). Fonagy and his colleagues (Fonagy et al., 2002) rest their theory of affect regulation and mentalization on a social biofeedback model of development that grew out of infant observational research (Gergely & Watson, 1996). In this model, the infant’s psycholog- ical self is built through a pattern of contingency detection linked to parental affect mirroring. This process of affect mirroring provides the cog- nitive-affective scaffolding that leads to the infant’s burgeoning representa- tional capabilities. Fonagy and his colleagues posit that the pathological form of this process could make one vulnerable to the development of nar- cissistic pathology. In short, when parental affect mirroring is noncontin- gent in that the infant’s emotion is acknowledged but misperceived by the caregiver, the baby will feel related to in ways that can only be experienced as foreign. In other words, in the early development of a subjective sense of self, the baby looks toward the primary caregiver to see his or her emotional state mirrored, elaborated, and fully established, but in the case of incon- gruent parental mirroring, he or she instead finds a noncontingent “alien self” reflected. According to this theory, the narcissist is one whose core sense of agency and experience is only partially integrated into his or her total personality, since it remains obscured by the ongoing experience of the false self. Because of the lack of integration of these two competing self- representations, the narcissist lacks the range of representational capabili- ties to regulate his or her affect and hold on to a stable self-representation, which is independent of others’ perceptions of him or her.
Alan Schore (1994, 2003) focuses on the connection between neurophys- iological development and parent-infant interactional processes in relation to the etiology of pathological narcissism. He describes the normal psy- chophysiological function of shame, which begins to serve an important role after the first year of life and continues to serve as a modulator for states of high arousal and affect. With respect to narcissistic pathology, Schore posits that the initial difficulties in the caregiver-infant dyad occur
  
252
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

during the 16- to 24-month period. It is during this period, he suggests, that consistent improper parental modulating of infant affect, particularly during the infant’s heightened affect states (which he sees as a form of grandiose hyperarousal), leaves the child without the ability to tolerate negative, shame-related affect resulting from normal narcissistic injury. Instead of being able to use shame to help regulate the self, the narcissist strives to evade the feeling of shame, which he or she experiences as intolerable and overwhelming. In many cases, Schore points out, misattunement takes the form of the mother hyperstimulating the child into dyscontrol, or failing to help the child recover from states of hypoarousal which she may have her- self induced. As in Fonagy’s model, what the narcissist lacked in childhood was the proper establishment of certain autoregulatory processes, mainly achieved within the infant-caregiver dyad during the first few years of life.
It is important to note that the theories of Kernberg, Kohut, Fonagy, Bowlby, and other psychodynamic theorists (e.g., Winnicott, 1960/1965) are probabilistic and not deterministic. That is, these theorists do not suggest that narcissistic personality develops in early childhood. Rather, each of these theories proposes that the potential for the disorder begins with early disrup- tions in the relationship with caregivers, but that these initial disruptions are elaborated over time due to the consistency of the caregiver’s problematic behavior over the course of the child’s development and the consistency cre- ated by the child’s resulting expectations and behavior toward others. Fonagy’s noncontingent parent is likely to continue to interact noncontin- gently with his or her child throughout the child’s development, and the child is likely to experience ongoing difficulties in both integrating representations of the core and false selves and developing self-regulatory capacities. Likewise, Kernberg’s cold and rejecting but intrusive parent is likely to con- tinue to be cold, rejecting, and intrusive throughout the child’s development, and the child is likely to continue to withdraw from the parent and increasingly attend to and depend on his or her defensively grandiose self-representation. Thus at each nodal point in development, the child experiences narcissism- inducing environments, and his or her range of behaviors and expectations is consistent with such environments. This idea that early vulnerabilities are maintained, reinforced, and possibly elaborated with subsequent experience is consistent with a number of current developmental theories of personality formation (Bowlby, 1979; Caspi, 2000; Cicchetti & Toth, 1998; Sroufe, Egeland, Carlson, & Collins, 2005) and the integrative work of Paul Wachtel (1977, 1994). Wachtel (1977, 1994) hypothesized that personality patterns, though heavily influenced by early experiences, are in large part sustained and perpetuated through current day-to-day interactions with others. He sug- gested that narcissistic individuals enlist others as “accomplices” in recapitu- lating experiences from the past and thus self-verifying beliefs about both oneself and others (Wachtel, 1987).
In a retrospective study of early parenting styles among college under- graduates, Watson, Little, and Biderman (1992) found that the Exploitativeness/ Entitlement subscale of the NPI was negatively correlated with mature
  
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 253

authoritative parenting style and positively correlated with parental permis- siveness. In Paul Wink’s (1992b) longitudinal study of narcissism among a sample of Mills College women, he found that his group of “hypersensi- tive” (or covert) narcissists described their parental relationships as gener- ally lacking warmth and claimed feelings of insecurity toward their mothers. His “willful” (or overt) narcissists reported an attitude of dislike toward their mothers with concurrent pride in their fathers.
Wink’s (1992b) findings seem to confirm and even expand some of Block’s (1971) observations of his female “dominating narcissists” group. Block followed this group from junior high school through adulthood and found that the familial context common among these women was charac- terized by parental discord; a dominant, self-indulgent, and extraverted father; and a neurotic, somewhat dysphoric, vulnerable mother. Block sug- gests that the extremely aggressive, condescending, self-indulgent, under- controlled dominant narcissist seen in his adult sample was the product of identification with one’s detached but impressive father.
Genetic hypotheses related to the development of narcissism have not been articulated. NPD has not been assessed in any family or adoption studies, although there are some initial twin study data relevant to its etiol- ogy. Livesley, Jang, Jackson, and Vernon (1993), using the Dimensional Assessment of Personality Pathology (DAPP), reported the heritability of narcissism to be 53%. The heritability of the specific traits of need for adu- lation, attention seeking, grandiosity, and need for approval ranged from 37% for grandiosity to 50% for need for approval. Torgersen and his col- leagues (2000) examined heritability in 92 monozygotic (MZ) and 129 dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs using the SCID-II. They found 45% concordance in MZ twins and 9% in DZ twins using a broad definition of NPD (three or more criteria met). Heritability was 79%; however, this estimate was determined using broad definition diagnoses, best-fitting models did not include shared environmental effects, and interviewers assessed both twins and were not blind to zygosity status. All three of these limitations are known to inflate the estimates of genetic effects.
Two other studies examined the genetic effects of narcissism (Coolidge, Thede, & Jang, 2001, 2004). In these studies, parents were asked to assess their children’s personality disorder features using the Coolidge Personality and Neuropsychological Inventory for Children (CPNI; Coolidge, 1998; Coolidge et al., 1990, 1992; Coolidge, Thede, Stewart, & Segal, 2002). In the 2001 study, 112 twin pairs (70 MZ and 42 DZ) were assessed; findings revealed a 66% concordance in MZ twins and a 34% concordance in DZ twins. In addition, these findings yielded a 66% heritability index. The gen- eralizability of these findings is limited by three methodological shortcom- ings: (1) the effects of environment on development were not estimated, (2) the sample was not recruited representatively, and (3) personality disorder traits were rated by parents. Parents are of course not blind to zygosity, and there is evidence that they tend to overestimate trait concordance in MZ twins (Hoffman, 1991). In their 2004 study, Coolidge and his colleagues
  
254
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

attempted to reduce the influence of preconceived ideas about twin similarity by having parents complete questionnaires on different days. A wholly envi- ronmental model specifying shared and nonshared environmental influences provided the most satisfactory fit to the narcissistic personality disorder scale; no heritable influence was detected for narcissistic personality disorder.
Paris (1993) suggested that the etiology of personality disorders is unlikely to be underpinned by simple, linear, monocausal processes; com- plex interactive processes among variables are likely to be involved in the eti- ology of personality disorders. A primary difficulty is the absence of a clear definitive phenotype, which is required for the establishment of inheritance. Lack of diagnostic clarity (e.g., misdiagnosis, overlap) inevitably leads to spurious estimates of inheritance (Jang & Vernon, 2001). Given the contra- dictory findings and limitations of the study designs, it is safe to say that at present the heritability of narcissistic personality disorder is uncertain.
Last, no studies have been conducted to directly identify biological mark- ers for narcissistic personality disorder, although a number of studies have examined biological markers in near neighbor disorders, such as borderline personality disorder and antisocial personality disorder, and some studies have examined markers in vague groups that might have included narcissis- tic patients (e.g., patients with Cluster B personality disorders or those high in impulsive aggression). However, it is difficult to know if there is a specific effect for NPD.
Course and Long-Term Outcome _____________________
Data on the long-term course and outcome of NPD are sparse. There is little systematic follow-up information on this group. Plakun (1989) compared the long-term (approximately 14-year) outcome of 17 inpatients with NPD with that of 33 inpatients with borderline personality disorder and showed that the outcome of the latter was superior. NPD patients were more likely to have been readmitted and had poorer overall functioning and sexual sat- isfaction. McGlashan and Heinssen (1989) found that, over time, individu- als with NPD show decreases in destructive interpersonal behaviors. However, both McGlashan and Heinssen (1989) and Stone (1989) found no differences over time in global functioning between narcissistic and border- line patients (provided there was no antisocial comorbidity). Ronningstam, Gunderson, and Lyons (1995) examined change in narcissism over a 3-year period in 20 treated patients diagnosed with NPD. They found that the majority of their patients (60%) who initially had NPD showed significant improvement in their levels of pathological narcissism at 3-year follow-up. Although grandiosity had differentiated narcissistic patients from borderline patients in an earlier study (Ronningstam & Gunderson, 1991), it did not predict stability of the disorder over time. Patients who continued to show high levels of narcissistic pathology were significantly more narcissistic in their interpersonal relationships and lacked a commitment to anyone.
  
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 255

Ronningstam and her colleagues (1995) found some evidence that three events might have effected change in the narcissistic pathology: (1) corrective achievements, (2) corrective disillusionments, and (3) corrective relation- ships. In a 30-month follow-up of depressed outpatients, Ferro, Klein, and Schwartz (1998) also found low stability for NPD, particularly compared to other personality disorders. In fact, narcissistic personality at baseline was more highly correlated with eight other disorders than with itself at follow- up. In contrast, Grilo, Becker, Edell, and McGlashan (2001) found that nar- cissism assessed dimensionally was stable over a 2-year period. These findings may be highly sample dependent, which makes them difficult to interpret and may limit their generalizability. Some of the studies involved inpatient samples, whereas others involved outpatients. For some patients, NPD was the primary or only disorder; for other patients, NPD was a comorbid disorder (with depression or borderline personality disorder). Only the Grilo et al. (2001) study utilized consecutively admitted patients. The use of nonconsecutive samples complicates the interpretation of the data, because the study groups may be skewed in some undefined way. For example, patients agreeing to participate may be more engaged with their therapists or may be more distressed, both of which factors have been related to better outcome (Clarkin & Levy, 2003). Second, some studies examined treated patients, which is likely to have influenced the outcome.
The most extensive work on the course of narcissism has been carried out in nonclinical samples in the context of examining normal development (Block, 1971; Wink, 1992a, 1992b). In Lives Through Time, Block (1971) reported that between ages 18 and 30, women classified as “dominant nar- cissists” increased in socialization and consideration of others. However, they continued to be egotistically dominating and exploitative. Over a 20- year period, Wink (1992b) examined the relationship between narcissism and midlife development in a sample of Mills College women who graduated between 1958 and 1960. Using the California Q-Set, Wink identified three patterns of narcissism—hypersensitive, willful, and autonomous (or healthy)—that showed quite distinct patterns of personality change during the transition from college to midlife. In their early 40s, hypersensitive nar- cissists showed a course of steady decline relative to how they had been func- tioning in their early 20s. Willful narcissists showed little change at age 43 relative to age 21, after showing some growth in their late 20s. Autonomous, or healthy, narcissists, following conflict in their late 20s, experienced a surge of personality development by their early 40s, as indicated by satisfy- ing intimate interpersonal relationships and career satisfaction and successes.
_________________________________ Treatment Research
Recommendations for psychotherapeutic management of patients suffering from NPD are based primarily on clinical experience and theoretical formu- lations. These clinical case studies illustrate that some patients with NPD can
  
256
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

be treated successfully while others fail to respond to treatment. These patients are believed to be difficult to treat because they are unable to admit weaknesses, appreciate the effect their behavior has on others, or incorpo- rate feedback from others. However, no randomized controlled treatment studies on NPD exist (Groopman & Cooper, 2001; Oldham, 1988). There are a number of psychotherapy studies of patients with a specific personal- ity disorder, a subset of personality disorders, personality disorders in gen- eral, or Axis I disorders that have included patients with NPD. However, these controlled studies are difficult to interpret because they focused on mixed personality disorders without specifying narcissistic cohorts. One exception is a naturalistic study by Teusch, Böhme, Finke, and Gastpar (2001), which examined the effects of client-centered psychotherapy (CCT) on personality disorders, alone and in combination with psychopharmaco- logical treatment. They examined the effect of diagnostic subgroup of per- sonality disorder on outcome. For NPD, they found that CCT, as compared to CCT plus medication,8 led to greater reductions in depression. The authors speculated that the CCT-only group might have experienced more autonomy and self-efficacy. Given the difficulty in medicating personality- disordered patients, it is also possible that the prescribed medication regi- mens had a negative effect. Callaghan, Summers, and Weidman (2003) presented single-subject data on a patient with histrionic and NPD behaviors who was treated with functional analytic psychotherapy (Kohlenberg & Tsai, 1991). They reported significant changes in NPD behaviors during the psychotherapy sessions. Using lag sequential analysis, they linked therapist responses to in-session patient behavior. However, the researchers did not assess any external outcomes, and thus these in-session changes were not linked to any external measures of improvement. In terms of the course of psychotherapy, Hilsenroth, Fowler, & Padawer (1998), in a study of early termination in a university-based community clinic, found that NPD patients had the largest percentage of dropout (64%). In addition, the criterion “requires excessive admiration” was one of four DSM-IV criteria that signif- icantly predicted dropout. The follow-up studies of Plakun, McGlashan, Ronningstam, and Stone and their respective colleagues (Plakun, 1989; McGlashan & Heinssen, 1989; Ronningstam et al., 1995; Stone, 1989) were all carried out on treated samples and therefore bear some limited implica- tions for treatment. Generally, these studies show improvement over time in the treated samples. In Ronningstam et al.’s (1995) prospective follow-up study, retrospectively obtained information about treatment experiences suggested that treatment utilization was not differentially distributed among the patients who improved and those who did not. However, the authors acknowledged that the treatment reports were not sufficiently detailed or structured to enable one to draw valid conclusions. Pharmacological treat- ment of NPD without Axis I comorbidity has not been sufficiently studied. Abramson (1983) presented a series of case reports in which he prescribed the benzodiazepine lorazepam in adjunct to standard individual psychother- apy in the treatment of patients exhibiting what Kohut (1973) referred to as
 
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 257

“narcissistic rage.” In all three cases, lorazepam resulted in relief from ten- sions associated with feeling slighted and angry, with minimal adverse effects. However, although each case was characterized by extremely angry outbursts and rage in response to feelings of being insulted or slighted, it is not clear from the case reports that any of the patients met criteria for NPD. In addition, there are a number of important limitations of case report methodology (Raulin & Lilienfeld, 1999). Given the absence of controlled trials, lack of data in general, and the limitations of the studies carried out thus far, clinical practice guidelines for the disorder are yet to be formulated.
Narcissistic Personality ______________________________ Disorder and Suicidality
Some have suggested that suicidality in patients with NPD is high because their self-esteem is fragile (Perry, 1990). However, there is little research on suicidality and NPD. Kernberg (1992) posited that these patients are at high risk for suicide despite the absence of depression. Stone (1989) found that patients with comorbid borderline personality disorder and NPD were at higher risk for suicide than borderline patients without NPD. Apter and his colleagues (Apter, King, & Kron, 1993) conducted a postmortem study examining the diagnoses of a group of young men who had committed sui- cide. They found that over 20% of the young men had been diagnosed with NPD. Although the seminal Stone study is historically important and many findings from it have been confirmed by later research, the lack of blind, structured, and reliable interviews permits only tentative conclusions.
______________________________ Narcissism and Culture
Although the concept of narcissism and the diagnosis of NPD have been con- ceptualized in a cultural and sociopolitical context, to date there has been little research on the relationship between culture and narcissism. Lasch (1979) called narcissism the “hallmark” of American culture in which “prevailing social conditions . . . tend to bring out narcissistic traits that are present, in varying degrees, in everyone” (p. 50). He posited that the modernization of Western society, with its focus on rugged individualism, mobility, breakdown of extended family systems, and increased consumerism, has contributed to the development of a pervasive narcissistic personality style.
Rivas (2001) suggested that the exclusion of NPD from the International Classification of Diseases (ICD) may provide some indication of the signif- icantly lower prevalence of NPD in cultures outside the United States. He further suggested that this may be “evidence of its cultural entrenchment” (p. 30). However, just as some cultural styles may be mistaken for narcis- sistic personality traits, it is just as likely that some cultures may tolerate
   
258
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

narcissistic traits and therefore minimize the disorder. It is certain that the paucity of research studies sufficiently examining any existing cultural dif- ferences in the expression, etiology, or prevalence of the disorder renders it difficult to conclude anything about the generalizability of pathological narcissism outside the United States.
In contrast, some have argued that a number of culturally determined interpersonal styles can be misconstrued as narcissistic (Alarcon & Foulks, 1995; Martinez, 1993; Smith, 1990) and that narcissism may be manifested differently in other cultures (Warren & Capponi, 1996). Roland (1991) sug- gested that Western-centric norms are not reflected in Japanese culture and that narcissism would appear more covert in Japan as opposed to the exhi- bitionistic form prevalent in America. However, Sato, Sakado, and Sato (1993) found that 18.8% of 96 nonbipolar outpatients with major depres- sion in a Japanese clinic met criteria for DSM-III-R NPD (the overtly exhi- bitionistic American type). In fact, NPD was the third most common disorder overall, and the most common disorder among younger adult patients (under age 32). In contrast, Smith (1990) found fewer narcissistic traits among Asian-American women compared to Caucasian-American women (using the NPI [Raskin & Terry, 1988]). Recently, Foster, Campbell, and Twenge (2003) employed the Internet in a worldwide study of narcis- sism using the NPI. They found that self-identified White and Asian partici- pants reported less narcissism than did either Black or Hispanic participants. Respondents from the United States produced the highest levels of narcissism compared to (in descending order) Europe, Canada, Asia, and the Middle East. Consistent with the findings of Foster et al. (2003), Choca, Shanley, Peterson, and Van Denburg (1990) examined the MCMI scores of African- American and Caucasian men hospitalized at a VA hospital and found that African-Americans scored higher on the narcissism scale than the Caucasians. The relative paucity of systematic research on culture highlights the need for increased and more sophisticated work in this area.
Further Research and
Recommendations for DSM-V_________________________
Although research on NPD has generally been insufficient, there has been sig- nificant progress in the empirical research describing NPD. These data are based largely on systematic assessments of patient groups using structured clinical interviews to assess all Axis II disorders. The existing data suggest that NPD occurs frequently enough that DSM-V should continue to include it in some fashion. Nevertheless, there are a number of limitations in the existing data that should be addressed by DSM-V. First, most research on NPD has not examined the concordance between DSM-IV criteria and the essential fea- tures of the disorder as seen in clinical practice. Recent research by Westen and his colleagues (Westen & Shedler, 1999) has begun looking at this
  
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 259

process; if it is replicated, this research will suggest that DSM-V broaden the criteria set to include controlling behaviors, tendency to engage in power struggles, and competitiveness. A second issue concerns the fact that patients who meet criteria for NPD rarely do not meet criteria for another Axis II dis- order. Future research will need to discriminate NPD from other Axis II dis- orders or may suggest that there is an NPD variant of these disorders. Again, research by Westen and Shedler (1999) has shown that broadening the NPD criteria set significantly reduces the overlap with other Axis II disorders. In addition to these two issues, there is a desperate need for validation studies to determine if a NPD diagnosis predicts etiology, course, and/or treatment response. Ultimately, the value of the diagnosis will rest on whether or not it is useful for predicting adult outcomes and treatment response.
________________________________________ Conclusions
The concept of narcissistic character, disorder, and/or organization was first articulated in 1925 by Waelder and further expanded by Nemiah, Kernberg, Kohut, and Millon. NPD was introduced into the official diagnostic system in 1980 (APA), and its criteria are based heavily upon the writings of Kernberg, Kohut, and Millon.
Although there is general agreement on the trait and behavior descriptions of narcissism—focusing on the feelings of exaggerated self-importance, privilege, grandiosity, and expectation of special treatment—there is little consensus on the etiology, prevalence, assessment, and dynamics of the dis- order. Despite the official recognition of the disorder in 1980, there has been disappointingly little empirical study of its course, treatment, and out- come. Data on the naturalistic course and outcome of the disorder are sparse, with information typically based upon small or selected samples of patients followed for relatively short periods of time. In general, these studies suggest that patients with NPD improve over time. One study sug- gests there may be meaningful subgroups of these patients with differential status over time.
There are no randomized, controlled treatment studies of patients with NPD. Consistent with clinical experience, the limited data available suggest that patients with significant narcissistic traits are prone to early dropout but show some improvement in response to treatment. Given the clinical interests and documented impairment of patients with the disorder, more research on its treatment and prevention is sorely needed.
______________________________________________ Notes
1. Narcissus is a flower whose name derives is derived from the Greek word narke (where we get narcotic) by virtue of its power to alleviate pain and suffering.
    
260
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

2. In the course of his writings, Freud used the term “narcissism” to describe (1) a stage of normal infant development, (2) a normal aspect of self-interest and self- esteem, (3) a way of conducting interpersonal relationships, especially by choosing a partner based on his or her similarity to the self (overinvestment of self) rather than his or her real aspects, and (4) a way of relating to the environment characterized by a relative lack of interpersonal relations. These multiple uses of the term have resulted in significant confusion about the concept, which persists even today.
3. Kernberg discussed countertransference as one of the three main channels a therapist uses to understand a patient’s experience. Additionally, Kohlenberg and Tsai (1994) discussed the similarities between a functional analytic approach and a psychoanalytic approach.
4. Murray (1938), building on earlier theorists, such as Freud, developed a mea- sure that assessed both the grandiose self-centered aspect of narcissism and the tendency of these individuals to present this way to mask a fragile, vulnerable, hypersensitive self.
5. Passive-aggressive personality disorder is no longer included in DSM-IV, but studies of the comorbidity of DSM-III-R NPD have found it most often comorbid with passive-aggressive personality disorder (Oldham et al., 1992).
6. Hendin and Cheek (1997) found that the NPI correlated positively with extra- version and openness to experience and that hypersensitivity correlated negatively with extraversion, agreeableness, and openness to experience but positively with neu- roticism. Exploitativeness-entitlement correlated negatively with agreeableness and positively with neuroticism.
7. Of the 29 cases presented as exemplary of narcissistic personality disorder in the three major works on narcissism (Kernberg’s [1975a] Borderline Conditions and Pathological Narcissism and Kohut’s [1971, 1977] Analysis of the Self and Restoration of the Self), only five of the cases are of women.
8. Thirty-seven of the 46 patients in this condition received antidepressants and a benzodiazepine, and nine patients received low-dose neuroleptics along with either benzodiazepines or beta blockers.
References _________________________________________
Abraham, K. (1924/1949). A short study of the development of the libido, viewed in the light of mental disorders. History of the development of the libido. In Selected papers on psycho-analysis (pp. 418–479). London: Hogarth Press.
Abrams, D. M. (1993). Pathological narcissism in an eight-year-old boy: An example of Bellak’s TAT and CAT diagnostic system. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 10, 573–591.
Abrams, R. C., Alexopoulos, G. S., & Young, R. C. (1987). Geriatric depression and DSM-III-R personality disorder criteria. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 35, 383–386.
Abrams, R. C., Rosendahl, E., Card C., & Alexopoulous, G. S. (1994). Personality disorder correlates of late and early onset depression. Journal of American Geriatrics Society, 42, 727–731.
Abramson, R. (1983). Lorazepam for narcissistic rage. Journal of Operational Psychiatry, 14, 52–55.
  
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 261

Adorno, T. (1967). Sociology and psychology. New Left Review, 46, 67–80. Adorno, T. (1968). Sociology and psychology. New Left Review, 47, 79–95. Akhtar, S., & Thomson, J. A. (1982). Overview: Narcissistic personality disorder.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 12–20.
Alarcon, R. D., & Foulks, E. F. (1995). Personality disorders and culture: Contemporary
clinical views (Part A). Cultural Diversity and Mental Health, 1(1), 3–17.
Alnaes, R., & Torgerson, S. (1988). DSM-III symptom disorders (Axis I) and person- ality disorders (Axis II) in an outpatient population. Acta Psychiatrica
Scandinavica, 78, 485–492.
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (3rd ed., revised). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual
of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of men-
tal disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.
Ames, A., & Molinari, V. (1994). Prevalence of personality disorders in community-
living elderly. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 7, 189–194. Andreoli, A., Gressot, G., Aapro, N., Tricot, L., & Gognalons, M. Y. (1989). Personality disorder as a predictor of outcome. Journal of Personality Disorders,
3, 307–320.
Apter, A., King, R. A., & Kron, S. (1993). Death without warning? A clinical post-
mortem study of suicide in 43 Israeli adolescent males. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 50, 138–142.
Ashby, H. U., Lee, R. R., & Duke, E. H. (1979). A narcissistic personality
disorder MMPI scale. Paper presented at the 87th Annual Convention of the
American Psychological Association, New York.
Auerbach, J. S. (1984). Validation of two scales for narcissistic personality disorder.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 649–653.
Auerbach, J. S. (1993). The origins of narcissism and narcissistic personality disor-
der: A theoretical and empirical reformulation. In J. M. Masling & R. F. Bornstein (Eds.), Empirical studies of psychoanalytic theories: Vol. 4. Psychoanalytic perspectives on psychopathology (pp. 43–110). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Ball, S. A., Kranzler, H. R., Poling, J. C., Rounsaville, B. J., & Tennen, H. (1997). Personality, temperament, and character dimensions and the DSM-IV personality disorders in substance abusers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 545–553.
Bardenstein, K. (1994). Narcissistic character disorder in children: Rorschach features. Presented at the annual meeting of The Society for Personality Assessment, Chicago. Bateman, A. W. (1998). Thick- and thin-skinned organizations and enactment in borderline and narcissistic disorders. International Journal of Psychoanalysis,
79, 13–25.
Baumeister, R. F., Bushman, B. J., & Campbell, W. K. (2000). Self-esteem, narcis-
sism, and aggression: Does violence result from low self-esteem or from threat-
ened egotism? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 26–29.
Beck, A. T., Butler, A. C., Brown, G. K., Dahlsgaard, K. K., & Beck, J. S. (2001). Dysfunctional beliefs discriminate personality disorders. Behavior Research and
  
Therapy, 39, 1213–1225.
262
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Beck, A. T., & Freeman, A. M. (1990). Cognitive therapy of personality disorders. New York: Guilford Press.
Becker, D. F., Edell, W. S., Grilo, C. M., & McGlashan, T. H. (2000). Comorbidity of borderline personality disorder with other personality disorders in hospital- ized adolescents and adults. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 2011–2016.
Bell, M., Billington, R., & Becker, B. (1986). A scale of the assessment of object rela- tions: Reliability, validity, and factorial invariance. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42, 733–741.
Berezin, M. A. (1977). Normal psychology of the aging process, revisited: II. The fate of narcissism in old age: Clinical case reports. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 10, 9–26.
Berg, J., Packer, A., & Nunno, V. (1993). A Rorschach analysis: Parallel disturbance in thought and in self/object representation. Journal of Personality Assessment, 61, 311–323.
Bernstein, D. P., Cohen, P., Velez, C. N., Schwab-Stone, M., Siever, L. J., & Shinsato, L. (1993). Prevalence and stability of the DSM-III-R personality disorders in a com- munity-based survey of adolescents. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1237–1243.
Bieling, P. J., MacQueen, G. M., Marriott, M. J., Robb, J. C., Begin, H., Joffe, R. T., et al. (2003). Longitudinal outcome in patients with bipolar disorder assessed by life charting is influenced by DSM-IV personality disorder symptoms. Bipolar Disorders, 5, 14–21.
Black, D. W., Noyes, R., Pfohl, B., Goldstein, R. B., & Blum, N. (1993). Personality disorder in obsessive-compulsive volunteers, well comparison subjects, and their first-degree relatives. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1226–1232.
Blais, M. A. (1997). Clinician ratings of the five-factor model of personality and the DSM-IV personality disorders. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 185, 388–393.
Blatt, S. J. (1983). Narcissism and egocentrism as concepts in individual and cultural development. Psychoanalysis & Contemporary Thought, 6, 291–303.
Blatt, S. J., & Levy, K. N. (1998). A psychodynamic approach to the diagnosis of psychopathology. In J. W. Barron (Ed.), Making diagnosis meaningful: Enhancing evaluation and treatment of psychological disorders (pp. 73–109). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Block, J. (1971). Lives through time. Berkeley, CA: Bancroft Books.
Bodlund, O., Ekselius, L., & Lindström, E. (1993). Personality traits and disorders among psychiatric outpatients and normal subjects on the basis of the SCID
screen questionnaire. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 47, 425–433.
Bourgeois, J. J., Crosby, R. M., Drexler, K. G., & Hall, M. J. (1993). An examina- tion of narcissistic personality traits as seen in a military population. Military
Medicine, 158, 170–174.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Separation: Anxiety and anger. New York: Basic Books. Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. London: Tavistock. Bradlee, P. M., & Emmons, R. A. (1992). Locating narcissism within the interpersonal
circumplex and the five-factor model. Personality and Individual Differences, 13,
821–830.
Brieger, P., Ehrt, U., & Marneros, A. (2003). Frequency of comorbid personality dis-
orders in bipolar and unipolar affective disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 44, 28–34.
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 263

Britton, R. (2000). Hyper-subjectivity and hyper-objectivity in narcissistic disorders.
Fort Da: The Journal of the Northern California Society for Psychoanalytic
Psychology, 6, 18–23.
Bursten, B. (1973). Some narcissistic personality types. International Journal of
Psychoanalysis, 54, 287–300.
Buss, D. M., & Chiodo, L. M. (1991). Narcissistic acts in everyday life. Journal of
Personality, 59, 179–215.
Callaghan, G. M., Summers, C. J., & Weidman, M. (2003). The treatment of histri-
onic and narcissistic personality disorder behaviors: A single-subject demonstra- tion of clinical improvement using functional analytic psychotherapy. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 33, 321–339.
Campbell, K. W. (1999). Narcissism and romantic attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1254–1270.
Caron, C., & Rutter, M. (1991). Comorbidity in child psychopathology: Concepts, issues and research strategies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 32, 1063–1080.
Carpenter, D., Clarkin, J. F., Glick, I. D., & Wilner, P. J. (1995). Personality pathol- ogy among married adults with bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 34, 269–274.
Carroll, L. (1987). A study of narcissism, affiliation, intimacy, and power motives among students in business administration. Psychological Reports, 61(2), 355–358. Carson, R. C. (1991). Dilemmas in the pathway of the DSM-IV. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 100, 302–307.
Caspi, A. (2000). The child is father of the man: Personality continuities from
childhood to adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78,
158–172.
Cattell, R. B., Horn, J. L., Sweney, A. B., & Radcliffe, J. A. (1964). Handbook for
the Motivation Analysis Test (MAT). Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality
and Ability Testing.
Choca, J. P., Shanley, L. A., Peterson, C. A., & Van Denburg, E. (1990). Racial bias
and the MCMI. Journal of Personality Assessment, 54, 479–490.
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (1998). The development of depression in children and
adolescents. American Psychologist, 53, 221–241.
Clark, L. A. (1989). Depressive and anxiety disorders: Descriptive psychopathology
and differential diagnosis. In P. C. Kendall & D. Watson (Eds.), Anxiety and depression: Distinctive and overlapping features (pp. 83–129). New York: Academic Press.
Clarkin, J. F., & Kendall, P. C. (1992). Comorbidity and treatment planning. Journal of Clinical and Consulting Psychology, 60, 904–908.
Clarkin, J. F., & Levy, K. N. (2003). A psychodynamic treatment for severe person- ality disorders: Issues in treatment development. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 23, 248–267.
Clarkin, J. F., Levy, K. N., Lenzenweger, M. F., & Kernberg, O. F. (2004). The Personality Disorders Institute/Borderline Personality Disorder Research Foundation randomized control trial for borderline personality disorder: Rationale, methods, and patient characteristics. Journal of Personality Disorders, 18, 52–72.
Cloninger, C. R., & Svrakic, D. M. (1994). Differentiating normal and deviant per- sonality by the seven-factor personality model. In M. Lorr & S. Strack (Eds.),
264
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Differentiating normal and abnormal personality (pp. 40–64). New York:
Springer.
Coolidge, F. L. (1998). Coolidge personality and neuropsychological inventory for
children: CPNI. Colorado Springs, CO: Author.
Coolidge, F. L., Aksamit, C. R., & Becker, L. A. (1994). Prediction of recidivism in
juvenile offenders. Indian Journal of Psychology, 2, 1–6.
Coolidge, F. L., Philbrick, P. B., Wooley, M. J., Bunting, E. K., Hyman, J. N., &
Stager, M. A. (1990). The KCATI: Development of an inventory for the assess- ment of personality disorders in children. Journal of Personality and Clinical Studies, 6, 225–232.
Coolidge, F. L., Reilman, B. J., Becker, L. A., Cass, V. J., Coolidge, R. L., & Stocker, R. (1992). Emotional problems and neuropsychological symptoms in juvenile non-violent offenders. Journal of Personality and Clinical Studies, 8(1), 7–13.
Coolidge, F. L., Thede, L. L., & Jang, K. E. (2001). Heritability of personality disor- ders in childhood: A preliminary investigation. Journal of Personality Disorders, 15, 33–40.
Coolidge, F. L., Thede, L. L., & Jang, K. E. (2004). Are personality disorders psy- chological manifestations of executive function deficits? Behavior Genetics, 34, 75–84.
Coolidge, F. L., Thede, L. L., Stewart, S. E., & Segal, D. L. (2002). The Coolidge Personality and Neuropsychological Inventory for Children (CPNI): Preliminary psychometric characteristics. Behavioral Modification, 26, 550–566.
Cooper, A. (1981). Narcissism. In S. Arieti, H. Keith, & H. Brodie (Eds.), American handbook of psychiatry (Vol. 4, pp. 297–316). New York: Basic Books.
Cooper, A., & Ronningstam, E. (1992). Narcissistic personality disorder. In A. Tasman & M. B. Riba (Eds.), American Psychiatric Press review of psychia- try (Vol. 11). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Corruble, E., Ginestet, D., & Guelfi, J. D. (1996). Comorbidity of personality dis- orders and unipolar major depression: A review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 37, 157–170.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1990). Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality. Journal of Personality Disorders, 4, 362–371.
Crosby, R. M., & Hall, M. J. (1992). Psychiatric evaluation of self-referred and non- self-referred active duty military members. Military Medicine, 157, 224–229.
Curtis, J. M., & Cowell, D. R. (1993). Relation to birth order and scores on mea- sures of pathological narcissism. Psychological Reports, 72, 311–315.
Dahl, A. A. (1986). Some aspects of the DSM-III personality disorders illustrated by a consecutive sample of hospitalized patients. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 73, 61–67.
Davis, R. D., & Millon, T. (1993). The five-factor model for personality disorders: Apt or misguided? Psychological Inquiry, 4, 104–109.
Dickinson, K. A., & Pincus, A. L. (2003). Interpersonal analysis of grandiose and vulnerable narcissism. Journal of Personality Disorders, 17, 188–207.
DiGiuseppe, R., Robin, M., Szeszko, P. R., & Primavera, L. (1995). Cluster analy- ses of narcissistic personality disorders on the MCMI-II. Journal of Personality Disorders, 9, 304–317.
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 265

Doidge, N., Simon, B., Brauer, L., Grant, D. C., First, M., Brunshaw, J., et al. (2002). Psychoanalytic patients in the U.S., Canada, and Australia: I. DSM-III-R, pre- vious treatment, medications, and length of treatment. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 50, 575–614.
Drake, R. E., Adler, D. A., & Vaillant, G. E. (1988). Antecedents of personality disor- ders in a community sample of men. Journal of Personality Disorders, 2, 60–68.
Drake, R. E., & Vaillant, G. E. (1988). Introduction: Longitudinal views of person- ality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 2, 44–48.
Duggan, C., Milton, J., Egan, V., McCarthy, L., Palmer, B., & Lee, A. (2003). Theories of general personality and mental disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 182, 19–23.
Duijsens, I. J., & Diekstra, R. F. W. (1996). DSM-III-R and ICD-10 personality dis- orders and their relationship with the Big Five dimensions of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 119–133.
Dyce, J. A., & O’Connor, B. P. (1998). Personality disorders and the five-factor model: A test of facet-level predictions. Journal of Personality Disorders, 12, 31–45. Ekselius, L., Bodlund, O., Knorring, L. von, Lindstrom, E., & Kullgren, G. (1996).
Sex differences in DSM-III-R, Axis II: Personality disorders. Personality and
Individual Differences, 20(4), 457–461.
Ekselius, L., Tillfors, M., Furmark, T., & Fredrikson, M. (2001). Personality disorders
in the general population: DSM-IV and ICD-10 defined prevalence as related to
sociodemographic pro file. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 311–320. Ellis, H. (1898). Auto-eroticism: A psychological study. Alienist and Neurologist, 19,
260–299.
Emmons, R. A. (1981). Relationship between narcissism and sensation seeking.
Psychological Reports, 48, 247–250.
Emmons, R. A. (1984). Factor analysis and construct validity of the narcissistic per-
sonality inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 291–300.
Emmons, R. A. (1987). Narcissism: Theory and measurement. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 52, 11–17.
Emmons, R. A. (1989). The personal striving approach to personality. In L. A. Pervin
(Ed.), Goal concepts in personality and social psychology (pp. 87–126).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Exner, J. (1969). Rorschach responses as an index of narcissism. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 33, 324–330.
Exner, J. (1973). The self-focus sentence completion: A study of egocentricity.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 37, 437–455.
Exner, J. (1995). Comment on “Narcissism in the comprehensive system for the
Rorschach.” Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 2, 200–206.
Eyring, W. E., III, & Sobelman, S. (1996). Narcissism and birth order. Psychological
Reports, 78, 403–406.
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1975). Eysenck Personality Questionnaire man-
ual. San Diego, CA: Educational Testing Service.
Fabrega, H., Ulrich, R., Pilkonis, P., & Mezzich, J. (1992). Pure personality disorders
in an intake psychiatric setting. Journal of Personality Disorders, 6, 153–161. Farris, M. (1988). Differential diagnosis of borderline and narcissistic personality dis- orders. In H. Lerner & P. Lerner (Eds.), Primitive mental states and the
Rorschach (pp. 299–338). New York: International Universities Press.
  
266
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Farwell, L., & Wohlwend-Lloyd, R. (1998). Narcissistic processes: Optimistic expec- tations, favorable self-evaluations, and self-enhancing attributions. Journal of Personality, 66, 65–83.
Fava, G. A., Grandi, S., Zielezny, M., Rafanelli, C. & Canestrari, R. (1996). Four- year outcome for cognitive behavioral treatment of residual symptoms in major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 945–947.
Ferro, T., Klein, D. N., & Schwartz, J. E. (1998). 30-month stability of personality disorder diagnosis in depressed outpatients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 653–659.
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1995). Structured clin- ical interview for DSM-IV. New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute.
Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E. L., & Target, M. (2002). Affect regulation, men- talization and the development of the self. New York: Other Press.
Foster, J. D., Campbell, W. K., & Twenge, J. M. (2003). Individual differences in narcissism: Inflated self-views across the lifespan and around the world. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 469–486.
Frances, A. J. (1980). The DSM-III personality disorder section: A commentary. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 1439–1448.
Frances, A., Clarkin, J. F., Gilmore, M., Hurt, S. W., & Brown, R. (1984). Reliability of criteria for borderline personality disorder: A comparison of DSM-III and the diagnostic interview for borderline patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 1080–1084.
Freud, S. (1905/1957). Three essays on sexuality. In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 7, pp. 125–245). London: Hogarth Press.
Freud, S. (1914/1957). On narcissism: An introduction. In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 14, pp. 73–102). London: Hogarth Press.
Freud, S. (1931/1950). Libidinal types. In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 21, pp. 217–220). London: Hogarth Press.
Gabbard, G. (1989). Two subtypes of narcissistic personality disorder. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 53, 527–532.
Gabriel, M. T., Critelli, J. W., & Ee, J. S. (1994). Narcissistic illusions in self-evalu- ations of intelligence and attractiveness. Journal of Personality, 62, 143–155.
Gacono, C., Meloy, J., & Berg, J. (1992). Object relations, defensive operations, and affective states in narcissistic, borderline, and antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Personality Assessment, 59, 32–49.
George, E. L., Miklowitz, D. J., Richards, J. A., Simoneau, T. L., & Taylor, D. O. (2003). The comorbidity of bipolar disorders and Axis II personality disorders: Prevalence and clinical correlates. Bipolar Disorders, 5, 115–122.
Gergely, G., & Watson, J. S. (1996). The social biofeedback theory of parental affect mirroring. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 77, 1181–1212.
Golomb, M., Fava, M., Abraham, M., & Rosenbaum, J. F. (1995). Gender differ- ences in personality disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 579–582.
Gough, H. G. (1957). Manual for the California Psychological Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Gough, H. G. (1987). The California Psychological Inventory administrator’s guide. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 267

Grilo, C. M., Becker, D. F., Edell, W. S., & McGlashan, T. H. (2001). Stability and change of DSM-III-R personality disorder dimensions in adolescents followed up 2 years after psychiatric hospitalization. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 42, 364–368.
Grilo, C. M., Becker, D. F., Fehon, D. C., Walker, M. L., Edell, W. S., & McGlashan, T. H. (1996). Gender differences in personality disorders in psychiatrically hos- pitalized adolescents. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 1089–1091.
Grilo, C. M., McGlashan, T. H., Quinlan, D. M., Walker, M. L., Greenfeld, D., & Edell, W. S. (1998). Frequency of personality disorders in two age cohorts of psychiatric inpatients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 140–142.
Groopman, L. C., & Cooper, A. M. (2001). Narcissistic personality disorder. In G. O. Gabbard (Ed.), Treatments of psychiatric disorders (3rd ed., pp. 2309–2326). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Grygier, J. G., & Grygier, P. (1976). Dynamic personality inventory. Montreal, Quebec, Canada: Institute of Psychological Research.
Gunderson, J. G., Ronningstam, E., & Bodkin, A. (1990). The diagnostic interview for narcissistic patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47(7), 676–680.
Gunderson, J. G., Ronningstam, E., & Smith, L. E. (1991). Narcissistic personality disorder: A review of data on DSM-III-R descriptions. Journal of Personality Disorders, 5, 167–177.
Gunther, M. S. (1976). The endangered self: A contribution to the understanding of narcissistic determinants of countertransference. Annual of Psychoanalysis, 4, 201–224.
Haaken, J. (1983). Sex differences and narcissistic disorders. American Journal of Psychoanalysis, 43, 315–324.
Hamilton, E. (1942). Mythology. Boston: Little, Brown.
Harder, D. W. (1979). The assessment of ambitious-narcissistic character style with
three projective tests: The Early Memories, TAT, and Rorschach. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 43, 23–32.
Harder, D. W., & Lewis, S. J. (1987). The assessment of shame and guilt. In
J. N. Butcher & C. D. Spielberger (Eds.), Advances in personality assessment
(Vol. 6., pp. 89–114). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Harder, D. W., & Zalma, A. (1990). Two promising shame and guilt scales: A con-
struct validity comparison. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55, 729–745. Hárnik, J. (1924). The various developments undergone by narcissism in men and in
women. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 5, 66–83.
Hendin, H. M., & Cheek, J. M. (1997). Assessing hypersensitive narcissism: A reex- amination of Murray’s narcissism scale. Journal of Research in Personality, 31,
588–599.
Hibbard, S., & Bunce, S. C. (1995, August). Two paradoxes of narcissism. Paper
presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association,
New York.
Hilsenroth, M. J., Fowler, J. C., & Padawer, J. R. (1998). The Rorschach
Schizophrenia Index (SCZI): An examination of reliability, validity, and diag-
nostic efficiency. Journal of Personality Assessment, 70, 514–534.
Hilsenroth, M. J., Fowler, J. C., Padawer, J. R., & Handler, L. (1997). Narcissism in the Rorschach revisited: Some reflections on empirical data. Psychological
Assessment, 9, 113–121.
Hilsenroth, M., Handler, L., & Blais, M. (1996). Assessment of narcissistic personal-
ity disorder: A multimethod review. Clinical Psychology Review, 16, 655–683.
268
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Hilsenroth, M., Hibbard, S., Nash, M., & Handler, L. (1993). A Rorschach study of narcissism, defense, and aggression in borderline, narcissistic and Cluster C per- sonality disorders. Journal of Personality Assessment, 60, 346–361.
Hilsenroth, M., Holdwick, D., Castlebury, F., & Blais, M. (1998). The effects of DSM-IV Cluster B personality disorder symptoms on the termination and con- tinuation of psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 35, 163–176.
Hoffman, L. (1991). The influence of the family environment on personality: Accounting for sibling differences. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 187–203.
Horkheimer, M. (1936). Studien über Autorität und Familie [Studies of authority and family]. Schriften des Instituts für Sozialforschung, 5, 947.
Horkheimer, M., & Adorno, T. W. (1944/1998). The dialectic of enlightenment (J. Cumming, Trans.). New York: Continuum.
Horney, K. (1939). New ways in psychoanalysis. New York: Horton.
Hyer, L., Matheson, S., Ofman, P., & Retzlaff, P. D. (1994). MCMI-II high-point codes: Severe personality disorder and clinical syndrome extensions. Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 50, 228–234.
Hyler, S. E., Kellman, H. D., Oldham, J. M., & Skodol, A. E. (1992). Validity of the
Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire—Revised: A replication in an outpatient
sample. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 33, 73–77.
Hynan, D. J. (2004). Unsupported gender differences on some personality disorder
scales of the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory—III. Professional Psychology:
Research and Practice, 35, 105–110.
Jackson, L. A., Ervin, K. S., & Hodge, C. N. (1992). Narcissism and body image.
Journal of Research in Personality, 26, 357–370.
Jang, K. L., & Livesley, W. J. (1999). Why do measures of normal and disordered
personality correlate? A study of genetic comorbidity. Journal of Personality
Disorders, 13, 10–17.
Jang, K. L., & Vernon, P. A. (2001). Genetics. In W. J. Livesley (Ed.), Handbook of
personality disorder: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 177–195). New
York: Guilford Press.
John, O. P., & Robins, R. W. (1994). Accuracy and bias in self-perception:
Individual differences in self-enhancement and the role of narcissism. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 206–219.
Jones, E. (1955). The life and work of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 2.). New York: Basic Books. Joubert, C. E. (1989). Birth order and narcissism. Psychological Reports, 64, 721–722. Kass, F., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. (1983). An empirical study of the issue of
sex bias in the diagnostic criteria of DSM-III Axis II personality disorders.
American Psychologist, 38, 799–801.
Kernberg, O. (1966). Structural derivatives of object relationships. International
Journal of Psychoanalysis, 47, 236–253.
Kernberg, O. F. (1967). Borderline personality organization. Journal of the American
Psychoanalytic Association, 15, 641–685.
Kernberg, O. (1970). Factors in the psychoanalytic treatment of narcissistic person-
alities. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 18, 51–85. Kernberg, O. (1975a). Borderline conditions and pathological narcissism. New
York: Jason Aronson.
Kernberg, O. (1975b). Further contributions to the treatment of narcissistic person-
alities: A reply to the discussion by Paul H. Ornstein. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 56, 245–247.
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 269

Kernberg, O. F. (1977). The structural diagnosis of borderline personality organiza- tion. In P. Hartocollis (Ed.), Borderline personality disorders: The concept, the syndrome, the patient. New York: International Universities Press.
Kernberg, O. F. (1983). Objects relations theory and character analysis. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 31, 247–271.
Kernberg, O. (1984). The treatment of severe character disorders. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Kernberg, O. (1992). Aggression in personality disorders and its perversions. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Kernberg, P. F., Hajal, F., & Normandin, L. (1998). Narcissistic personality disorder in adolescent inpatients: A retrospective record review study of descriptive char- acteristics. In E. F. Ronningstam (Ed.), Disorders of narcissism: Diagnostic, clinical, and empirical implications (pp. 437–456). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Klein, D. N., Riso, L. P., Donaldson, S. K., Schwartz, J. E., Anderson, R. L., Ouimette, P. C., et al. (1995). Family study of early-onset dysthymia: Mood and personal- ity disorders in relatives of outpatients with dysthymia and episodic major depression and normal controls. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52, 487–496.
Klein, M. H., Benjamin, L. S., Rosenfeld, R., Treece, C., Husted, J., & Greist, J. H. (1993). The Wisconsin Personality Disorders Inventory: I. Development, relia- bility, and validity. Journal of Personality Disorders, Suppl., 18–33.
Koerner, K., Kohlenberg, J. R., & Parker, C. R. (1996). Diagnosis of personality dis- order: A radical behavioral alternative. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 1169–1176.
Kohlenberg, R., & Tsai, M. (1991). Functional analytic psychotherapy: Creating intense and curative therapeutic relationships. New York: Plenum.
Kohlenberg, R. J., & Tsai, M. (1994). Functional analytic psychotherapy: A behav- ioral approach to treatment and integration. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 4, 175–201.
Kohut, H. (1968). The psychoanalytic treatment of narcissistic personality disorders: Outlineofasystematicapproach. PsychoanalyticStudyoftheChild,23,86–113. Kohut, H. (1971). The analysis of the self. New York: International Universities Press. Kohut, H. (1973). Reflections on narcissism and narcissistic anger. Psyche:
Zeitschrift für Psychoanalyse und ihre Anwendungen, 27, 513–554.
Kohut, H. (1977). The restoration of the self. New York: International Universities Press. Kohut, H., & Wolf, E. S. (1978). The disorders of the self and their treatment: An
outline. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 59, 413–425.
Kutcher, S. P., Marton, P., & Korenblum, M. (1990). Adolescent bipolar illness and personality disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry, 29, 355–358.
LaForge, R., & Suczek, R. F. (1955). The interpersonal dimension of personality: III.
An interpersonal check list. Journal of Personality, 24, 94–112.
Lasch, C. (1979). The culture of narcissism: American life in an age of diminishing
expectations. New York: Warner Books.
Leary, T. (1957). Interpersonal diagnosis of personality. New York: Ronald Press. Lehne, G. K. (1994). The NEO–PI and the MCMI in the forensic evaluation of sex
offenders. In P. T. Costa, Jr., & T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality (pp. 175–188). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  
270
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Livesley, W. J., Jang, K. L., Jackson, D. N., & Vernon, P. A. (1993). Genetic and environmental contributions to dimensions of personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1826–1831.
Livesley, W. J., Reiffer, L. I., Sheldon, A. E. R., & West, M. (1987). Prototypicality ratings of DSM-III criteria for personality disorders. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 395–401.
Loranger, A. W. (1999). International personality disorder examination: DSM-IV and ICD-10 interviews. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Loranger, A. W. (2000). Personality disorders: General considerations. In M. G.
Gelder, J. J. Lopez-lbor, & N. Andreasen (Eds.), The New Oxford textbook of
psychiatry (Vol. 1, pp. 923–926). New York: Oxford University Press. Loranger, A. W., Lenzenweger, M. F., Gartner, A. F., Susman, V. L., Herzig, J., Zammit, G. K., et al. (1991). Trait-state artifacts and the diagnosis of personal-
ity disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 720–728.
Loranger, A. W., Sartorius, N., Andreoli, A., Berger, P., Buchheim, P., Channabasavanna, S. M., et al. (1994). The international personality dis- order examination: The World Health Organization/Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration international pilot study of personality disorders.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 215–224.
Maier, W., Lichtermann, D., Klingler, T., Heun, R., & Hallmayer, J. (1992).
Prevalences of personality disorders (DSM-III-R) in the community. Journal of
Personality Disorders, 6, 187–196.
Marcuse, H. (1955). Eros and civilization. New York: Vintage Books.
Martinez, C. (1993). Psychiatric care of Mexican-Americans. In A. C. Gaw (Ed.),
Culture, ethnicity, and mental illness (pp. 431–466). Washington, DC: American
Psychiatric Press.
Masterson, J. F. (1981). The narcissistic and borderline disorders. New York: Brunner/ Mazel. Masterson, J. F. (1990). Psychotherapy of borderline and narcissistic disorders:
Establishing a therapeutic alliance (A developmental, self, and object relations
approach). Journal of Personality Disorders, 4, 182–191.
McCann, J. T., & Biaggio, M. K. (1989). Narcissistic personality features and self-
reported anger. Psychological Reports, 64, 55–58.
McCann, J. T., Flens, J. R., Campagna, V., Collman, P., Lazzaro, T., & Connor, E.
(2001). The MCMI–III in child custody evaluations: A normative study. Journal
of Forensic Psychology Practice, 1, 27–44.
McGlashan, T. H., Grilo, C. M., Skodol, A. E., Gunderson, J. G., Shea, M. T.,
Morey, L. C., et al. (2000). The Collaborative Longitudinal Personality Disorders Study: Baseline Axis I/II and II/II diagnostic co-occurrence. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 102, 256–264.
McGlashan, T. H., & Heinssen, R. K. (1989). Narcissistic, antisocial, and non- comorbid subgroups of borderline disorder: Are they distinct entities by long- term clinical profiles? Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 12, 621–641.
Miller, A. (1981). The drama of the gifted child. New York: Basic Books.
Millon, T. (1969). Modern psychopathology. Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland. Millon, T. (1981). Disorders of personality: DSM III, Axis II. New York: Wiley. Millon, T. (1987). Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory—II manual. Minneapolis,
MN: National Computer Systems.
Millon, T. (1998). DSM narcissistic personality disorder: Historical reflections and
future directions. In E. F. Ronningstam (Ed.), Disorders of narcissism: Diagnostic,
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 271

clinical, and empirical implications (pp. 75–101). Washington, DC: American
Psychiatric Association.
Millon, T., & Davis, R. (2000). Personality disorders in modern life. New York: Wiley. Millon, T., Davis, R., & Millon, C. (1997). The Millon Clinical Multiaxial
Inventory-III manual. Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems. Moldin, S. O., Rice, J. P., Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L., & Squires-Wheeler, S. ( 1 9 9 4 ) . Latent structure of DSM-III-R, Axis II psychopathology in a normal sample.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 259–266.
Morey, L. C. (1988). Personality disorders in DSM-III and DSM-III-R: Convergence,
coverage, and internal consistency. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 573–577. Morey, L. C. (1992). Personality assessment inventory: Spanish translation. Odessa,
FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Morey, L. C., & Jones, J. K. (1998). Empirical studies of the construct validity of
narcissistic personality disorder. In E. F. Ronningstam (Ed.), Disorders of nar- cissism: Diagnostic, clinical, and empirical implications (pp. 351–373). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Morey, L. C., Waugh, M. H., & Blashfield, R. K. (1985). MMPI scales for the DSM- III personality disorders: Their derivation and correlates. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 245–251.
Mullins, L. S., & Kopelman, R. E. (1988). Toward an assessment of the construct valid- ity of four measures of narcissism. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52, 610–625. Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in personality: A clinical and experimental study
of fifty men of college age. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Murray, H. A. (1943). Thematic Apperception Test manual. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Narayan, C. (1990). Birth order and narcissism. Psychological Reports, 67,
1184–1186.
Nelson, K. (1977). The syntagmatic-paradigmatic shift revisited: A review of research
and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 93–116.
Nelson-Gray, R. O, & Farmer, R. F. (1999). Behavioral assessment of personality
disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 347–368.
Nelson-Gray, R. O., Huprich, S. K., Kissling, G. E., & Ketchum, K. R. (2004). A pre- liminary examination of Beck’s cognitive theory of personality disorders in undergraduate analogues. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 219–233. Nemiah, J. C. (1961). Foundations of psychopathology. Oxford, UK: Oxford
University Press.
Nezworski, T., & Wood, J. (1995). Narcissism in the comprehensive system for the
Rorschach. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 2, 179–199.
O’Brien, M. L. (1987). Examining the dimensionality of pathological narcissism: Factor analysis and construct validity of the O’Brien Multiphasic Narcissism
Inventory. Psychological Reports, 61, 499–510.
O’Connell, R. A., Mayo, J. A., & Sciutto, M. S. (1991). PDQ-R personality disor-
ders in bipolar patients. Journal of Affective Disorders, 23, 217–221.
Oldham, J. (1988). Brief treatment of narcissistic personality disorder. Journal of
Personality Disorders, 2, 88–90.
Oldham, J. M., Skodol, A. E., Kellman, H. D., Hyler, S. E., Rosnick, L., & Davies,
M. (1992). Diagnosis of DSM-III-R personality disorders by two structured interviews: Patterns of comorbidity. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 213–220.
  
272
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

O’Leary, J., & Wright, F. (1986). Shame and gender issues in pathological narcis- sism. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 3, 327–339.
Ovid. (8/1958). The metamorphoses (H. Gregory, Trans.). New York: Viking Press. Paris, J. C. (1993). Borderline personality disorder: Etiology and treatment.
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Patton, M., Connor, G., & Scott, K. (1982). Kohut’s psychology of the self: Theory and
measures of counseling outcome. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 29, 268–282. Pepper, L. J., & Strong, P. N. (1958). Judgmental subscales for the MF scale of the
MMPL. Unpublished manuscript.
Perry, J. C. (1990). Personality disorders and suicide and self-destructive behavior.
In D. Jacobs & H. Brown (Eds.), Suicide: Understanding and responding.
Madison, CT: International Universities Press.
Perry, J. C. (1992). Problems and considerations in the valid assessment of personal-
ity disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 1645–1653.
Pesslow, E. D., Sanfilipo, M. P., & Fieve, R. R. (1995). Relationship between hypo- mania and personality disorders before and after successful treatment. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 232–238.
Pfohl, B., Blum, N., & Zimmerman, M. (1997). Structured interview for DSM per-
sonality. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Pfohl, B., Coryell, W., Zimmerman, M., & Stangl, D. (1986). DSM-III personality
disorders: Diagnostic overlap and internal consistency of individual DSM-III cri-
teria. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 27, 21–34.
Phares, E. J., & Erskine, N. (1984). The measurement of selfism. Educational and
Psychological Measurement, 44, 597–608.
Philipson, I. (1985). Gender and narcissism. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 9,
213–228.
Pica, S., Edwards, J., Jackson, H. J., Bell, R. C., Bates, G. W., & Rudd, R. P. (1990).
Personality disorders in recent-onset bipolar disorder. Comprehensive
Psychiatry, 31, 499–510.
Plakun, E. M. (1989). Narcissistic personality disorder: A validity study and compar-
ison to borderline personality disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of North America,
12(3), 603–620.
Pulver, S. (1970). Narcissism: The term and the concept. Journal of the American
Psychoanalytic Association, 18, 319–341.
Rank, O. (1911). Ein beitrag zum narcissmus [A contribution to narcissism]. Jahrbuch
fur Psychoanalytische und Psychopathologische Forschungen, 3, 410–423. Raskin, R., & Hall, C. S. (1979). A narcissistic personality inventory. Psychological
Reports, 40, 590.
Raskin, R., & Novacek, J. (1989). An MMPI description of the narcissistic person-
ality. Journal of Personality Assessment, 53, 66–80.
Raskin, R., Novacek, J., & Hogan, R. (1991a). Narcissism, self-esteem, and defen-
sive self-enhancement. Journal of Personality, 59, 20–38.
Raskin, R., Novacek, J., & Hogan, R. (1991b). Narcissistic self-esteem management.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 911–918.
Raskin, R., & Terry, H. (1988). A principal-components analysis of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory and further evidence for its construct validity. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 890–902.
Rathvon, N., & Holmstrom, R. W. (1996). An MMPI–2 portrait of narcissism.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 66, 1–19.
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 273

Raulin, S. and Lilienfeld, S. (1999). Research strategies for studying psychopathol- ogy. In T. Millon, P. H. Blaney, & R. D. Davis (Eds.), Oxford textbook of psy- chopathology (Vol. 4, pp. 49–78). New York: Oxford University Press.
Reich, A. (1953). Narcissistic object choice in women. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 1, 22–44.
Reich, A. (1960). Pathologic forms of self-esteem regulation. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 18, 218–238.
Reich, J. (1987). Sex distribution of DSM-III personality disorders in psychiatric out- patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 485–488.
Reich, J., Yates, W., & Nduaguba, M. (1989). Prevalence of DSM-III personality dis- orders in the community. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 24, 12–16.
Rhodewalt, F., & Morf, C. C. (1995). Self and interpersonal correlates of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory. Journal of Research in Personality, 29, 1–23. Rhodewalt, F., & Morf, C. C. (1998). On self-aggrandizement and anger: A tempo- ral analysis of narcissism and affective reactions to success and failure. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 672–685.
Richman, J. A., & Flaherty, J. A. (1988). “Tragic man” and “tragic woman:” Gender
differences in narcissistic styles. Psychiatry, 51, 368–377.
Richman, J. A., & Flaherty, J. A. (1990). Gender differences in narcissistic styles. In
E. Plakun (Ed.), New perspectives on narcissism (pp. 73–100). Washington, DC:
American Psychiatric Association.
Rivas, L. A. (2001). Controversial issues in the diagnosis of narcissistic personality dis-
order: A review of the literature. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 23, 22–35. Robbins, S. B. (1989). Validity of the Superiority and Goal Instability Scales as mea-
sures of defects in the self. Journal of Personality Assessment, 53, 122–132. Robbins, S. B., & Patton, M. J. (1985). Self-psychology and career development: Construction of the Superiority and Goal Instability Scales. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 32, 221–231.
Robbins, S. B., & Schwitzer, A. M. (1988). Validity of the Superiority and Goal
Instability Scales as predictors of women’s adjustment to college life.
Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 21, 117–123. Robins, R. W., & John, O. P. (1997a). The quest for self-insight: Theory and research on the accuracy of self-perceptions. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. R. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 649–679). San Diego, CA: Academic
Press.
Robins, R. W., & John, O. P. (1997b). Effects of visual perspective and narcissism
on self-perception: Is seeing believing? Psychological Science, 8, 37–42. Roland, A. (1991). In search of self in India and Japan: Toward a cross-cultural
psychology. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Ronningstam, E. (1996). Pathological narcissism and narcissistic personality
disorder in Axis I disorders. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 3, 326–340. Ronningstam, E., & Gunderson, J. (1989). Descriptive studies on narcissistic person-
ality disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 12, 585–601. Ronningstam, E., & Gunderson, J. G. (1990). Identifying criteria for narcissistic
personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 918–922. Ronningstam, E., & Gunderson, J. (1991). Differentiating borderline personality disorder from narcissistic personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders,
5, 225–232.
274
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Ronningstam, E., Gunderson, J. G., & Lyons, M. (1995). Changes in pathological narcissism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 253–257.
Rose, P. (2002). The happy and unhappy faces of narcissism. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 379–392.
Rosenfeld, H. A. (1987). Impasse and interpretation: Therapeutic and anti-therapeutic factors in the psychoanalytic treatment of psychotic, borderline, and neurotic patients. London: Tavistock and the Institute of Psycho-Analysis.
Samuels, J. F., Nestadt, G., Romanoski, A. J., Folstein, M. F., & McHugh, P. R. (1994). DSM-III personality disorders in the community. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 1055–1062.
Sato T., Sakado, K., & Sato, S. (1993). DSM-III-R personality disorders in outpatients with non-bipolar depression: The frequency in a sample of Japanese and the rela- tionship to the 4-month outcome under adequate antidepressant therapy. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 242, 273–278.
Saulsman, L. M., & Page, A. C. (2004). The five-factor model and personality dis- order empirical literature: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 23, 1055–1085.
Schore, A. N. (1994). Affect regulation and the origin of the self: The neurobiology of emotional development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Schore, A. (2003). Affect regulation and disorders of the self. New York: W. W. Norton. Serkownek, K. (1975). Subscales for scale 5 and O of the MMPI. Unpublished man-
uscript.
Shea, M. T., Glass, D., Pilkonis, P. A., Watkins, J., & Docherty, J. P. (1987).
Frequency and implications of personality disorders in a sample of depressed
outpatients. Journal of Personality Disorders, 1, 27–42.
Shedler, J., & Westen, D. (2004). Refining personality disorder diagnosis: Integrating
science and practice. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 1350–1365.
Siever, L., & Klar, H. (1986). A review of DSM-III criteria for the personality disor- ders. In A. Frances & R. Hales (Eds.), American Psychiatric Association annual review (Vol. 5, pp. 299–301). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
Association.
Skodol, A. E., Buckley, P., & Charles, E. (1983). Is there a characteristic pattern in
the treatment history of clinic outpatients with borderline personality? Journal
of Nervous and Mental Disease, 171, 405–410.
Smith, B. M. (1990). The measurement of narcissism in Asian, Caucasian, and
Hispanic American women. Psychological Reports, 67(3, Pt. 1), 779–785. Soldz, S., Budman, S., Demby, A., & Merry, J. (1993). Representation of personal- ity disorders in circumplex and five-factor space: Explorations with a clinical
sample. Psychological Assessment, 5, 41–52.
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E., & Collins, W. A. (2005). The development
of the person: The Minnesota Study of Risk and Adaptation from Birth to
Adulthood. New York: Guilford Press.
Stangl, D., Pfohl, B., Zimmerman, M., Bowers, W., & Corenthal, C. (1985). A struc-
tured interview for the DSM-III personality disorders: A preliminary report.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 591–596.
Stern, P. N. (1980). Grounded theory methodology: Its uses and processes. Image,
12, 20–23.
Stone, M. H. (1989). Long-term follow-up of narcissistic/borderline patients.
Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 12, 621–641.
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 275

Stormberg, D., Ronningstam, E., Gunderson, J., & Tohen, M. (1998). Pathological narcissism in bipolar disorder patients. Journal of Personality Disorders, 12, 179–185.
Stuart, S., Pfohl, B., Battaglia, M., Bellodi, L., Grove, W., & Cadoret, R. (1998). The coocurrence of DSM-III-R personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 12(4), 302–315.
Teusch, L, Böhme, H., Finke, J., & Gastpar, M. (2001). Effects of client-centered psychotherapy for personality disorders alone and in combination with psy- chopharmacological treatment: An empirical follow-up study. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 70, 328–336.
Torgersen, S., Kringlen, E., & Cramer, V. (2001). The prevalence of personality dis- orders in a community sample. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 590–596.
Torgersen, S., Lygren, S., Oien, A., Skre, I., Onstad, S., Edvardson, J., et al. (2000). A twin study of personality disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 41, 416–425. Trull, T. J. (1992). DSM-III-R personality disorders and the five-factor model of per- sonality: An empirical comparison. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101,
553–560.
Tschanz, B. T., Morf, C. C., & Turner, C. M. (1998). Gender differences in the struc-
ture of narcissism: A multi-sample analysis of the Narcissistic Personality
Inventory. Sex Roles, 38, 863–870.
Turkat, I. D. (1990). The personality disorders: A psychological approach to clinical
management. Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press.
Turkat, I. D., & Maisto, S. A. (1985). Personality disorders: Applications of the
experimental method to the formulation and modification of personality disor- ders. In D. H. Barlow (Ed.), Clinical handbook of psychological disorders: A step-by-step treatment manual (pp. 503–570). New York: Guilford Press.
Turley, B., Bates, G. W., Edwards, J., & Jackson, H. J. (1992). MCMI-II diagnosis in recent-onset bipolar disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 48, 320–329. Turner, R. M. (1994). Borderline, narcissistic, and histrionic personality disorders. In M. Hersen & R. T. Ammerman (Eds.), Handbook for prescriptive treatments
for adults (pp. 393–420). New York: Plenum.
Tyrer, P., Seivewright, N., Murphy, S., Ferguson, B., Kingdon, D., Barczak, P., et al.
(1988). The Nottingham study of neurotic disorder: Comparison of drug and
psychological treatments. Lancet, 8605, 235–240.
Vaillant, G. E., & Perry, J. C. (1985). Personality disorders. In H. I. Kaplan &
B. J. Sadock (Eds.), Comprehensive textbook of psychiatry/IV (4th ed., Vol. 1,
pp. 958–986). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
van Velzen, C. J. M., & Emmelkamp, P. M. G. (1996). The assessment of personal-
ity disorder: Implications for cognitive and behavior therapy. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 34, 655–668.
van der Waals, H. G. (1965). Problems of narcissism. Bulletin of the Menninger
Clinic, 29, 293–311.
Wachtel, P. L. (1977). Psychoanalysis and behaviour therapy: Toward an integra-
tion. New York: Basic Books.
Wachtel, P. (1987). Action and insight. New York: Guilford Press.
Wachtel, P. L. (1994). Cyclical processes in personality and psychopathology.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 51–54.
Waelder, R. (1925). The psychoses: Their mechanisms and accessibility to influence.
  
International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 6, 259–281.
276
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Warren, M. P., & Capponi, A. (1996). The role of culture in the development of narcissistic personality disorders in America, Japan, and Denmark. Journal of Applied Social Sciences, 20, 77–82.
Watson, P. J., & Biderman, M. D. (1989). Failure of only-child status to predict nar- cissism. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 69, 1346.
Watson, P. J., Grisham, S. O., Trotter, M. V., & Biderman, M. D. (1984). Narcissism and empathy: Validity evidence for the narcissistic personality inven- tory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 301–305.
Watson, P. J., Little, T., & Biderman, M. D. (1992). Narcissism and parenting styles. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 9(2), 231–244.
Weirzbicki, M., & Goldade, P. (1993). Sex-typing of the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory. Psychological Reports, 72, 1115–1121.
Weise, K., & Tuber, S. (2004). The self and object representations of narcissistically disturbed children: An empirical investigation. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 21, 244–258.
Westen, D. (1985). Self and society: Narcissism, collectivism, and the development of morals. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Westen, D. (1990a). The relations among narcissism, egocentrism, self-concept, and self-esteem. Psychoanalysis & Contemporary Thought, 13, 185–241.
Westen, D. (1990b). Toward a revised theory of borderline object relations: Implications of empirical research. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 71, 661–693. Westen, D. (1996). A model and a method for uncovering the nomothetic from the
idiographic: An alternative to the five-factor model? Journal of Research in
Personality, 30, 400–413.
Westen, D. (1997). Divergences between clinical and research methods for assessing
personality disorders: Implications for research and the evolution of Axis II.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 895–903.
Westen, D., & Arkowitz-Westen, L. (1998). Limitations of Axis II in diagnosing per-
sonality pathology in clinical practice. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155,
1767–1771.
Westen, D., & Shedler, J. (1999). Revising and assessing Axis II: I. Developing a clin-
ically meaningful and empirically valid assessment method. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 156, 258–272.
Westen, D., Shedler, J., Durrett, C., Glass, S., & Martens, A. (2003). Personality
diagnosis in adolescence: DSM-IV Axis II diagnoses and an empirically derived
alternative. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 952–966.
Widiger, T. A. (1987). Personality interview questions—revised. Unpublished man-
uscript, University of Kentucky, Lexington.
Widiger, T. A. (1995). Deletion of the self-defeating and sadistic personality disorder
diagnoses. In W. J. Livesley (Ed.), The DSM-IV personality disorders (pp. 359–373).
New York: Guilford Press.
Widiger, T. A., Frances, A., Harris, M., Jacobsberg, I., Fyer, M., & Manning, D.
(1991). Comorbidity among Axis II disorders. In J. Oldham (Ed.), Personality disorders: New perspectives on diagnostic validity (pp. 163–194). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Wink, P. (1991). Two faces of narcissism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 590–597.
Wink, P. (1992a). Three narcissism scales for the California Q-Set. Journal of Personality Assessment, 58, 51–66.
  
Narcissistic Personality Disorder 277

Wink, P. (1992b). Three types of narcissism in women from college to mid-life. Journal of Personality, 60, 7–30.
Wink, P., & Gough, H. G. (1990). New narcissism scales for the California Psychological Inventory and MMPI. Journal of Personality Assessment, 54, 446–463.
Winnicott, D. W. (1960/1965). Ego distortions in terms of true and false self. In D. W. Winnicott, The maturational processes and the facilitating environment (pp. 140–152). New York: International Universities Press.
Wolf, E. S. (1979). Transferences and countertransferences in the analysis of disor- ders of the self. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 15, 577–594.
Wolfe, T. (1976, August 23). The “me” decade and the third great awakening. New York Magazine, 26–40.
Yeung, A. S., Lyons, M. J., Waternaux, C. M., Faraone, S. V., & Tsuang, M. T. (1993). A family study of self-reported personality traits and DSM-III-R person- ality disorders. Psychiatry Research, 48, 243–255.
Young, J. E. (1994). Cognitive therapy for personality disorders: A schema-focused approach (Rev. ed.). Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource Press.
Young, J. E. (1998). The Young schema questionnaire: Short form. Retrieved November 10, 1999, from http://.home.sprynet.com/sprynet/schema/ysqs1.htm
Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Chauncey, D. L., & Gunderson, J. G. (1987). The Diagnostic Interview for PDs: Interrater and test-retest reliability. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 28, 467–480.
Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Dubo, E. D., Sickel, A. E., Trikha, A., Levin, A., et al. (1998). Axis I comorbidity of borderline personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 1733–1739.
Zimmerman, M., & Coryell, W. (1989). DSM-III personality disorder diagnoses in a nonpatient sample. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 682–689.
Zimmerman, M., & Coryell, W. H. (1990). DSM-III personality disorder dimen- sions. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 178, 686–692.
  

10
Avoidant Personality Disorder
James D. Herbert Drexel University

Homo sapiens is a highly gregarious species. It would be difficult to overstate the importance of interpersonal relationships to our psy- chological functioning. Serious disruption of relationships with others can lead to a cascade of psychological and even medical problems. Avoidant personality disorder (APD) is a condition characterized by just such disruption in interpersonal functioning. According to the fourth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994), the cardinal features of APD include social inhibition, feelings of inadequacy, and fear of neg- ative evaluation. These concerns lead individuals with APD to avoid a wide range of social situations, especially those involving close interac- tions with others. Pervasive social anxiety and avoidance result in a cir- cumscribed social world, with relatively few contacts outside of a small number of trusted confidants.
Like all personality disorders, APD begins in childhood or adolescence and is believed to follow a chronic, unremitting course without intervention. As discussed later, there is growing evidence that APD represents a severe variant of the generalized subtype social anxiety disorder. APD is thought to be among the most common of the personality disorders. In a large commu- nity sample, Torgersen, Kringlen, and Cramer (2001) found a prevalence rate of 5.0%; Ekselius, Tillfors, Furmark, and Fredrikson (2001) found a similar rate of 6.6% in a community sample. APD is also common in clini- cal samples. For example, Stuart et al. (1998) reported that 25% of a large sample of patients met criteria for APD.
279
280
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Given its high prevalence and the ubiquity of social concerns in normal psychological functioning, it is not surprising that questions have been raised regarding whether APD represents a distinct diagnostic entity. Before review- ing the literature on the etiology and treatment of APD, I will examine the diagnostic status of the disorder.
Does APD Represent a Distinct Disorder? ______________
A considerable body of research has examined the extent to which the symp- toms of APD comprise a distinct diagnostic entity. Shyness, social inhibition and anxiety, interpersonal reticence, and social avoidance are common fea- tures of normal personality functioning as well as of many clinical conditions. In fact, the absence of some degree of social anxiety is itself often considered problematic (Frances, First, & Pincus, 1995). It is therefore not surprising that questions have been raised about the degree to which APD is distin- guishable from other clinical conditions on the one hand and normal person- ality functioning on the other. One way in which this question has been addressed is by examining the overlap of APD with other clinical conditions.1 Research reveals high diagnostic co-occurrence between APD and various anxiety disorders, including panic disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, and of course social anxiety disorder, as dis- cussed later; co-occurrence is likewise quite high with other Axis I disorders, including mood disorders, somatoform disorders, eating disorders, and body dysmorphic disorder (see Alden et al., 2002, for a review). APD also shows substantial overlap with certain Axis II personality disorders, particularly schizoid and dependent personality disorders (e.g., Stuart et al., 1998).
Although high degrees of diagnostic co-occurrence may raise questions about distinctiveness, the fact that two disorders frequently co-occur does not ipso facto preclude their representing distinct conditions. Many examples of highly co-occurring yet distinct disorders can be found in medicine (e.g., diabetes and coronary heart disease). To represent a distinct disorder, a set of symptoms must be shown not only to covary across indi- viduals, but also to be distinct from more “basic” conditions. A disease in turn requires the recognition of a distinct pathogenic process, etiology, or both (Kazdin, 1983). In fact, despite high rates of diagnostic co-occurrence, an argument could be made that APD is distinct from most of the other con- ditions just described. Consider, for example, schizoid personality disorder (SPD). Although both disorders are associated with social withdrawal, the avoidant individual desperately desires social contact but fears rejection, whereas the schizoid is ambivalent at best toward others, preferring to live as a loner; this distinction was first made by Millon (1969). In fact, research demonstrates that the differential diagnosis of APD and SPD is rarely prob- lematic (Trull, Widiger, & Frances, 1987). Similarly, although APD shares
   
Avoidant Personality Disorder 281

some features in common with depression, panic disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder, it is both conceptually and empirically distinct from each of these conditions. However, there is one condition from which it is less clear that APD is actually distinct.
______________________ APD and Social Anxiety Disorder
One of the most vexing problems with APD is its overlap with social anxi- ety disorder (SAD), also known as social phobia.2 In fact, more research has addressed the distinction of these putatively separate disorders than any other issue relating to APD.
The two disorders evolved from unique historical traditions. Over the various incarnations of the DSM, however, their descriptions have con- verged, to the point that there is now considerable overlap in their diag- nostic criteria. In fact, despite minor differences in wording, comparison of the diagnostic criteria reveals substantive overlap. Not surprisingly, as discussed later, research has demonstrated high diagnostic co-occurrence between the two disorders.
A number of studies have attempted to establish meaningful distinc- tions between the two conditions (e.g., Herbert, Hope, & Bellack, 1992; Holt, Heimberg, & Hope, 1992; Turner, Beidel, & Townsley, 1992). The consistent finding has been that whereas patients diagnosed with APD demonstrate more severe symptoms than those with generalized SAD and without APD, they do not differ qualitatively on theoretically important variables that would support the notion of distinct disorders. Moreover, psychotherapy trials reveal that although patients with APD begin and end treatment with more severe symptoms than those with SAD, the rate of improvement is comparable (Chambless, Tran, & Glass, 1997; Feske, Perry, Chambless, Renneberg, & Goldstein, 1996; Hope, Herbert, & White, 1995; Mersch, Jansen, & Arntz, 1995; Turner, Beidel, Cooley, Woody, & Messer, 1994). The most parsimonious con- clusion is that APD and generalized SAD reflect the same spectrum of psychopathology (i.e., pathological social anxiety and avoidance) rather than distinct disorders.
Most scholars of anxiety disorders have in fact endorsed this conclu- sion (e.g., Craske, 1999; Heimberg & Becker, 2002; Herbert & Dalrymple, 2005; Hoffman, Heinrichs, & Moscovitch, 2004; McNeil, 2001; Turner et al., 1992). The implication, of course, is that the two diagnoses should somehow be combined in future revisions of the DSM. Heimberg (1996), Liebowitz et al. (1998), and Reich (2001) argued that APD should be subsumed within generalized SAD. These authors sug- gested, among other things, that clinicians may be more likely to employ
  
282
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

pharmacotherapy or an empirically supported psychotherapy, such as cognitive-behavioral therapy, for an anxiety disorder than they would for a personality disorder. Personality disorder scholars, however, have been more reluctant to concede that the two disorders are largely the same and offer a number of arguments in favor of retaining APD as a dis- tinct diagnosis from SAD, or even folding the generalized subtype of SAD into APD.
Five arguments have been advanced in favor of this perspective. First, the high degree of overlap of the disorders may be more apparent than real, and may represent an artifact of sampling characteristics. Second, the psy- chopathology described by APD fits better with the characteristics of a per- sonality disorder than it does with an Axis I syndrome. Third and closely related, APD is thought to be more temporally stable than one would expect an Axis I disorder to be. Fourth, subsuming APD within generalized SAD would simply shift existing diagnostic problems to Axis I rather than solve them. Fifth and finally, eliminating APD would discourage basic personality research into the disorder. I discuss each of these arguments, along with counterarguments, next.
Overlap Between APD and Generalized SAD. Several studies reveal high rates of APD among patients diagnosed with generalized SAD. However, Arntz (1999) pointed out that the majority of these studies examined the presence of APD in samples of persons with social phobia. He argued that a true assessment of the overlap of APD and SAD also requires examination of the rates of SAD in samples diagnosed with APD, as well as consideration of the rates of each disorder in reference to other disorders. That is, it is possible that APD is as common in another anxi- ety disorder as it is in SAD. Using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R Personality Disorders (Spitzer, Williams, & Gibbon, 1987), Jansen, Arntz, Merckelbach, and Mersch (1994) found that the rates of APD did not differ between a group of social phobics and panic disorder patients, although the former did have more avoidant traits. In an unpub- lished master’s thesis, Tacken (1998) administered structured clinical interviews assessing both Axis I and Axis II disorders to a sample of 515 patients. Consistent with other studies, approximately half of the patients diagnosed with generalized SAD also met criteria for APD. If APD in fact represents a severe variant of SAD, one would expect all patients with APD to also meet criteria for SAD. However, among the 109 patients with APD, only 56 (51%) had generalized SAD, 12 (11%) had nongener- alized SAD, and 41 (38%) did not meet criteria for SAD at all. Arntz argued that these two studies support the distinctiveness of APD and SAD. However, neither of these studies definitively refutes the possibility that APD and generalized SAD reflect the same underlying spectrum of psychopathology. Much appears to ride on how one interprets the specific criteria that define each disorder, and the APD criteria in particular leave considerable room for interpretation. The criteria for each disorder are of
  
Avoidant Personality Disorder 283

course different; hence, one would not expect 100% overlap between them. The important question is whether the differences are meaningful or rather simply reflect different aspects of the same behavioral, cognitive, and affective themes. In addition, in keeping with the hierarchical orga- nization in the DSM in which a separate diagnosis of a “lesser” disorder is not made if the symptoms can be attributed to a more severe disorder, it is possible that Tacken (1998) did not make a diagnosis of SAD if APD was also present in some cases, even if the criteria for both disorders were technically met.
Distinctive Features of Personality Disorders. Widiger (2003) argued that personality disorders are distinguished by three key features: early onset, chronic course, and ego-syntonic features (i.e., features that are viewed as consistent with one’s identity) that reflect “problems with everyday functioning” (p. 103). It should be noted that ego-syntonicity implies only that the characteristics are accepted as part of one’s identity. For many personality disorders, including APD, the individual may nev- ertheless view these features as undesirable or problematic. In contrast, Axis I disorders are thought to have variable onsets, episodic courses, and ego-dystonic symptoms that represent extreme departures from normal- ity. Because the psychopathology described by APD has an onset in child- hood or adolescence, tends to be chronic and unremitting without treatment, and is characterized by interpersonal symptoms disrupting routine functioning, it is held to be more consistent with the notion of personality disorder. Of course, generalized SAD also shares each of these factors. Moreover, it is not clear that the characteristics suggested by Widiger reliably distinguish Axis I and Axis II disorders in general. Schizophrenia, for example, would appear to meet each of these criteria, as would early-onset dysthymia, both of which are coded on Axis I. Although one might argue that the more extreme depressive symptoms of dysthymia and the psychotic symptoms of schizophrenia are not ego-syn- tonic and go beyond “problems with everyday functioning,” could not the same be said for the extreme affective lability and parasuicidal behav- ior of borderline personality disorder?
Temporal Stability. A closely related argument is that personality disor- ders in general, and APD in particular, are more stable than the major psy- chiatric syndromes. A recent study by Shea and Yen (2003), however, raises serious questions about this common belief. These authors compared the temporal stability of personality disorders (including APD), mood disorders, and anxiety disorders by examining the results of three naturalistic longitu- dinal studies. Contrary to expectations, they found that the anxiety disorders had the lowest remission rates of all three classes of disorders, with SAD having the lowest rate of any disorder. In a subsequent prospective, longitu- dinal study, Shea et al. (2004) found high associations across time in the course of APD and SAD. Thus, it does not appear that APD can be distin- guished from generalized SAD on the basis of temporal stability.
  
284
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Shifting Problems to Axis I. Widiger (2003) also argued that subsuming APD within generalized SAD on Axis I would simply shift the various diag- nostic difficulties associated with APD to a different location within the DSM nomenclature rather than resolve them. In particular, the difficulties in defining the boundary between normal and abnormal functioning that char- acterize the personality disorders would not be resolved by moving APD to Axis I. It is of course true that combining generalized SAD and APD into a single diagnosis would not resolve boundary issues between normalcy and pathology, but it would resolve at least one diagnostic dilemma: the difficulty of distinguishing between two supposedly distinct disorders that appear to reflect the same underlying phenomenon.
The Risk of Deemphasizing Personality Research. Finally, personality disorder researchers worry that eliminating APD would signal a shift away from the study and clinical consideration of personality factors. This is a legitimate concern. The way in which a disorder is classified in the DSM system tends to impact how it is conceptualized and the nature of research efforts and clinical interventions directed toward it. For example, the clas- sification of posttraumatic stress disorder as an anxiety disorder may have restricted the consideration of symptoms not related to anxiety and processes related to the aftermath of trauma (Herbert & Sageman, 2004), leading some to suggest moving it to a new category of “stress related dis- orders” (Resick, 2004). Research on basic personality features in APD has been fruitful. For example, within the context of the five-factor model of personality (Costa & McCrae, 1990), a number of studies have found APD to be characterized by high levels of neuroticism and low levels of extraversion (see Alden et al., 2002, for a review). It is understandable that scholars would be concerned about a shift in emphasis away from the study of such personality factors. This is no reason, however, to maintain an artificial distinction between disorders. To the extent that personality research is relevant, it can make contributions to the understanding and treatment of this spectrum of psychopathology, whatever it is called (see Bienvenu & Stein, 2003, for a recent example of this kind of integration).
Given the dubious distinction between APD and generalized SAD, the dis- cussion of etiology, assessment, and treatment that follows will draw from the literatures on both disorders. I will note the diagnostic status of the spe- cific samples in the studies discussed.
Etiology ____________________________________________
Although the etiology of APD and SAD remains unknown, research has implicated a range of factors in the development and maintenance of social anxiety and avoidance (Dalrymple, Herbert, & Gaudiano, 2005). These include such biological factors as genetic and childhood temperament as well
  
Avoidant Personality Disorder 285

as more traditional “psychological” factors, such as childhood experiences, cognitive biases, and social skills deficits.
Genetic Factors
Several studies support the heritability of APD and generalized SAD. First- degree relatives of probands with APD demonstrate higher rates of both APD and generalized SAD (Johnson et al., 1995; Tillfors, Furmark, Ekselius, & Fredrikson, 2001). Likewise, first-degree relatives of those with generalized SAD show elevated rates of both generalized SAD and APD, but not non- generalized SAD (Stein et al., 1998). Of course, these studies confound shared genetics with shared environments, limiting their interpretability. In one of the few twin studies of SAD, Kendler, Karkowski, and Prescott (1999) esti- mated the heritability of SAD to be approximately 50%, although this study is limited by the use of DSM-III criteria, a female-only sample, and the failure to distinguish generalized from nongeneralized SAD. In a twin study of a gen- eral community sample, Stein, Jang, and Livesley (2002) found that genetic influences accounted for 42% of the variance in fear of negative evaluation, a core cognitive feature associated with both APD and generalized SAD. These same genetic factors were associated with personality characteristics linked to APD, including submissiveness, anxiousness, and social avoidance.
However, other studies suggest that what may be inherited is anxiety proneness or even a broader dimension of negative affectivity (which is a major component of anxiety and depression). For example, in a twin study of female adolescents, Nelson et al. (2000) found evidence for shared genetic vulnerability between SAD and both major depressive disorder and alcohol dependence. The literature suggests that the considerable genetic contribu- tions to both APD and generalized SAD are not unique to either disorder, reflecting instead a more general vulnerability to negative affectivity, although there may be a smaller genetic role in social anxiety per se (Rapee & Spence, 2004). Furthermore, Ollendick and Hirshfeld-Becker (2002) suggested that genetic vulnerabilities probably manifest themselves differently at different developmental periods, although the specific patterns are not yet known.
Temperament
Kagan and his colleagues (1984, 1988) identified a temperamental style in young children known as behavioral inhibition (BI), which they described as a tendency to react to unfamiliar persons or situations with fear, restraint, withdrawal, or all three. Theorists hypothesized that BI might be associated with the subsequent development of SAD. In fact, several researchers have found that children with high BI are at higher risk for the later development of elevated levels of social anxiety and SAD (e.g., Biederman et al., 2001;
  
286
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Mick & Telch, 1998). In addition, high BI children have been shown to have parents with elevated rates of SAD (Rosenbaum et al., 1991). There are three points worth noting about this literature. First, BI is at best only moderately stable, even within childhood (see Rapee & Spence, 2004, for a review). Second, many behaviorally inhibited children do not develop clinical levels of social anxiety or avoidance. Third, consistent with the findings on genetic factors discussed earlier, childhood BI appears to convey a risk for a range of adult psychopathology, including panic disorder, depression, violence, and alcohol abuse, rather than social anxiety or avoidance per se (Caspi, Moffitt, Newman, & Silva, 1996; Rosenbaum, et al., 2000).
Childhood Experiences
Theories of APD tend to emphasize pathological parenting experiences (Beck & Freeman, 1990; Millon, 1981; Pretzer & Beck, 1996). Likewise, anxiety theorists have highlighted parenting styles emphasizing excessive concern over social evaluation, overprotectiveness, and high degrees of crit- icism in the development of SAD (e.g., Chorpita & Barlow, 1998). In sup- port of these hypotheses, several studies have found that patients with APD or SAD retrospectively report characteristic parental experiences as children. Stravynski, Elie, and Franche (1989) found that patients with APD (per DSM-III criteria) perceived their parents as shaming, guilt engendering, and less tolerant than matched nonclinical controls perceived their parents to be. Meyer and Carver (2000) reported that APD symptoms in a sample of students were correlated with negative childhood narratives generated through a semistructured writing exercise. Among the most negative narratives, a common theme was parental arguments and conflicts. Arbel and Stravynski (1991), however, did not find that APD patients recalled more parental threats or separation relative to matched controls, although they perceived their parents as less affectionate.
Several retrospective studies have found that adults with SAD report their parents as having been less warm, more concerned with social evaluation, and having used shame as a discipline style (Arrindell et al., 1989; Bruch & Heimberg, 1994; Lieb et al., 2000; Parker, 1979; Rapee & Melville, 1997). Although this line of research is suggestive, an obvious weakness is the reliance on retrospective self-report. It is quite possible that observed differ- ences in reports of persons with APD or SAD relative to controls are a result of memory biases associated with current psychological state. This interpre- tation is bolstered by the research on cognitive biases in SAD discussed later. Another weakness is that most studies failed to include a clinical comparison group, so that the specificity of any findings to APD/SAD has not been estab- lished. Battle et al. (2004) found that not only patients with APD but also those with other personality disorders (e.g., borderline, schizotypal, and obsessive-compulsive) reported elevated rates of parental abuse and neglect.
  
Avoidant Personality Disorder 287

McGinn, Cukor, and Sanderson (in press) found that retrospective ratings of parents as abusive and neglectful were associated with depression but not anxiety. Finally, the confound of shared environments and genetics is an addi- tional limitation. It is possible that retrospective reports of things such as high concern over social evaluation, even if they are accurate, reflect shared genetic effects rather than any direct impact of parenting style. Just as estimates of heritability derived from family studies are limited by shared environmental effects, so too are studies of parenting effects limited by shared genetics.
A few researchers have attempted to address the limitations associated with retrospective self-report by directly examining parent-child interactions. In general, this research suggests that parents of children with anxiety disor- ders tend to be controlling and overinvolved (e.g., Hirshfeld, Biederman, Brody, Faraone, & Rosenbaum, 1997; Siqueland, Kendall, & Steinberg, 1996). Barrett, Rapee, Dadds, and Ryan (1996) also found that parents of children with anxiety disorders tended to encourage an avoidant coping style to a hypothetical threat situation. Hudson and Rapee (2001) found that mothers of anxious children demonstrated more intrusive involvement and more criticism of their children during a stressful task relative to nonclinical controls. However, the mothers of anxious children did not differ from the mothers of oppositional defiant children, who demonstrated similar behaviors. Moreover, only 21% of the anxious child group had avoidant disorder/SAD; the others included children with separation anxiety disorder, overanxious disorder/generalized anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. Although the observational research is suggestive of a link between overin- volved and critical parenting and SAD, the specificity of this link is dubious. Most studies have not included comparison groups, and those that have (e.g., Hudson & Rapee, 2001) do not find unique effects even for anxiety disorders, much less for APD/SAD. In addition, even if specific effects were observed, the causal direction would remain unclear. For example, overin- volved parenting might contribute to childhood anxiety and avoidance, but it could just as well reflect a reaction to an anxious and avoidant child, a phenomenon known as reactive gene-environment correlation (Plomin, 1994; Plomin, Owen, & McGuffin, 1994).
Cognitive Factors
Relatively little research has examined cognitive biases in APD, although a large literature exists with respect to SAD. This literature indicates that SAD is associated with several biases in information processing, including attentional, memory, and interpretation biases (see Clark, 2001, and Hirsch & Clark, 2004, for reviews). These biases are also evident in socially anx- ious youth (Ollendick & Hirshfeld-Becker, 2002). Persons with SAD tend to overestimate the likelihood of and cost associated with hypothetical neg- ative social outcomes (Foa, Franklin, Perry, & Herbert, 1996). They tend to
  
288
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

interpret ambiguous social situations negatively (Amir, Foa, & Coles, 1998; Stopa & Clark, 2000). They underestimate their performance in social inter- actions (Rapee & Lim, 1992; Stopa & Clark, 1993). Alden, Mellings, and Laposa (2004) found that relative to nonanxious controls, patients with SAD reacted to positively framed feedback following a conversational task with an increase in anticipatory anxiety related to a second conversation. The positive feedback may have increased the patients’ perceptions of per- formance expectations, which they did not feel confident in meeting.
One of the most consistent cognitive findings is that patients with SAD show enhanced self-focused attention and corresponding decreased process- ing of external cues in social situations (e.g., Hope, Heimberg, & Klein, 1990; Mellings & Alden, 2000). The research on memory biases has been less con- sistent. Several studies have found that persons with SAD demonstrate a neg- ative explicit memory bias, particularly related to their perceptions of how they are perceived by others, but only under conditions in which social threat is anticipated. Most studies, however, have failed to find evidence of biases in implicit memory (see Hirsch & Clark, 2004, for a review).
These information processing biases may explain one of the central para- doxes of APD and SAD: anxiety is not extinguished despite the fact that behav- ioral avoidance is rarely complete. That is, most individuals continue to engage to some degree in social interactions, yet habituation typically does not occur. Cognitive theorists suggest that such biases preclude the cognitive processing necessary for anxiety reduction. Similarly, experiential avoidance—the ten- dency to avoid aversive thoughts, feelings, images, memories, and so on— may contribute to the maintenance of the disorder despite the apparent absence of behavioral avoidance (Herbert & Cardaciotto, in press).
Social Skills Deficits
Given that persons with APD frequently avoid a range of social situations, it is not surprising that they fail to develop normal social skills. Turner and Beidel (1989) suggested that the presence of social skills deficits could dis- tinguish APD from generalized SAD. Herbert and his colleagues (1992), however, found no differences between patients with APD and SAD relative to those with only SAD on behavioral measures of social skills, results that add to the evidence that APD and generalized SAD reflect the same spectrum of psychopathology.
A number of researchers have found that the social performance of persons with generalized SAD is rated as poorer relative to the performance of both clin- ical and nonclinical controls, and many of these samples included persons who also met criteria for APD (Baker & Edelmann, 2002; Fydrich, Chambless, Perry, Buergener, & Beazley, 1998; Hopko, McNeil, Zvolensky, & Eifert, 2001; Norton & Hope, 2001; Stopa & Clark, 1993). Several authors have noted, however, that these performance impairments do not necessarily reflect social skills deficits but may instead result from the disruptive effects of anxiety
  
Avoidant Personality Disorder 289

or the use of safety behaviors (Clark & Wells, 1995; Heimberg & Becker, 2002; Herbert, 1995; Kashdan & Herbert, 2001). Just as there is considerable vari- ability across individuals in the degree of social impairments, it is likely that the nature of the impairments also differs, with a subset reflecting true skills deficits.
In summary, although both biological and psychosocial factors have been found to be associated with APD and SAD, these associations are generally not unique to this spectrum of psychopathology. Further research is needed to clarify the specific factors that lead to social anxiety and avoidance.
__________________________________________ Assessment
Before formal assessment of APD, SAD, or both can begin, the individual must first come to the attention of a mental health professional. There is evi- dence that most persons suffering from these disorders are not recognized by professionals and do not seek professional help (Herbert, Crittenden, & Dalrymple, 2004). This is probably due to two reasons. First, because APD and SAD represent extremes of normal functioning, patients, their families, and even health care professionals often fail to recognize that the problem merits professional attention. Second, the very nature of the problem fre- quently contributes to reluctance to seek help. Once it is identified, however, both clinical interviews and self-report measures are useful assessment tools. Although such projective tests as the Rorschach inkblot test have tradition- ally been used in the assessment of personality pathology, their scientific status is highly dubious. Their use is therefore not recommended (Wood, Nezworski, Lilienfeld, & Garb, 2003).
The Clinical Interview
The most commonly used assessment tool for APD or SAD is the clinical interview, which varies widely in format, ranging from highly structured protocols to unstructured interviews. Structured clinical interviews are most commonly employed in research settings, as their standardization reduces interviewer bias and increases interrater reliability, whereas unstructured formats tend to be preferred in front-line clinical settings. Regardless of for- mat, the typical goals of the clinical interview for APD include establishment of rapport, psychodiagnosis, and assessment of symptom patterns, phobic stimuli, and impairment in functioning.
The most commonly used structured interview for personality disorders (including APD) is the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II; First, Gibbon, Spitzer, Williams, & Benjamin, 1997). The SCID-II is known for being relatively user-friendly, and has been shown to have adequate psychometric properties (Maffei et al., 1997). Another structured interview commonly used to assess personality disorders is the Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality (SIDP-IV; Pfohl, Blum, &
   
290
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Zimmerman, 1997; Stangl, Pfohl, Zimmerman, Bowers, & Corenthal, 1985). The SIDP-IV is a semistructured interview designed as a “nonpejorative” measure of personality disorder symptoms. Interrater reliability has been shown to be quite high, with kappa of up to .94 (Bockian, Lee, & Fidanque, 2003). The International Personality Disorder Examination (IPDE; Loranger, Susman, Oldham, & Russakoff, 1987) is a popular instrument in research contexts, although it is rather long and cumbersome to administer, making it less popular in clinical settings. The IPDE has been shown to have good interrater reliability, with intraclass correlations ranging from .84 to .92 (Lenzenweger, 1999), as well as adequate temporal stability (Loranger et al., 1994). Another popular instrument, the Personality Disorder Interview—IV (Widiger, Mangine, Corbitt, Ellis, & Thomas, 1995), has not been subjected to psy- chometric evaluation, and the manual includes no normative data. Trull (1995) developed the Iowa Personality Disorder Screen (IPDS), a brief, 11- item screening interview; initial psychometric data are promising (Trull & Amdur, 2001). Considering the overlap between the criteria used to define APD and SAD, it is worth noting that the single item tapping symptoms of APD on the IPDS is “excessive social anxiety.”
Self-Report Instruments
In addition to measures of personality traits, such as the revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) or NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI; Costa & McCrae, 1992), several self-report measures of personality disor- ders have been developed. These vary in the degree to which the items adhere closely to the DSM-IV criteria for the various personality disorders. Among the more commonly used measures are the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory—III (MCMI-III; Millon, Millon, & Davis, 1994), a 175-item questionnaire measuring both Axis I and Axis II conditions corresponding to the DSM-IV, and the Wisconsin Personality Disorders Inventory—IV (Klein & Benjamin, 1996), a 204-item self-report inventory based on an interper- sonal model of personality functioning.
A serious problem with the assessment of personality disorders in general is the relatively low level of agreement among various measures putatively tapping the same constructs (Perry, 1992). Even among structured interviews such as the SCID-II and the IPDE, kappas for various personality disorders tend to be under .50 (Clark, Livesley, & Morey, 1997). Agreement is even lower between interview measures and self-report measures. For example, median correlations range from .26 to .38 between the SCID-II and MCMI- II (Kennedy et al., 1995; Marlowe et al., 1997). These relatively low correla- tions reflect some personality disorder scholars’ concerns that patients may not be able or willing to accurately describe personality disorder features, ren- dering self-rating instruments of limited value for diagnostic purposes (Westen, 1997). Of course, low correlations alone do not necessarily indicate
  
Avoidant Personality Disorder 291

that any particular procedure is either inaccurate or not useful. For example, self-report instruments may tend to assess different aspects of personality dis- orders (e.g., self-perceptions, mood states) from those that interviews assess. In addition, interviews permit probing for multiple behavioral examples of a symptom or trait, which may lead to more informed global conclusions than those obtained from self-report measures.
Two approaches have been employed to increase diagnostic accuracy. The LEAD procedure (longitudinal expert evaluation using all available data; Spitzer, 1983) consists of a consensus diagnosis by a team of clinicians using data collected over the course of assessment and treatment. However, the concordance between LEAD diagnoses and those generated by struc- tured interviews for the personality disorders has been disappointingly low. A second approach involves a Q-sort procedure developed by Westen and Shedler (2000). The Shedler-Westen Assessment Procedure–200 (SWAP- 200) involves the clinician’s sorting of 200 descriptive cards into one of seven categories according to how well each describes the patient, based on data gathered during a semistructured interview. Westen and Muderrisoglu (2003) found good interrater reliability between the SWAP-200 and semistructured clinical interview (median r > .80). However, Davidson, Obonsawin, Seils, and Patience (2003) found poor agreement between clin- icians and their patients on the SWAP-200, raising questions about the instrument’s validity. It is worth noting, however, that the highest agreement (although still only modest) was for the APD prototype.
In general, the psychometric data on instruments designed to assess Axis I disorders are more encouraging. It should be noted, however, that few studies have directly compared instruments targeting APD with those tar- geting SAD, and those that have done so did not distinguish the specific and generalized subtypes of SAD (e.g., Shea & Yen, 2003). Given the high diag- nostic overlap between APD and generalized SAD, one would expect instru- ments assessing each construct to overlap significantly. Examination of the specific items within measures of APD and SAD supports this conclusion. Nevertheless, most measures of APD are subscales of larger instruments that cover the full range of personality disorders rather than focus exclusively on APD per se. In contrast, there exist several measures of SAD in and of itself, and even general measures of anxiety disorders tend to devote more items to assessing symptoms of SAD than corresponding measures of personality disorders devote to APD.
The most commonly employed structured interviews for SAD are the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV (ADIS-IV; Brown, DiNardo, & Barlow, 1994) and the Structured Clinical Interview for Axis I DSM-IV Disorders (SCID-IV; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1994). Commonly employed self-report measures include the Social Phobia and Anxiety Inventory (Turner, Beidel, Dancu, & Stanley, 1989) and the Social Phobia Scale (Mattick & Clark, 1998; see Herbert, Rheingold, & Brandsma, 2000, for a review of the assessment of SAD).
  
292 PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Treatment _________________________________________
Relatively few studies have examined the treatment of individuals specifi- cally selected for APD (Alden et al., 2002). However, a substantial literature exists on the treatment of generalized SAD, and many of the samples include individuals who also met diagnostic criteria for APD. As noted earlier, studies that have compared treatment outcome among patients with gener- alized SAD with versus without APD have generally found that the former group begins and ends treatment more impaired than the latter group does, but the groups do not differ in their rate of improvement (Brown, Heimberg, & Juster, 1995; Hoffman, Newman, Becker, Taylor, & Roth, 1995; Hope et al., 1995). We will therefore supplement our review of the treatment of APD per se with a review of the treatment of generalized SAD.
Pharmacotherapy
A variety of psychopharmacological agents have been used for both APD and SAD, including the beta adrenergic antagonists, various benzodiazepine anxiolytics, the monoamine oxidase inhibitors (MAOIs), and various selec- tive serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs; Trestman, Woo-Ming, deVegvar, & Siever, 2001). For nongeneralized (i.e., “specific”) SAD, beta blockers or benzodiazepines are generally considered the first-line drug therapies, and they can be used on an as-needed basis. For patients with APD or general- ized SAD, SSRIs are recommended, with MAOIs as alternatives for those who do not respond to SSRIs (Blanco, Schneier, & Liebowitz, 2001). For example, in a double-blind, placebo-controlled trial, Fahlén (1995) found that the reversible and selective MAOI-A brofaromine reduced not only symptoms of social anxiety but also avoidant personality traits in a sample of patients with SAD. In fact, the percentage of patients who met diagnostic criteria for APD dropped from 60% (n = 15) at pretreatment to 20% (n = 5) following 12 weeks of drug therapy. Although pharmacotherapy is generally effective during acute treatment, a serious limitation is the high rate of relapse following medication discontinuation. As discussed later, attempts to address relapse have focused on combining drug therapy and psychotherapy.
Psychotherapy
Although several authors have provided clinical descriptions of the tradi- tional psychodynamic treatment of APD (e.g., Harper, 2004), such interven- tions have only rarely been subjected to systematic research, and they have been only for so-called time-limited versions of psychodynamic treatments. Barber, Morse, Krakauer, Chittams, and Crits-Christoph (1997) found sig- nificant reductions in avoidant symptoms in patients with APD following
  
Avoidant Personality Disorder 293

52 sessions of time-limited supportive-expressive psychotherapy. Conclusions that can be drawn from this study are limited, however, by the absence of any control or comparison condition. Svartberg, Stiles, and Seltzer (2004) found comparable and positive results for 40-session programs of short-term dynamic psychotherapy and of cognitive therapy for patients with Cluster C personality disorders, 62% of whom had APD. Although the magnitude of these results suggested clinically meaningful effects, the study relied solely on self-rating instruments to assess treatment outcome. Moreover, measures commonly used in cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) trials were not used, precluding comparison of effect sizes with those from other studies.
In contrast to the paucity of research on psychodynamic psychotherapy, several studies have evaluated various forms of CBT for APD. In keeping with the common belief that APD is associated with social skills deficits, most of these studies included social skills training as a core treatment com- ponent. Alden (1989) found positive effects of systematic, graduated expo- sure for patients with APD; the addition of social skills training did not augment the effects of exposure alone. Stravynski, Lesage, Marcouiller, and Elie (1989) found social skills training (SST) plus group discussion, admin- istered sequentially, to be effective for APD. In a small, uncontrolled trial for APD, Renneberg, Goldstein, Phillips, and Chambless (1990) found positive effects of a brief but intensive group treatment program consisting of sys- tematic desensitization and SST.
The most extensively researched psychotherapy for SAD (with or without APD) is the cognitive-behavioral group therapy (CBGT) program developed by Heimberg and Becker (2002). CBGT is a time-limited, group-based pro- gram consisting of cognitive restructuring conducted in the context of simu- lated exposure exercises. The program is based on cognitive models of SAD and aims to modify dysfunctional cognitive processes believed to maintain social anxiety and avoidance. A number of studies from various research groups have documented the considerable effectiveness of CBGT (Chambless et al., 1997; Heimberg et al., 1998; Hope et al., 1995; Woody & Adessky, 2002). CBGT has been shown to be more effective than psychoeducation (Heimberg et al., 1990). Several studies have recently found that individual versions of the CBGT protocol appear to be at least as effective as the stan- dard group format while offering the advantage of greater convenience in scheduling of treatment sessions (Herbert, Rheingold, Gaudiano, & Myers, 2004; Zaider et al., 2003).
A similar CBT program was recently developed by Clark and his col- leagues (Clark et al., 2003; Stangier, Heidenreich, Peitz, Lauterbach, & Clark, 2003). Like Heimberg and Becker’s CBGT, the Clark program focuses on cognitive change as the key target of intervention. Clark places relatively less emphasis on disputational methods of cognitive restructuring, however, utilizing instead video feedback of simulated social encounters to provide evi- dence against dysfunctional beliefs. Emphasis is also placed on fostering an external focus to disrupt the self-focused attention that characterizes SAD.
  
294
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Neither the Heimberg and Becker nor the Clark protocol explicitly addresses deficits in social skills. As discussed earlier, there appears to be considerable heterogeneity among individuals with APD and generalized SAD with respect to social skills, suggesting that some patients may benefit from interventions that incorporate explicit SST. Several studies have found SST to be effective for SAD (e.g., Mersch, Emmelkamp, Bogels, & van der Sleen, 1989; Wlazlo, Schroeder-Hartwig, Hand, Kaiser, & Münchau, 1990). Turner and his colleagues (1994; Turner, Beidel, & Cooley-Quille, 1995) integrated SST with exposure, achieving promising results. Herbert and his colleagues (Herbert, Gaudiano, et al., 2005) recently found that a CBGT program that included social skills training was more effective for a sample of patients with generalized SAD, 75% of whom met criteria for APD, rela- tive to standard CBGT without social skills training.
Combined Treatment
Researchers have recently investigated the possibility that a combination of pharmacotherapy and psychotherapy may be more beneficial than either monotherapy alone. Kool, Dekker, Duijsens, de Jonghe, and Puite (2003) found greater reduction in APD symptoms in depressed patients treated with a combination of antidepressants and short-term psychodynamic supportive psychotherapy relative to those treated with antidepressants only. The rele- vance of these findings to APD per se is difficult to gauge, however, as patients were selected for the presence of major depressive disorder, and it is not clear how many met diagnostic criteria for APD. Two large-scale studies, both of which are not yet published, have compared CBT combined with antidepressants in the treatment of SAD. Davidson et al. (2004) found no benefit of combined SSRI (fluoxetine) and CBT relative to either monother- apy alone; Heimberg and colleagues (Heimberg, 2002) found similar results with combined CBT and an MAOI (phenelzine). Haug et al. (2003) found that patients who received combined exposure therapy plus an SSRI (sertra- line) actually fared significantly worse over a 28-week follow-up period than those who received exposure alone. The reasons for this surprising effect are unclear but could be related to patients taking medications making external attributions for their improvements, or to medications interfering with expo- sure-mediated habituation.
New Directions in Treatment _________________________
Despite the promising results of current intervention technologies, there remains significant room for improvement. Treatment does not produce improvement in all patients, and even among treatment responders, most remain at least somewhat symptomatic (see Herbert, Gaudiano, et al., 2005, for a discussion). These findings suggest the need for further treatment innovations. One obvious
  
Avoidant Personality Disorder 295

possibility is combined psychotherapy plus medication. As just discussed, how- ever, the results of trials of combination interventions have proved disappointing. An open trial of interpersonal psychotherapy recently yielded promising results and warrants further study (Lipsitz, Markowitz, Cherry, & Fyer, 1999). Herbert and Dalrymple (2004) adapted a novel form of CBT known as acceptance and commitment therapy (Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999), which encourages overcoming avoidance of distressing thoughts and feelings while promoting behavior change, for generalized SAD. Preliminary results
from an open trial are promising (Dalrymple & Herbert, 2004).
Recent efforts have begun to focus on the dissemination of treatments to front-line clinical settings. For example, the recent shift in emphasis away from group-based CBT and toward individual therapy described earlier speaks to the need for treatment programs that are easily transportable to clinical settings. In terms of efficiency, Herbert, Rheingold, and Goldstein
(2002) found surprising efficacy of a brief, 6-week version of CBT.
_____________________ Conclusion and Future Directions
Since its initial appearance in the DSM-III more than two decades ago, we have learned a great deal about the nature, etiology, assessment, and treat- ment of APD. Nevertheless, many questions remain, and current interven- tions leave considerable room for improvement.
Perhaps the most pressing issue is resolution of the diagnostic dilemma regarding the generalized subtype of SAD. There is compelling evidence that APD and generalized SAD are drawn from the same fundamental spectrum of psychopathology. The coexistence of the two diagnostic labels for the same underlying phenomenon contributes to diagnostic confusion among clinicians and hampers communication among researchers (Farmer, 2000). It therefore makes little sense to continue the status quo in future generations of the DSM. This conclusion of course raises the question of which label should prevail, a question on which personality scholars and anxiety theo- rists predictably differ. Generalized SAD and APD appear to be virtually identical. However, both conditions differ on many dimensions, including heritability and treatment response, from such “nongeneralized” SAD as public speaking phobia, which has alternatively been labeled “discrete,” “specific,” or “circumscribed” SAD. One solution would be to distinguish formally specific SAD from APD/generalized SAD. Consistent with the orig- inal DSM-III conceptualization, the former could be termed “social phobia,” which would be limited to anxiety and avoidance of a single social situation. APD and generalized SAD could then become “social anxiety and avoidance disorder,” a term that captures both the subjective distress provoked by social situations and the resulting tendency to engage in experiential and behavioral avoidance. Whether this or another diagnostic scheme is imple- mented, the issue should be resolved with the publication of the DSM-V. It is worth noting that this issue bears implications for the broader relationship
 
296
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

between other spectrums of psychopathology that appear to cut across current Axis I and Axis II disorders (e.g., schizophrenia and schizotypal personality disorder; obsessive-compulsive disorder and obsessive-compul- sive personality disorder).
Personality scholars understandably worry that eliminating APD as a dis- tinct diagnosis could result in a loss of focus on personality factors in social anxiety and avoidance. However, this need not be the case. To the extent that personality factors are relevant, they can make contributions to psy- chopathology regardless of where it is located and named in a nosological scheme. For example, Hoffman et al. (2004) proposed an interesting dimen- sional approach to the classification of disorders of social anxiety and avoid- ance that is based in part on the work of personality theorists. Such work highlights the importance of investigating basic processes that may underlie various forms of psychopathology that cut across diagnostic categories and that are linked to normal psychological functioning (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004).
Finally, although impressive advances have been made in the treatment of APD and SAD over the past two decades, much more remains to be done. Some of the especially promising possibilities include the incorporation of technologies such as video feedback, treatments focused on interpersonal relationships, and interventions targeting experiential avoidance of distress- ing cognitions and affect as alternatives to the disputational strategies associated with traditional CBT. One thing is clear: the next two decades promise to be every bit as exciting as the preceding two in the quest to under- stand, assess, and treat problems of social anxiety and avoidance.
Notes _____________________________________________
1. In keeping with the recommendations of Lilienfeld, Waldman, and Israel (1994), I use such terms as “overlap” and “diagnostic co-occurrence” rather than the more familiar “comorbidity” to describe the relationship of APD with other disor- ders. These terms avoid unnecessary connotations about the underlying structure of the diagnostic categories.
2. Although “social anxiety disorder” and “social phobia” are often used inter- changeably, there is a growing preference for the former term, which many believe better reflects the pervasiveness of social anxiety and avoidance among most indi- viduals with the condition (Liebowitz, Heimberg, Fresco, Travers, & Stein, 2000).
References _________________________________________
Alden, L. (1989). Short-term structured treatment for avoidant personality disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 756–764.
Alden, L. E., Laposa, J. M., Taylor, C. T., & Ryder, A. G. (2002). Avoidant per- sonality disorder: Current status and future directions. Journal of Personality Disorders, 16, 1–29.
   
Avoidant Personality Disorder 297

Alden, L. E., Mellings, T. M. B., & Laposa, J. M. (2004). Framing social information and generalized social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 588–600.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of men- tal disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Amir, N., Foa, E. B., & Coles, M. E. (1998). Negative interpretation bias in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 945–957.
Arbel, N., & Stravynski, A. (1991). A retrospective study of separation in the devel- opment of adult avoidant personality disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 83, 174–178.
Arntz, A. (1999). Do personality disorders exist? On the validity of the concept and its cognitive-behavioral formulation and treatment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, S97–S134.
Arrindell, W. A., Kwee, M. G. T., Methorst, G. J., van der Ende, J., Pol, E., & Moritz, B. J. M. (1989). Perceived parental rearing styles of agoraphobic and socially phobic in-patients. British Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 526–535.
Baker, S. R., & Edelmann, R. J. (2002). Is social phobia related to lack of social skills? Duration of skill-related behaviours and ratings of behavioural adequacy. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41, 243–257.
Barber, J. P., Morse, J. Q., Krakauer, I. D., Chittams, J., & Crits-Christoph, K. (1997). Change in obsessive-compulsive and avoidant personality disorders following time-limited supportive-expressive therapy. Psychotherapy, 34, 133–143.
Barrett, P. M., Rapee, R. M., Dadds, M. R., & Ryan, S. (1996). Family enhancement of cognitive style in anxious and aggressive children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24, 187–203.
Battle, C. L., Shea, M. T., Johnson, D. M., Yen, S., Zlotnick, C., Zanarini, M. C., et al. (2004). Childhood maltreatment associated with adult personality disor- ders: Findings from the collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study. Journal of Personality Disorders, 18, 193–211.
Beck, A. T., & Freeman, A. (1990). Cognitive therapy of personality disorders. New York: Guilford Press.
Biederman, J., Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R., Rosenbaum, J. F., Herot, C., Friedman, D., Snidman, N., et al. (2001). Further evidence of association between behavioral inhibition and social anxiety in children. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 1673–1679.
Bienvenu, O. J., & Stein, M. B. (2003). Personality and anxiety disorders: A review. Journal of Personality Disorders, 17, 139–151.
Blanco, C., Schneier, F. R., & Liebowitz, M. R. (2001). Psychopharmacology. In S. G. Hofmann & P. M. DiBartolo (Eds.), From social anxiety to social phobia: Multiple perspectives (pp. 335–353). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Bockian, N. R., Lee, A., & Fidanque, C. S. (2003). Personality disorders and spinal cord injury: A pilot study. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 10, 307–313.
Brown, E. J., Heimberg, R. G., & Juster, H. R. (1995). Social phobia subtype and avoidant personality disorder: Effect of severity of social phobia, impairment, and outcome of cognitive-behavioral treatment. Behavior Therapy, 26, 467–486.
Brown, T. A., DiNardo, P. A., & Barlow, D. H. (1994). Anxiety disorders interview schedule for DSM-IV (ADIS-IV). Albany, NY: Graywind.
Bruch, M. A., & Heimberg, R. G. (1994). Differences in perceptions of parental and personal characteristics between generalized and nongeneralized social phobics. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 8, 155–168.
298
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Newman, D. L., & Silva, P. A. (1996). Behavioral obser- vations at age 3 years predict adult psychiatric disorders: Longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 1033–1039.
Chambless, D. L., Tran, G. Q., & Glass, C. R. (1997). Predictors of response to cog- nitive-behavioral group therapy for social phobia. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 11, 211–240.
Chorpita, B. F., & Barlow, D. H. (1998). The development of anxiety: The role of control in the early environment. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 3–21.
Clark, D. M. (2001). A cognitive perspective on social phobia. In W. R. Crozier & L. E. Alden (Eds.), International handbook of social anxiety: Concepts, research and interventions relating to the self and shyness (pp. 405–430). New York: John Wiley.
Clark, D. M., Ehlers, A., McManus, F., Hackmann, A., Fennell, M., Campbell, H., et al. (2003). Cognitive therapy vs. fluoxetine in generalized social phobia: A randomized placebo controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 1058–1067.
Clark, D. M., & Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. In R. G. Heimberg, M. R. Liebowitz, D. A. Hope, & F. R. Schneier (Eds.), Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment and treatment (pp. 69–93). New York: Guilford Press.
Clark, L. A., Livesley, W. J., & Morey, L. (1997). Personality disorder assessment: The challenge of construct validity. Journal of Personality Disorders, 11, 205–231.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1990). Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality. Journal of Personality Disorders, 4, 362–371.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Craske, M. G. (1999). Anxiety disorders: Psychological approaches to theory and treatment. Boulder, CO: Westview.
Dalrymple, K. L., & Herbert, J. D. (2004, November). Acceptance and commitment therapy for generalized social anxiety disorder: Initial findings from a pilot study. Paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy, New Orleans, LA.
Dalrymple, K. L., Herbert, J. D., & Gaudiano, B. A. (2005). Onset of illness and developmental factors in social anxiety disorder: Findings from a retrospective interview. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Davidson, J. R. T., Foa, E. B., Huppert, J. D., Keefe, F. J., Franklin, M. E., Compton, J. S., et al. (2004). Fluoxetine, comprehensive cognitive behavioral therapy, and placebo in generalized social phobia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 1005–1013.
Davidson, K. M., Obonsawin, M. C., Seils, M., & Patience, L. (2003). Patient and clinician agreement on personality using the SWAP-200. Journal of Personality Disorders, 17, 208–218.
Ekselius, L., Tillfors, M., Furmark, T., & Fredrikson, M. (2001). Personality disorders in the general population: DSM-IV and ICD-10 defined prevalence as related to sociodemographic profile. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 311–320.
Fahlén, T. (1995). Personality traits in social phobia: II. Changes during drug treat- ment. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 56, 569–573.
Farmer, R. F. (2000). Issues in the assessment and conceptualization of personality disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 823–851.
Avoidant Personality Disorder 299

Feske, U., Perry, K. J., Chambless, D. L., Renneberg, B., & Goldstein, A. J. (1996). Avoidant personality disorder as a predictor for treatment outcome among gen- eralized social phobics. Journal of Personality Disorders, 10, 174–184.
First, M. B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., & Benjamin, L. (1997).
User’s guide for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality
Disorders (SCID-II). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1994). Structured clin- ical interview for Axis I DSM-IV disorders: Patient edition. New York:
Biometrics Research Department.
Foa, E., Franklin, M., Perry, K., & Herbert, J. D. (1996). Cognitive biases in gener-
alized social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 433–439.
Frances, A. J., First, M. B., & Pincus, H. A. (1995). DSM-IV guidebook.
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Fydrich, T., Chambless, D. L., Perry, K. J., Buergener, F., & Beazley, M. B. (1998).
Behavioral assessment of social performance: A rating system for social phobia.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 995–1010.
Harper, R. G. (2004). Personality-guided therapy in behavioral medicine.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Harvey, A. G., Watkins, E., Mansell, W., & Shafran, R. (2004). Cognitive behav-
ioural processes across psychological disorders: A transdiagnostic approach to
research and treatment. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Haug, T. T., Blomhoff, S., Hellstrøm, K., Holme, I., Humble, M., Madsbu, H. P., et al. (2003). Exposure therapy and sertraline in social phobia: 1-year follow-up
of a randomized controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry, 182, 312–318. Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and commitment therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. New York: Guilford Press. Heimberg, R. G. (1996). Social phobia, avoidant personality disorder and the multi- axial conceptualization of interpersonal anxiety. In P. M. Salkovskis (Ed.),
Trends in cognitive and behavioural therapies (pp. 43–61). New York: Wiley. Heimberg, R. G. (2002, November). The understanding and treatment of social anx- iety: What a long strange trip it’s been (and will be). Paper presented at the
Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Reno, NV.
Heimberg, R. G., & Becker, R. E. (2002). Cognitive-behavioral group therapy for social phobia: Basic mechanisms and clinical strategies. New York: Guilford Press. Heimberg, R. G., Dodge, C. S., Hope, D. A., Kennedy, C. R., Zollo, L. J., & Becker, R. E. (1990). Cognitive behavioral group treatment for social phobia: Comparison
with a credible placebo control. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 1–23. Heimberg, R. G., Liebowitz, M. R., Hope, D. A., Schneier, F. R., Hold, C. S., Welkowitz, L., et al. (1998). Cognitive-behavioral group therapy versus phenelzine in social phobia: 12-week outcome. Archives of General Psychiatry,
55, 1133–1141.
Herbert, J. D. (1995). An overview of the current status of social phobia. Applied and
Preventive Psychology, 4, 39–51.
Herbert, J. D., & Cardaciotto, L. (in press). Social anxiety disorder. In S. Orsillo &
L. Roemer (Eds.), Acceptance and mindfulness-based approaches to anxiety:
Conceptualization and treatment. New York: Kluwer Academic Publishers. Herbert, J. D., Crittenden, K., & Dalrymple, K. L. (2004). Knowledge of social anxiety disorder relative to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder among
300
PERSONALITY DISORDERS

educational professionals. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology,
33, 366–372.
Herbert, J. D., & Dalrymple, K. L. (2004). Acceptance and commitment therapy for
social anxiety disorder: A treatment manual. Unpublished manuscript, Drexel
University, Philadelphia, PA.
Herbert, J. D., & Dalrymple, K. (2005). Social anxiety disorder. In A. Freeman,
S. H. Felgoise, A. M. Nezu, C. M. Nezu, & M. A. Reinecke (Eds.), Encyclopedia
of cognitive behavior therapy (pp. 368–372). New York: Springer.
Herbert, J. D., Gaudiano, B. A., Rheingold, A., Harwell, V., Dalrymple, K., & Nolan, E. M. (2005). Social skills training augments the effectiveness of cognitive behav-
ior group therapy for social anxiety disorder. Behavior Therapy, 36, 125–138. Herbert, J. D., Hope, D., & Bellack, A. (1992). Validity of the distinction between gen- eralized social phobia and avoidant personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 101, 332–339.
Herbert, J. D., Rheingold, A., & Brandsma, L. (2000). Assessment of social anxiety.
In S. G. Hofmann & P. M. DiBartolo (Eds.), From social anxiety to social pho-
bia: Multiple perspectives (pp. 20–45). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Herbert, J. D., Rheingold, A., Gaudiano, B. A., & Myers, V. H. (2004). Standard versus extended cognitive behavior therapy for social anxiety disorder: A ran-
domized-controlled trial. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 32, 1–17. Herbert, J. D., Rheingold, A. A., & Goldstein, S. G. (2002). Brief cognitive behav- ioral group therapy for social anxiety disorder. Cognitive and Behavioral
Practice, 9, 1–8.
Herbert, J. D., & Sageman, M. (2004). First do no harm: Emerging guidelines for the
treatment of posttraumatic reactions. In G. Rosen (Ed.), Posttraumatic stress
disorder: Issues and controversies. West Sussex, UK: Wiley.
Hirsch, C. R., & Clark, D. M. (2004). Information-processing bias in social phobia.
Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 799–825.
Hirshfeld, D. R., Biederman, J., Brody, L., Faraone, S. V., & Rosenbaum, J. F.
(1997). Associations between expressed emotion and child behavioral inhibition and psychopathology: A pilot study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 205–213.
Hoffman, S. G., Heinrichs, N., & Moscovitch, D. A. (2004). The nature and expres- sion of social phobia: Toward a new classification. Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 769–797.
Hoffman, S. G., Newman, M. G., Becker, E., Taylor, C. B., & Roth, W. T. (1995). Social phobia with and without avoidant personality disorder: Preliminary behavior therapy outcome findings. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 9, 427–438.
Holt, C. S., Heimberg, R. G., & Hope, D. A. (1992). Avoidant personality disorder and the generalized subtype of social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 318–325.
Hope, D. A., Heimberg, R. G., & Klein, J. F. (1990). Social anxiety and the recall of interpersonal information. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 4, 185–195.
Hope, D. A., Herbert, J. D., & White, C. (1995). Diagnostic subtype, avoidant per- sonality disorder, and efficacy of cognitive-behavioral group therapy for social phobia. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19, 339–417.
Hopko, D. R., McNeil, D. W., Zvolensky, M. J., & Eifert, G. H. (2001). The rela- tion between anxiety and skill in performance-based anxiety disorders: A behav- ioral formulation of social phobia. Behavior Therapy, 32, 185–207.
Avoidant Personality Disorder 301

Hudson, J. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2001). Parent-child interactions and anxiety disor- ders: An observational study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 1411–1427. Jansen, M. A., Arntz, A., Merckelbach, H., & Mersch, P. P. A. (1994). Personality disorders and features in social phobia and panic disorder. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 103, 391–395.
Johnson, B. A., Brent, D. A., Connolly, J., Bridge, J., Matta, J., Constantine, D.,
et al. (1995). Familial aggregation of adolescent personality disorders. Journal
of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 798–804. Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., Clarke, C., Snidman, N., & Garcia-Coll, C. (1984).
Behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar. Child Development, 55, 2212–2225. Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Snidman, N. (1988). Biological bases of childhood shy-
ness. Science, 240, 167–171.
Kashdan, T. B., & Herbert, J. D. (2001). Social anxiety disorder in childhood and
adolescence: Current status and future directions. Clinical Child and Family
Psychology Review, 4, 37–61.
Kazdin, A. E. (1983). Psychiatric diagnosis, dimensions of dysfunction, and child
behavior therapy. Behavior Therapy, 14, 73–99.
Kendler, K. S., Karkowski, L. M., & Prescott, C. A. (1999). Fears and phobias:
Reliability and heritability. Psychological Medicine, 29, 539–553.
Kennedy, S. H., Katz, R., Rockert, W., Mendolwitz, S., Raleveski, E., & Clewes, J. (1995). Assessment of personality disorders in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: A comparison of self-report and structured interview methods. Journal
of Nervous and Mental Disease, 183, 358–364.
Klein, M. H., & Benjamin, L. S. (1996). The Wisconsin personality disorders
inventory—IV. Madison: University of Wisconsin.
Kool, S., Dekker, J., Duijsens, I. J., de Jonghe, F., & Puite, B. (2003). Changes in per-
sonality pathology after pharmacotherapy and combined therapy for depressed
patients. Journal of Personality Disorders, 17, 60–72.
Lenzenweger, M. F. (1999). Stability and change in personality disorder features: The
longitudinal study of personality disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56,
1009–1015.
Lieb, R., Wittchen, H. U., Hoefler, M., Fuetsch, M., Stein, M. B., & Merikangas,
K. R. (2000). Parental psychopathology, parenting styles, and the risk of social phobia in offspring: A prospective-longitudinal community study